Skip to main content

Application of the extended parallel process model and risk perception attitude framework to obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behaviors among Korean adults

Abstract

Background

Perceiving oneself as obese has been associated with weight loss attempts. However, such a perception may not sufficiently drive significant weight reduction in many individuals. Hence, relying solely on the traditionally emphasized perceived risk of behavioral changes in obesity is challenging. This study used an extended parallel process model and a risk perception attitude framework to explore the influence of perceived risk and perceived efficacy on individual obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behaviors.

Methods

Data were obtained from 1,100 Korean adults aged 40–69 years through an online survey conducted in October 2022. Multinomial logistic regression and analysis of variance were employed to assess the relationships among perceived risk, perceived efficacy, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors.

Results

Sex was associated with being underweight, overweight, and obese. Moreover, perceived severity was associated with obesity, whereas perceived susceptibility was associated with overweight and obese. Response efficacy was related to being overweight alone, whereas self-efficacy was associated with being underweight, overweight, and obese. The main effects of sex and perceived risk, and their interaction effect were statistically significant for obesity knowledge. Additionally, the main effects of sex, perceived risk, and perceived efficacy on obesity prevention behaviors were statistically significant.

Conclusions

The extended parallel process model and risk perception attitude framework proved effective in classifying obesity based on body mass index, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors.

Peer Review reports

Background

By 2035, the number of individuals with overweight and obesity worldwide could surpass 4 billion, ultimately exceeding the 2.6 billion record set in 2020 [1]. This development signifies a notable increase from 38% of the global population by 2020 to over 50% by 2035. Moreover, the prevalence of obesity alone is expected to increase from 14 to 24% within the same timeframe, affecting nearly 2 billion adults, children, and adolescents by 2035 [1]. Obesity is associated with a multitude of morbidities, including heart disease, diabetes, hypertension, and certain cancers [2, 3], and the World Health Organization (WHO) has defined obesity as a disease (International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision [ICD-10]: E66.0–E66.9).

The intricate nature of motivating health behavioral change is apparent through the multitude of conceptual frameworks that have been postulated [4, 5]. Among these, the health belief model (HBM) [6], protection motivation theory (PMT) [7], and parallel process model (PPM) [8] underscore the significance of risk perception as the driving force behind the adoption of behaviors aimed at mitigating risks.

However, a considerable number of individuals do not perceive obesity as a serious concern [9, 10] and remain unaware of its associated health risks [11]. Moreover, although perceiving oneself as overweight has been linked to weight loss attempts, this perception appears to be insufficient for driving weight reduction among many individuals [12, 13]. Therefore, regarding obesity, expecting behavioral changes based solely on the traditionally emphasized perceived risk is challenging. Furthermore, recent studies have suggested that risk perception alone cannot fully explain all the factors that motivate individuals to take preventive actions. Yoon and Seo [14] proposed that individuals’ perceptions of risk do not always align with the actual level of risk and have a greater influence on the decision-making process than the objective risk itself. Consequently, individuals may opt for different behaviors even when facing equal health risks. Understanding the underlying factors that shape behavioral changes is crucial for explaining such behavioral variations. Accordingly, the extended parallel process model (EPPM) [15] and risk perception attitude (RPA) framework [16] incorporate perceived efficacy, recognizing that relying solely on risk perception is inadequate for predicting individuals’ behavioral changes.

The current study used EPPM and RPA to explore the influence of perceived efficacy and perceived risk on individual obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behaviors. The inclusion of perceived efficacy and risk was aimed at enhancing our understanding of the factors that influence individuals’ engagement in efforts to prevent obesity.

The research questions (RQs) investigated in this study were as follows;

  • RQ1. How do the factors (i.e., perceived severity, perceived susceptibility, response efficacy, and self-efficacy) of the EPPM, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors relate to groups classified by body mass index (BMI)?

  • RQ2. How are obesity groups distributed based on RPA?

  • RQ3. How do obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behaviors vary by RPA type?

Extended parallel process model (EPPM)

The EPPM is a theoretical framework that elucidates individuals’ behavioral responses to fear-provoking messages [15]. Originally derived from the PPM proposed by Leventhal [8] and centered on the interplay between danger and fear control, the EPPM delves into the mechanisms through which individuals engage in adaptive protective behaviors as a result of their attempts to mitigate potential risks. Furthermore, EPPM incorporates key elements of the PMT proposed by Roger [7]. The EPPM contends that individuals do not solely engage in preventive behaviors by perceiving the presence of danger; rather, they require a robust conviction regarding the efficacy of disease prevention. Perceived risk serves as a driving force for the adoption of protective behaviors to ward off disease, whereas the nature of active or passive behaviors is mediated by individuals’ perceived efficacy of an action or behavior [15].

According to the EPPM, when individuals encounter fear-inducing stimuli, they engage in two simultaneous modes of message processing: cognitive processing (perceived efficacy appraisal) and emotional processing (perceived risk appraisal) [15]. The EPPM conceptualizes the process of evaluating risk as a means of fear control, with perceived risk playing a crucial role in determining this process. Additionally, the evaluation of coping mechanisms is depicted as a risk control process, with perceived efficacy serving as the key determinant. These two factors are further subdivided into the following dimensions: (1) perceived severity and susceptibility in relation to perceived risk, and (2) response efficacy and self-efficacy in relation to perceived efficacy. Perceived severity refers to an individual’s perception of the magnitude of risk. Perceived susceptibility refers to an individual’s perception of the likelihood of risk affecting them. Response efficacy refers to an individual’s perception that an action, if carried out, can successfully control risk. Self-efficacy refers to an individual’s perception of their competence in performing the tasks needed to control risk.

The EPPM differs from previous models (e.g., HBM, PMT, and PPM) that focus on fear appeal. While previous fear appeal models primarily concentrated on why fear appeals succeeded through perceived risk, the EPPM also explores why fear appeals fail through the interaction between perceived risk and perceived efficacy. The EPPM posits that when an individual experiences fear and perceives a high level of risk, risk and fear control processes are triggered based on the level of perceived efficacy. If individuals perceive high risk and efficacy, they are likely to exhibit an adaptive response; that is, a risk-control response. Conversely, if an individual perceives high risk but low efficacy, they tend to display a maladaptive response; that is, a fear-control response [17]. Hence, perceived efficacy plays a crucial moderating role in the risk- and fear-control processes.

Risk perception attitude (RPA)

The RPA framework developed by Rimal and Real [16] aims to categorize individuals into groups based on their perceived risk and efficacy concerning diseases. The RPA framework draws on the PMT by Rogers [7] and the EPPM introduced by Witte [15]. Additionally, the RPA framework seeks to elucidate variations in health outcomes observed among different groups [16, 18].

According to the RPA framework proposed by Rimal and Juon [19], individuals can be categorized into four distinct groups based on their perceived risk and efficacy. First, the responsive group (RG) is postulated to exhibit higher levels of health behaviors than the other groups, given its high perceived risk and efficacy. The avoidance group (AG) faces a dilemma in which their motivation to undertake preventive behaviors is undermined by high perceived risk and low perceived efficacy. Consequently, their fear levels may intensify, resulting in avoidant behaviors. For the proactive group (PG) and indifferent group (IG), the motivation to take action against potential hazards is low owing to their low perceived risk; in this case, perceived efficacy cannot easily stimulate health behaviors [16]. Thus, the RPA model assumes that, despite disparities in the magnitude of perceived efficacy between the PG and IG, these groups will not show discernible disparities in health behavioral outcomes owing to their low perceived risk [16].

The RPA model offers a theoretical framework that enables the classification of target audiences and the development of tailored campaign strategies for each group. The effectiveness of the RPA framework has been substantiated in various health domains including cardiovascular health [18], skin cancer prevention [16, 20], lung cancer prevention [21], human immunodeficiency virus /human immunodeficiency virus (AIDS/HIV) prevention [22], and coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) prevention [23].

Methods

Participants

This study analyzed data acquired from Macromill Embrain (https://embrain.com), a market research enterprise in Seoul, South Korea. The survey was administered online in October 2022. The participants were selected from Embrain’s extensive panel database, encompassing over 1 million participants. Specifically, this study focused on individuals aged 40–69 years who were residing in Korea during the study period. Individuals who were retired or classified as younger adults and did not meet the criteria for receiving preventive medical services under the universal healthcare system were excluded from this study. These selection criteria facilitated the establishment of a cohort characterized by homogeneous health behaviors, consequently enhancing the precision of the ensuing outcomes. The potential participants were approached online and written informed consent was obtained from each participant before their inclusion. Ethical approval was obtained from the Jeonju University Research Ethics Committee (JJIRB-220,526-HR-20,220,501).

Participant characteristics

Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of the participants. Age distribution revealed that 34.5%, 32.7%, and 32.7% of the individuals belonged to the 40–49, 50–59, and 61–69-year age groups, respectively, with an equal representation of males and females. The largest segment of participants consisted of college graduates. Moreover, most respondents were married, with 47.3% employed full-time, and their income levels showing a uniform distribution. Regarding self-rated health, 50.5% of the respondents reported that their health was fair. According to the obesity criteria based on the Korea Disease Control and Prevention Agency (KDCA), individuals are categorized as underweight, normal weight, overweight, and obese if their BMI is < 18.5 kg/m2, 18.5–22.9 kg/m2, 23.0–24.9 kg/m2, and ≥ 25 kg/m2, respectively. Based on the KDCA obesity criteria, 38 (3.5%), 506 (46.0%), 248 (22.6%), and 307 (27.9%) participants in this study were categorized as underweight, healthy weight, overweight, and obese, respectively.

Table 1 Participants’ characteristics

Measures

Table 2 shows specific questions about perceived risk (i.e., perceived severity and perceived susceptibility), perceived efficacy (i.e., response efficacy and self-efficacy), obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors.

Assessments for perceived risk and efficacy were conducted using the EPPM through the implementation of a five-point Likert scale [20, 22]. Consistent with prior research [24, 25] that employed the RPA framework to categorize participants based on median values, this study employed the same approach to classify individuals based on their medians of risk perception and perceived efficacy. The median values for perceived risk and perceived efficacy were both 3.50. Consequently, individuals in the high perceived risk and high perceived efficacy categories were designated as the RG. Those with a low perceived risk and high perceived efficacy were classified as having PG. Those with a high perceived risk and low perceived efficacy were classified as having AG. Those with low perceived risk and low perceived efficacy were referred to as the IG group.

Obesity knowledge was calculated as the sum of correct responses minus the sum of incorrect answers. The respondents were asked to answer five true-or-false questions regarding obesity-related facts according to the Korean Ministry of Health and Welfare [26]. The respondents indicated whether each statement was true or false or if they were unsure. Correct answers were scored one point, uncertain responses were scored zero points, and a point was subtracted from the total score for incorrect answers. The total possible score ranged from − 5 (all incorrect answers) to 5 (all correct answers). Obesity prevention behaviors were measured using four items obtained from the Obesity Prevention Scale developed by the Korean Ministry of Health [26]. The participants evaluated their engagement with each preventive measure on a five-point Likert scale.

Table 2 Measurement of perceived risk, perceived efficacy, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors

Analysis

Stata SE18 was used to conduct the multinomial logistic regression. The main objective of this study was to investigate the influence of participants’ characteristics, perceived risk, perceived efficacy, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behavior on the classification of groups based on BMI. Moreover, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to examine the main and interaction effects of perceived risk and perceived efficacy on obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behavior.

Results

Multinomial logistic regression results of participants’ characteristics, perceived risk, and perceived efficacy on classification according to BMI

The results for RQ 1 aligned with the findings of the multinomial logistic regression analysis presented in Table 3. First, compared with the healthy weight group, the likelihood of being underweight increased among females, those who were economically inactive, and those with increased self-efficacy. Second, compared with the healthy weight group, the probability of belonging to the overweight group increased among males and those with increased perceived susceptibility, increased response efficacy, decreased self-efficacy, increased obesity knowledge, and decreased obesity prevention behavior. Finally, compared with the healthy weight group, the probability of belonging to the obesity group increased among males; married individuals; and those with decreased perceived severity, increased perceived susceptibility, decreased self-efficacy, increased obesity knowledge, and decreased obesity prevention behavior.

Table 3 Multinomial logistic regression of characteristics, perceived risk, and perceived efficacy on classification according to BMI

Classification according to perceived risk and perceived efficacy

The results for RQ2 are presented in Table 4. A total of 182 respondents (16.5%) were categorized into the IG, which is characterized by low perceived risk and low perceived efficacy. The PG comprised 247 respondents (22.5%) with a low perceived risk and high perceived efficacy. The AG group comprised 301 individuals (27.4%) with high perceived risk and low perceived efficacy. Finally, the RG included 370 participants (33.6%) with high perceived risk and high perceived efficacy. The proportion of RG respondents was relatively high. The χ2 value for the group classification, 1.494, was not statistically significant.

Table 4 Groups based on perceived risk and perceived efficacy

Obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behavior according to groups based on risk perception attitude

The results for RQ3 correspond to the findings obtained from the GLM analysis presented in Table 5. This table presents the results of the main and interaction effects of perceived risk and perceived efficacy on obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behavior, considering the influence of sex, which consistently affects underweight, overweight, and obese relative to healthy weight.

Table 5 Knowledge and behaviors according to groups based on risk perception attitude framework

The main effects of sex and the perceived risk of obesity on knowledge were statistically significant. As illustrated in Fig. 1, participants in AG (1.979) and RG (1.808), characterized by higher perceived risk, demonstrated higher knowledge scores than those in IG (1.617) and PG (1.549), characterized by lower perceived risk.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Obesity knowledge according to perceived risk and perceived efficacy

Furthermore, the interaction effect between sex and perceived risk of obesity knowledge was statistically significant (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Obesity knowledge according to sex and perceived risk

Additionally, Table 6 shows the simple main effects of sex and perceived risk. The difference in sex within the low perceived risk group and the difference in perceived risk among men were statistically significant.

Table 6 Simple main effects of sex and perceived efficacy on obesity knowledge

The main effects of sex, perceived risk, and perceived efficacy on obesity prevention behaviors were statistically significant; however, the interaction effects were not. As shown in Fig. 3, participants in the PG (3.656) and RG (3.596) with higher perceived efficacy showed higher behavioral scores than those in the IG (3.106) and AG (3.002) with lower self-efficacy.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Obesity prevention behavior according to perceived risk and perceived efficacy

Discussion

This study analyzed a survey of individuals aged 40–69 years to examine the applicability of the EPPM and RPA for classification according to BMI, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors. The results confirmed the influences of perceived risk and efficacy, which are the main components of the EPPM and RPA, on the aforementioned factors.

The factors influencing the likelihood of belonging to the underweight, overweight, and obese groups relative to the healthy weight group were sex, perceived risk, perceived efficacy, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors.

Previous studies have reported differences in the prevalence of obesity between males and females [27,28,29,30]. Furthermore, reports have indicated that substantial differences in the prevalence of overweight and obesity and negative mental health resulting from being overweight or obese are more common among females than among males [31,32,33,34]. Therefore, the influence of sex must be considered in research on overweight and obesity.

Regarding perceived risk, perceived severity affected only the obese group, whereas perceived susceptibility affected the overweight and obese groups. The lower the perceived severity, the more probable it is that individuals will be classified into the obese group rather than the healthy weight group. Conversely, greater perceived susceptibility increases the likelihood of individuals belonging to the overweight or obese groups rather than to the group with a healthy weight. Interestingly, individuals in the obese group tended to underestimate the physical and economic risks associated with obesity. Moreover, this group and those around them had a high likelihood of developing obesity. This observation highlights the distinct effects of perceived severity and susceptibility, which are two components of perceived risk that manifest in contrasting ways. However, despite this, perceived severity and susceptibility still appear to be integrated as factors of perceived risk, and there seems to be no need to interpret them as independent factors within the framework of the EPPM in the context of obesity. Nevertheless, it is more valid to consider perceived severity as a factor that diminishes perceived risk in the obese group, while perceived susceptibility is a factor that heightens perceived risk in the overweight and obese groups.

Concerning perceived efficacy, response efficacy only influenced the overweight group, while self-efficacy exerted an impact not only on the overweight and obese groups but also on the underweight group. Specifically, in the overweight group, a significantly high evaluation of response efficacy was observed concerning obesity prevention behaviors. Conversely, obesity was not significantly associated with response efficacy. Consequently, distinct approaches should be implemented to educate overweight and obese individuals on obesity prevention behaviors. Specifically, the response efficacy in individuals with obesity must be enhanced.

Self-efficacy was identified as the most influential factor among the EPPM components. Higher self-efficacy scores were associated with an increased likelihood of belonging to the underweight group, whereas lower self-efficacy scores were associated with a higher probability of belonging to the overweight or obese groups. Consequently, the enhancement of self-efficacy, particularly in overweight and obese individuals, must be prioritized to promote the adoption of obesity prevention behaviors. Furthermore, the impact of obesity knowledge and prevention behaviors on BMI classification was not substantiated in the underweight group. In the overweight and obese groups, the influence was statistically significant. These findings indicate that higher obesity knowledge increased the likelihood of belonging to the overweight and obese groups. These results also suggest that the overweight and obese groups displayed relatively high levels of obesity knowledge, possibly because of their heightened awareness and involvement in obesity. Moreover, fewer obesity prevention behaviors increased the likelihood of belonging to the overweight and obese groups. When respondents were classified according to perceived risk and perceived efficacy relative to obesity, the RG showed the highest proportion (33.6%), followed by the IG (16.5%). The main effects of sex and perceived risk and the interaction effect of perceived risk and sex were statistically significant relative to obesity knowledge. Additionally, the main effects of sex, perceived risk, and perceived efficacy on obesity prevention behaviors were statistically significant.

Initially, female respondents demonstrated a greater degree of obesity knowledge than their male counterparts. Individuals with high perceived risk exhibited higher levels of obesity knowledge than those with low perceived risk. However, contrary to prior research findings [32,33,34], the AG displayed higher levels of obesity knowledge than the RG in this study. Nevertheless, the statistical analysis revealed non-significant outcomes for the main and interaction effects of perceived efficacy on obesity knowledge. These results imply that the disparity between the AG and the RG was not statistically significant.

Finally, regarding obesity prevention behaviors, the group with a low perceived risk achieved higher scores than the group with a high perceived risk. This finding contradicts those reported previously [16, 18, 22, 23], wherein higher perceived risk was associated with greater behavioral changes. The EPPM and RPA posit that a high level of perceived risk is necessary to elicit behavioral changes. In cases where the perceived risk is not sufficiently high, individuals are likely to exhibit non-responsive behaviors, avoidance, or a tendency to disengage from preventive actions. Nonetheless, in the context of obesity, in contrast to other illnesses or risks, the perceived severity of the risk may be considerably low. Despite WHO’s acknowledgment of obesity as a disease, individuals often tend to underestimate its severity [9,10,11]. Consequently, in the context of obesity, the influence of perceived risk may be diminished compared with that of other illnesses or risks. Therefore, a cautious approach is warranted when considering the impact of perceived risk in the application of EPPM and RPA to address obesity-related behaviors. Conversely, the effect size (ηp2) for the impact of perceived efficacy was substantial at 0.177, indicating the significant and sizable effect of perceived efficacy on obesity prevention behaviors [35]. Ultimately, the results of the present study revealed that EPPM and RPA provided explanatory power for classification according to BMI, obesity knowledge, and obesity prevention behaviors, with perceived efficacy exerting a stronger influence than perceived risk. This study introduces a novel research paradigm by applying EPPM and RPA, which have been previously employed in diverse health and risk contexts, to obesity. However, this study has several limitations, as outlined below.

First, this study deviates from previous research by uncovering contrasting findings regarding obesity prevention behaviors in relation to perceived risk. Specifically, the results indicated that individuals with a lower perceived risk exhibited higher scores on obesity prevention behavior. Hence, follow-up studies are required to validate and substantiate the effects of perceived risk.

Second, the inclusion criteria restricted the age range of the participants to individuals aged 40–69 years, thereby excluding children and adolescents. Future research should involve these populations as they are crucial to include in comprehensive obesity studies.

Data availability

The data are available upon request. Please contact the corresponding authors for further details.

Abbreviations

WHO:

World Health Organization

HBM:

Health Belief Model

PMT:

Protection Motivation Theory

PPM:

Parallel Process Model

EPPM:

Extended Parallel Process Model

RPA:

Risk Perception Attitude

BMI:

Body Mass Index

IG:

Indifferent Group

PG:

Proactive Group

AG:

Avoidance Group

RG:

Responsive Group

KDCA:

Korea Disease Control and Prevention Agency

References

  1. World Obesity Federation. World obesity Atlas 2023. Available from: https://data.worldobesity.org/publication.?cat=19. Accessed 2 August 2023.

  2. Field AE, Coakley EH, Must A, Spadano JL, Laird N, Dietz WH, Rimm E, Colditz S. Impact of overweight on the risk of developing common chronic diseases during a 10-year period. JAMA Intern Med. 2001;161(13):1581–6. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.161.13.1581.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Must A, Spadano J, Coakley EH, Field AE, Colditz G, Dietz WH. The disease burden associated with overweight and obesity. Jama. 1999; 282:1523–1529. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.282.16.1523.

  4. Baranowski T, Cullen KW, Nicklas T, Thompson D, Baranowski J. Are current health behavioral change models helpful in guiding prevention of weight gain efforts? Obes Res. 2003;11(Suppl):S23–43. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2003.222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Fishbein M, Ajzen I, Belief. Attitude, intention, and Behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Janz N, ChampionV, Strecher V. The health belief model, In: Glanz, K., Rimer, B., Lewis, F, editors, Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice, Josey-Bass, 2002. San Francisco, CA.

  7. Rogers R. Cognitive and psychological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: a revised theory of protection motivation. In: Cacioppo J, Petty R, editors. Social psychobiology: a sourcebook. New York: Guildford; 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Leventhal H. Fear appeals and persuasion: the differentiation of a motivational construct. Am J Public Health. 1971;61:1208–24. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.61.6.1208.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Lee T, Oliver J. Public Opinion and the Politics of America’s Obesity Epidemic. John F. Kennedy School of Government. Harvard University Faculty Research Working Paper Series, 2002. RWP02-017, Cambridge, MA.

  10. Margetts BM, Rogers E, Widhal K, Remaut de Winter AM, Zunft HJ. Relationship between attitudes to health, body weight and physical activity and level of physical activity in a nationally representative sample in the European Union. Public Health Nutr. 1999;2:97–103. https://doi.org/10.1017/s368980099000142.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ziebland S, Robertson J, Jay J, Neil A. Body image and weight change in middle age: a qualitative study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002;26:1083–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Horm J, Anderson K. Who in America is trying to lose weight? Ann Intern Med. 1993;119:672–6. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-119-7_part_2-199310011-00009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Wardle J, Johnson F. Weight and dieting: examining levels of weight concern in British adults. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002;26:1144–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0802046.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Yoon E, Seo SH. Differences on perceptions and attitudes towards food safety based on behavioral intention to prevent foodborne illness among middles school students in Seoul. Korean J Food Cook Sci. 2012;28:149–58. https://doi.org/10.9724/kfcs.2012.28.2.149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Witte K. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the extended parallel process model. Communication Monogr. 1992;59:329–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759209376276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Rimal RN, Real K. Perceived risk and efficacy beliefs as motivators of change: use of the risk perception attitude (RPA) framework to understand health behaviors. Hum Comm Res. 2003;29:370–99. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410230902804109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Cha DP. The role of risk and efficacy in the fear appeal messages: multiplicative relationship, or additive relationship? Korean J Advertising Public Relations. 2007;9(2):339–63.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Rimal RN. Perceived risk and self-efficacy as motivators: understanding individuals’ long term use of health information. J Commun. 2001;51:633–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2001. tb02900.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Rimal RN, Juon H-S. Use of the risk perception attitude framework for promoting breast cancer prevention. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010;40(2):287–310. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00574.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Turner MM, Rimal RN, Morrison D, Kim H. The role of anxiety in seeking and retaining risk information: testing the risk perception attitude framework in two studies. Hum Commun Res. 2006;32:130–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2006.000006.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Turner J, Pond GR, Tremblay A, Johnston M, Goss G, Nicholas G, Martel S, Bhatia R, Liu G, Schmidt H, Tammemagi MC, Puksa S, Atkar-Khattra S, Tsao M, Lam S, Goffin J. Risk perception among a lung cancer screening population. Chest J. 2021;160(2):718–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chest.2021.02.050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Rimal RN, Bose K, Brown J, Mkandawire G, Folda L. Extending the purview of the risk perception attitude framework: findings from HIV/AIDS prevention research in Malawi. Health Commun. 2009;24:210–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410230902804109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Nam S-J. Food consumption behavior in a health crisis: application of the risk perception attitude framework during the COVID-19 pandemic, Human and Ecological Risk Assessment. Int J. 2022;28:930–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/10807039.2022.2112505.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Ko Dh, Song H. Effects on preventive behavior intention to particulate matter by risk perception attitude and anxiety: focus on adoption of RPA model. J Digit Convergence. 2020;18(10):517–26. https://doi.org/10.1440/JDC.2020.18.10.517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Sullivan HW, Beckjord EB, Rutten LF, Hesse BW. Nutrition-Related Cancer Prevention cognitions and behavioral intentions: testing the risk perception attitude Framework. Health Educ Behav. 2008;35(6):866–8793. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198108326164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Korean Ministry of Health and Welfare. Get to know obesity. Korean Ministry of Health and Welfare. Seoul; 2022.

  27. Carsley S, Pope EI, Anderson LN, Tremblay MS, Tu K, Birken CS. Temporal trends in severe obesity prevalence in children and youth from primary care electronic medical records in Ontario: a repeated cross-sectional study. CMAJ Open. 2019;7:E351–9. https://doi.org/10.9778/cmajo.20180174.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Kowal M, Woronkowicz A, Kryst Ł, Sobiecki J, Poledki MW. Sex differences in prevalence of overweight and obesity, and in extent of overweight index, in children and adolescents (3–18 years) from Kraków, Poland in 1983, 2000 and 2010. Public Health Nutr. 2015;19:1035–46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980015002281.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Rodd C, Sharma AK. Recent trends in the prevalence of overweight and obesity among Canadian children. CMAJ. 2016;188:E313–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Wang R, Zhang P, Gao C, Li Z, Lv X, Song Y, Yu Y, Li B. Prevalence of overweight and obesity and some associated factors among adult residents of northeast China: a cross-sectional study. BMJ OPEN. 2016;6:e010828. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010828.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Bays HE, Bazata DD, Fox KM, Grandy S, Gavin JR, Group SS. Perceived body image in men and women with type 2 diabetes mellitus: correlation of body mass index with the figure rating scale. Nutr J. 2009;6:57. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2891-8-57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Silvestris E, Pergola G, Rosania R, Loverro G. Obesity as disruptor of the female fertility. Reproductive Biology Endocrinol. 2018;16:22. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12958-018-0336-z.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Nam S-J, Park J-H. The moderating effects of sex relationship between obesity, well-being, and stress perception in Korean adolescents. BMC Public Health. 2021;21:1859. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11894-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Paeratakul S, White MA, Williamson DA, Ryan DH, Bray GA. Sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and BMI in relation to self-perception of overweight. Obes Res. 2002;10:345–50. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2002.48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The authors (s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, or publication of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SJN conceptualized the study design. JHP and SJN drafted the manuscript. JEK and NCK revised the manuscript. All authors have read, provided feedback, and approved the final manuscript. All authors are accountable for all aspects of this study.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Su-Jung Nam.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the ethics committee of Jeonju University (JJIRB-220,526-HR-20,220,501). All participants provided informed consent before participating in the study. The data were anonymized and linked by a market research company before access was granted for research purposes.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Park, JH., Nam, SJ., Kim, JE. et al. Application of the extended parallel process model and risk perception attitude framework to obesity knowledge and obesity prevention behaviors among Korean adults. BMC Public Health 24, 748 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18268-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18268-5

Keywords