Skip to main content

Are physical activity and sleep associated with emotional self-regulation in toddlers? a cross-sectional study

Abstract

Background

Toddlerhood (2–3 years) is a crucial period for the development of physical activity, sleep, and emotional self-regulation skills. Although there is growing evidence of positive associations between physical activity, sleep, and emotional self-regulation in school-aged children, the associations in toddlers remain unclear. Thus, this study aimed to examine the association between physical activity, sleep, and emotional self-regulation in toddlers.

Methods

Baseline data from 1350 toddlers (2.2 ± 0.33 year) from the Let’s Grow randomised controlled trial were used. Toddlers’ total physical activity (TPA) and moderate- to vigorous-intensity physical activity (MVPA) were assessed via 4 + days of accelerometry and a 3-item parent-report scale. Parent-reported total sleep quantity was calculated using the sum of average night-time sleep and daytime nap durations. Sleep behaviour data including bedtime routine, bedtime resistance, sleep onset-delay, sleep duration, and night waking were collected using relevant subsections from the Child Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ) and Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire revised (BISQ-R). A 4-item parent-report scale adapted from the Fast Track Project Child Behaviour Questionnaire was used to assess toddlers’ emotional self-regulation. Linear regression models were used, adjusting for child age, sex, and parental education.

Results

Accelerometer-derived TPA, MVPA and parent-reported TPA were not associated with emotional self-regulation. Higher parent-reported MVPA (B = -0.01 CI95 -0.03, -0.003) was associated with poorer emotional self-regulation. Higher sleep duration was associated with better emotional self-regulation (B = 0.06 CI95 0.04, 0.08). The five sleep behaviours assessed were also positively associated with emotional self-regulation (all p < 0.01), with fewer problem sleep behaviours being associated with better emotional self-regulation.

Conclusions

This cross-sectional study suggests that sleep may be important for emotional self-regulation in toddlers, but the role of physical activity remains unclear. These findings suggest that interventions targeting sleep duration and sleep behaviours during the early toddler years may benefit the positive development of emotional self-regulation skills in this population.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Self-regulation skills refer to an individual’s ability to manage their cognitions, emotions and behaviour, and respond to situational demands [1]. Self-regulation encompasses these three domains that are interrelated but distinguishable [2] and respectively refer to the cognitive, emotional, and behavioural impulses of an individual [3,4,5]. The cognitive domain aids in reasoning, planning, problem solving and goal-oriented thinking, while the behavioural domain is central to beneficial learning experiences, social interaction, and school readiness [6, 7]. Emotional self-regulation, the domain that is the focus of this study, is key to higher-order cognitive processes and behaviour regulation [1, 8, 9]. Competent emotional self-regulation is complex and includes both the ability to successfully manage one’s emotions and adaptively use emotions depending on the situation [10]. Emotional self-regulation skills are fundamental to successful adaptations in challenging situations across the lifespan [11], including fostering social competence, higher academic achievement, better school readiness [1, 11, 12], and are crucial to the development of the other domains (cognitive and behavioural) of self-regulation [1, 13]. Thus, acquiring emotional self-regulation skills is a critical achievement of early childhood (0–5 years) [14,15,16]. Emotional self-regulation skills in the early years are malleable [1, 10]. Many factors influence a young child’s emotional self-regulation, including parent-child interactions, the social environment, and child temperament [17, 18], in addition to modifiable heath behaviours like physical activity and sleep [19,20,21,22].

Contemporary research suggests that physical activity plays an important role in emotional self-regulation for adolescents [8, 23, 24] but evidence for school-aged children is scant [25]. Although evidence for an association between physical activity and emotional self-regulation in preschool-aged children (3–5 years) [19, 20, 26] is growing, the findings remain inconsistent and evidence in toddlers (24–36 months) is scarce [27]. Most studies that have examined the association between physical activity and the various domains of self-regulation in early childhood have focused on the cognitive domain, with little attention paid to the emotional and behavioural domains [28].

For sleep, research has been limited to consideration of associations between sleep problems and emotional self-regulation [21, 29,30,31,32]. Poor sleep patterns have been associated with emotional difficulties in toddlers [21] and are associated with emotional self-regulation difficulties in pre-schoolers [33, 34], and across childhood [35]. Research in toddlers indicates that sleep deprivation/restriction curbs their ability to effectively manage emotional challenges, increases emotional reactivity, reinforces sensitivity to negative stimuli and diminishes sensitivity to positive stimuli [36, 37]. Despite previous studies finding meaningful associations between sleep and emotional self-regulation, the majority have examined associations between sleep problems/deprivation and emotional self-regulation problems. It is also important to consider the different facets of regular sleep such as sleep quantity, duration consistency, bedtime routine, bedtime resistance, sleep onset-delay, and, night-time awakening as they may be differentially associated with emotional self-regulation [21, 38]. In a study of 18-month-olds, Sivertsen et al. found that sleep behaviour issues such as shorter sleep duration and increased night time awakening were associated with both concurrent and subsequent incidence of emotional self-regulation difficulties in pre-schoolers [34].

Whether sleep duration and sleep behaviours are associated with emotional self-regulation in young children remains unclear but there is a theoretical basis for hypothesising that sleep may influence emotional self-regulation in a similar way to physical activity. The temporal self-regulation theory (TST) model suggests a positive feedback loop between physical activity behaviours and the brain regions that are responsible for self-regulation development [39]. Applying this model, we can hypothesis that there also exists a positive feedback loop between sleep and self-regulation. Neuroscience research and FMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) studies provide evidence that sleep modulates the emotional brain response systems (pre-frontal cortex, limbic system, and amygdala) [40]. Lack of sleep results in pre-frontal amygdala disconnect which in turn results in disruptive emotional-behaviour regulation [40, 41]. Given that the brain is more plastic during infancy and early childhood than later in life [42], then we posit that the neuronal plasticity of the amygdala and pre-frontal cortex (brain region involved in emotional self-regulation development) could be enhanced through adequate sleep and improved physical activity, in the early years.

Currently research has focused on understanding the association between either physical activity or sleep with emotional self-regulation. Exploring both behaviours in the same toddler cohort is novel and will enable us to understand if one behaviour is more strongly associated with emotional self-regulation than the other behaviour. Previous studies [26, 43] that have investigated both physical activity and sleep in relation to overall self-regulation have been able to compare the strength of associations for the two behaviours but provided inconsistent results. No previous studies have looked at a specific domain of self-regulation which may provide greater clarity into these associations. Toddlerhood is a transition period from dependency to budding autonomy, where rapid changes occur; not only across the emotional domain of self-regulation [44], but also in health behaviours like sleep and physical activity [36, 45,46,47]. It is also the foundational period for the development of physical activity behaviours [45], sleep habits [38] and emotional processing [45, 48], thus understanding the associations between these constructs is important to identify any risk factors that may contribute to adverse developmental trajectories in young children [34]. Understanding this will further help inform targeted intervention strategies, and toddler-specific physical activity and sleep guidelines. Therefore, the current study aimed to understand the associations between these health behaviours and emotional self-regulation skills in toddlers. More specifically, we hypothesise that:

  1. 1.

    Higher accelerometer-derived and parent-reported moderate- to vigorous-intensity physical activity (MVPA) or total physical activity (TPA) will be associated with better emotional self-regulation in toddlers.

  2. 2.

    Higher parent-reported sleep quantity, and sleep behaviours (sleep duration, bedtime resistance, night waking, sleep onset delay, sleep consistency, sleep location, bedtime routine) will be associated with better emotional self-regulation in toddlers.

Methods

Participants and procedures

This cross-sectional study used baseline data from the Let’s Grow randomised controlled trial (RCT) targeting parents of two-year-old children across Australia [49]. To be eligible, participants needed to (1) have a child aged between 18 and 35 months; (2) be a resident of Australia; (3) have a mobile phone capable of accessing the internet; (4) be able to complete surveys and engage with intervention content in English; and (5) have a child who was walking independently. Participants were recruited between March 2021 – July 2022 via social media platforms (e.g., Facebook, Instagram, Twitter, parenting blogs) and snowball recruitment. All participants provided informed consent prior to participating in the study. Children who had a condition that impacted their movement, sleep, or emotional self-regulation (e.g., requires a wheelchair, has a clinical sleep problem [e.g., sleep apnoea], or autism spectrum disorder) were excluded from data analyses for the current study (n = 25). Let’s Grow was approved by the Deakin University Human Research Ethics Committee (2020-077).

Measures

Exposure variables

Physical activity

Children wore ActiGraph GT3X + accelerometers fastened to an elastic belt on the right hip, just above the iliac crest, continuously for 24 h across eight consecutive days to objectively assess their physical activity. Monitors were mailed to the participants along with information and instructions on how to wear them. Accelerometer data counts were recorded in 5-second epochs to ensure that the sporadic nature of the young children’s movement was captured [50, 51], and non-wear time was classified as 20 min of consecutive zero counts [52]. The frequency of the accelerometer data was set to 30 Hz. To be included in analyses children were required to have data recorded for at least 7.4 h per day for at least four days (weekdays or weekend days) [53, 54]. Accelerometer data were processed using a customised Microsoft Excel macro. Total physical activity (TPA) was defined as > 25 counts/15s and MVPA as > 417 using Trost’s cut points specific for toddlers [52].

Toddlers’ physical activity was also assessed via parent report of the average time their toddler spent being physically active indoors and outdoors for both weekdays and weekends in a typical week and the proportion of time that their toddler spent in MVPA on a usual day. These questions were purpose designed for Let’s Grow. Daily TPA was obtained by calculating the weighted average of weekday and weekend responses ([average weekday TPA*5 + average weekend TPA*2]/7); average MVPA was calculated similarly ([average weekday MVPA*5 + average weekend MVPA*2]/7).

Sleep

Toddlers’ sleep was measured via parent report using the Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ) [55] and Brief Infant Questionnaire (BISQ-R - revised/extended version) [56]. In addition, the survey questions on and usual bedtime, waketime, amount of night-time sleep, and nap for weekdays and weekends separately, which were purpose designed for Let’s Grow, were also used in this study. Four subscales from the CSHQ [55, 57] were used including bedtime resistance (6 items), sleep duration (3 items), night waking (3 items) and sleep onset delay (1 item), with all items rated on a 3 point likert scale from 1 (usually) to 3 (rarely). Internal consistency (alpha = 0.68 to 0.82) [55] and test-retest reliability (alpha = 0.62 to 0.79) [57] for the sub-scales of the CSHQ used in this study were both acceptable. The following question was adapted from the from the larger 33-item BISQ-R [56] which has established reliability and validity, and used in this study: In a typical week how often does [child_firstname] have a bedtime routine? Parents responded to this question using a 5-point likert scale from ‘never’ to ‘every night’. Average night-time sleep duration was obtained by calculating the weighted average for both weekday and weekend sleep (i.e., [usual weekday night-time sleep*5 + usual weekend night-time sleep*2]/7). Average nap duration was calculated in the same way. Total sleep quantity was then calculated by taking the sum of the average nap and night-time sleep durations.

Outcome variables

Emotional regulation

The emotional regulation questions were adapted from the Fast-Track Project Child Behaviour Checklist. The original Fast Track Project [58] Child Behaviour Questionnaire [59] is a 20-item questionnaire that was designed to measure self-regulation skills in children and adolescents; four questions from this measure were used in the current study. Parents reported on their toddlers’ emotional self-regulation skills on a 4-point scale from ‘all of the time’ to ‘none of the time’ by responding to the following statements: (1) child copes well when things don’t go their way; (2) child has tantrums; (3) child is defiant; and (4) when child gets upset or angry, they can calm themselves down quickly. A composite score was created by taking the sum of the scores of the four items (two of the items were reverse scored), with higher scores indicating better emotional self-regulation. In the current study the internal consistency for this scale was acceptable (α = 0.68).

Data analyses

Stata 17.0 (StataCorp, Texas, USA) was used to perform the data analyses. Descriptive statistics were used to characterize the sample characteristics and behavioural variables. Accelerometer variables were adjusted for wear-time using the residual methods [60]. A series of linear regression models were conducted to determine the associations between accelerometer derive MVPA, TPA, parent reported MVPA, TPA, sleep quantity, sleep behaviours (bedtime routine, bedtime resistance, sleep onset-delay, sleep duration, and night waking) and emotional self-regulation while adjusting for child age, sex, and parental education. The statistical significance level was set to above 95% confidence interval (i.e., p > 0.05).

Results

Data were collected from 1350 children. To be included in analyses, children needed to have complete data for emotional self-regulation scores (n = 1350) and any physical activity or sleep data. Complete case analyses were conducted for each of the exposure variables.

Sample characteristics are reported in Table 1. The average accelerometer-derived TPA was over 4 h including around 1 h of MVPA. Average parent-reported TPA was over 5 h and MVPA was more than 1.5 h. The average parent-reported sleep duration was over 12 h, comprised roughly of 10.5 h of night-time sleep and 1.5 h of nap time.

Table 1 Sample characteristics (n = 1350), mean ± SD unless otherwise noted

Physical activity and emotional self-regulation

Table 2 shows the results for associations between accelerometer-derived and parent-reported TPA and MVPA with emotional self-regulation. After adjusting for age, sex, and parental education, accelerometer-derived TPA (B = -0.16 CI95 -0.06 0.03) and MVPA (B = -0.04 CI95 -0.16, 0.07) and parent-reported TPA (B = -0.01 CI95 -0.02, 0.00) were not associated with emotional self-regulation. However, higher parent-reported MVPA was associated with poorer emotional self-regulation (B = -0.09 CI95 -0.03, -0.00).

Table 2 Associations of physical activity and sleep behaviours with emotional self-regulation

Sleep and emotional self-regulation

The associations between sleep and emotional self-regulation are presented in Table 2.

After adjusting for age, sex and parental education, higher daily nap duration (B = 0.047 CI95 0.012, 0.082), night-time sleep duration (B = 0.059 CI95 0.038, 0.081) and total sleep duration (B = 0.063 CI95 0.042, 0.082), were associated with better emotional self-regulation skills, as was higher bedtime routine consistency (B = 0.040 CI95 0.011, 0.069). In contrast, increased bedtime resistance (B = -0.017 CI95 -0.247, -0.010), night-time awaking (B = -0.024 CI95 -0.036, -0.011), sleep onset delay (B = -0.079 CI95 -0.111, -0.047), and sleep duration consistency problems (B = -0.077 CI95 -0.095, -0.059) were all associated with poorer emotional self-regulation.

Discussion

This study analysed the cross-sectional associations between physical activity, sleep and toddlers’ emotional self-regulation. This study advances the current literature by focusing on young children and by examining the associations between both accelerometer-derived and parent-reported physical activity with emotional self-regulation, and the lesser explored sleep constructs such as sleep quantity and sleep behaviours. While sleep quantity and sleep behaviours all showed consistent positive associations with emotional self-regulation in toddlers, associations for physical activity were inconsistent but mostly suggested no association.

Physical activity and emotional self-regulation

We found no associations between accelerometer-derived physical activity and emotional self-regulation. When examining parent-reported physical activity, we found no association between TPA and emotional self-regulation, but a negative association between parent-reported MVPA and emotional self-regulation. However, it is important to consider the cross-sectional nature of the study. It could be that parents’ perceptions of their toddler’s MVPA levels might have been influenced by their toddlers’ apparent emotional self-regulation. Parents may have viewed children with increased MVPA levels as having poor emotional self-regulation skills or children with poor emotional self-regulation skills as being more physically active. For example, parents may be more inclined to rate more active children as having poor emotional self-regulation, as they often expect children be calm particularly in social situations. More active children could be viewed as ‘defiant’ or unable to remain calm and hence may be more likely to be rated as having poor emotional self-regulation skills by their parents. Conversely, children with poor emotional self-regulation could be viewed as more active, due to their inability to confer to social expectation of remaining calm.

Previous studies in the early childhood population (0–5 years) have reported positive [20, 61], negative [27] and no associations [62] between physical activity and emotional self-regulation. The findings of this study fail to clarify the associations. Conversely, research in adolescence suggests positive associations between physical activity and emotional self-regulation [8, 23, 24, 63]. It could be that the influence of physical activity on emotional self-regulation only becomes perceptible as children grow older. Further, we suspect that methodological aspects, including study design, the measure of physical activity used, and intensity considered, may contribute to the inconsistent findings in the literature in young children. Some studies measured physical activity subjectively (parent/teacher report), some objectively (accelerometer/direct observation) and some not at all, assuming that introduction of a physical activity program will increase overall physical activity levels. Thus, suggesting the lack of consistency in the study design and measures used, as a potential contributor to the inconsistent findings.

Sleep and emotional self-regulation

Overall, we found that sleep quantity and a range of sleep behaviours were beneficially associated with emotional self-regulation in toddlers. Poor sleep behaviours such as increased bedtime resistance, increased night-time awakening, longer sleep onset delay and increased inconsistency in sleep duration were associated with poorer emotional self-regulation. Conversely, all measures of sleep duration/quantity were associated with better emotional self-regulation, as was having a consistent bedtime routine. These findings concur with previous studies, which report positive associations between sleep and social-emotional difficulties in older children and adolescence [48, 64, 65]. Findings of the current study suggest that the associations between sleep and emotional self-regulation appear to begin from as young as toddlerhood. Understanding the connection between sleep and emotional self-regulation may help to inform interventions aimed at improving sleep from a very young age.

We posit that young children who struggle with emotional self-regulation may experience difficulty falling asleep due to separation anxiety and may exhibit bedtime resistance and have increased sleep onset delay. In addition, their inability to self-soothe and resettle may lead to increased night waking and inconsistencies in sleep duration [48]. Further, sleep is associated with the brain regions including pre-frontal cortex and the amygdala which are key regions responsible for emotional self-regulation [66], thus healthy sleep could aid in enhanced processing experiences and consequently contribute to improved emotional self-regulation. Hence, the relationship could be potentially bidirectional. Our findings confirm previous findings that better sleep is associated with better emotional self-regulation, although the reciprocal nature of the associations remain unclear. Parents of young children often grapple to effectively manage their child’s sleep habits, which often results in tantrums and difficulties during bedtime, making bedtime particularly stressful for both the parent and the child. By understanding the underlying mechanisms and the association between sleep and emotional self-regulation, we can provide parents with practical solutions to help their child regulate and address these bedtime struggles. However, the cross-sectional nature of the association precludes establishing any causal relationship. Additionally, the subjective nature of the assessment tools used for both sleep and emotional self-regulation must be considered when interpreting the findings, as they may be subject to parental bias of either overestimation or underestimation of the behaviours.

Strengths, limitations and future directions

The present study included a large sample from across Australia. It strengthens the literature by addressing several gaps in an emerging area of research. It is the first to examine the association between emotional self-regulation and physical activity assessed using accelerometry and parent-report. Most of the sleep literature has focused on the association between sleep problems and emotional self-regulation problems from a clinical perspective. There is a paucity of literature examining the association of sleep quantity and behaviours with emotional self-regulation skills, and this study adds to the literature by addressing this gap. The results highlight the importance of including broader sleep constructs like healthy sleep behaviours in addition to the amount of sleep, while attempting to understand the role of sleep in the development of functional emotional self-regulation skills in toddlers. However, future studies would benefit from using objective measures of both sleep and emotional self-regulation.

This study is not without limitations. The cross-sectional nature prevents us from determining directionality or causality; longitudinal studies and RCTs examining bidirectionality are necessary to confirm direction and causal associations. The mismatch between the epoch length used for data collection in this study (5 s) and the length used to generate the Trost cutpoints applied in this study (15 s) could also be considered a limitation. While Trost’s cutpoints are well established for use with toddlers and have shown good reliability and validity in this population [52], they have not been validated for 5 s epochs. Shorter recording epochs are recommended to capture the sporadic nature of young children’s activity [50, 51], and 5 s epochs have been shown to produce the most accurate estimate of MVPA (compared to a range of longer epochs) across various cutpoints [51]. While it is acknowledged that established cutpoints are often adjusted to epoch lengths other than what they were originally validated for [50] such adjustment may not be ideal. Data collected in shorter epochs may overestimate activity levels when applied with cutpoints generated using longer epoch durations [50].

The present study used subjective assessments of sleep and emotional self-regulation, which is another limitation. It is possible that parent reports of their child’s emotional self-regulation may be influenced by their individual views, beliefs, and socio-cultural expectations as discussed above. Further this study is also limited in that it has not included sedentary behaviour, another key health behaviour which may impact emotional self-regulation. Future studies Including sedentary behaviour alongside physical activity and sleep would allow investigation of the relative influence and interplay of these three movement behaviours with emotional self-regulation within the context of the 24-hour day.

Conclusions

This study aimed to examine associations of physical activity and sleep with emotional self-regulation in toddlers. The findings indicate that physical activity is inconsistently associated with emotional self-regulation. Longer duration of sleep and healthy sleep behaviours were associated with better emotional self-regulation. Consistent with previous literature in pre-schoolers, school-aged children, and adolescents, this study highlights the important role that sleep duration and sleep behaviours play in toddlers’ emotional self-regulation skills. The findings suggests that sleep behaviours are important targets for interventions aimed at improving young children’s sleep and emotional self-regulation. However, more research is needed to fully understand the role physical activity plays in the development of emotional self-regulation skills in toddlers.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BISQ-R:

Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire – Revised

CSHQ:

Child Sleep Habits Questionnaire

MVPA:

Moderate- to vigorous-intensity physical activity

RCT:

Randomise controlled trial

TPA:

Total physical activity

References

  1. Blair C, Calkins S, Kopp L. Self-regulation as the interface of emotional and cognitive development: implications for education and academic achievement. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Handbook of personality and self-regulation. Wiley-Blackwell; 2010. pp. 64–90.

  2. Berger A, Kofman O, Livneh U, Henik A. Multidisciplinary perspectives on attention and the development of self-regulation. Prog Neurobiol. 2007;82(5):256–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Duckworth AL, Carlson SM. Self-regulation and school success. Self-regulation and autonomy: Social and developmental dimensions of human conduct. 2013;40:208.

  4. Raver CC. Placing emotional self-regulation in Sociocultural and socioeconomic contexts. Child Dev. 2004;75(2):346–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Robinson LE, Palmer KK, Bub KL. Effect of the Children’s Health Activity Motor Program on Motor skills and Self-Regulation in Head Start preschoolers: an efficacy trial. Front Public Health. 2016;4:173.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Blair C, Raver CC. Individual development and evolution: experiential canalization of self-regulation. Dev Psychol. 2012;3:647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Ponitz CC, McClelland MM, Matthews JS, Morrison FJ. A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Dev Psychol. 2009;45(3):605–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Vasilopoulos F, Ellefson MR. Investigation of the associations between physical activity, self-regulation and educational outcomes in childhood. PLoS ONE. 2021;16(5):1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Williams KE, Nicholson JM, Walker S, Berthelsen D. Early childhood profiles of sleep problems and self-regulation predict later school adjustment. Br J Educ Psychol. 2016;86(2):331–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Grolnick WS, McMenamy JM, Kurowski CO. Emotional self-regulation in infancy and toddlerhood. Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues. 1999:3–22.

  11. Wang M, Saudino KJ. Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in emotion regulation and its relation to working memory in toddlerhood. Emotion. 2013;13(6):1055.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Blair C, Raver CC. School readiness and self-regulation: a developmental psychobiological approach. Ann Rev Psychol. 2015;66:711–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Ursache A, Blair C, Stifter C, Voegtline K. Emotional reactivity and regulation in infancy interact to predict executive functioning in early childhood. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(1):127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Sroufe L. The organization of emotional life in the early years. NY NY: Cambridge. 1996.

  15. Bronson M. Self-regulation in early childhood: nature and nurture. Guilford Press; 2000.

  16. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD. Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory and applications. Guilford Press; 2004.

  17. Rosanbalm K, Murray D. Promoting self-regulation in the First Five years: a practice brief. OPRE brief 2017-79. Administration for Children & Families; 2017.

  18. Carrère S, Bowie BH. Like parent, like child: parent and child emotion dysregulation. Arch Psychiatr Nurs. 2012;26(3):e23–e30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lindsey Eric W, Malinda JC. Pretend and physical play: links to preschoolers’ affective social competence. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2013;59(3):330–60.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Xie S, Li X, Wang S. Effects of combined exercise therapy on positive self evaluation and emotional regulation of children: an analysis based on randomized controlled trial. Revista Argentina De Clínica Psicológica. 2020;29(2):901.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Mindell JA, Leichman ES, DuMond C, Sadeh A. Sleep and Social-Emotional Development in infants and toddlers. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychology: Official J Soc Clin Child Adolesc Psychol Am Psychol Association Div 53. 2017;46(2):236–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Zheng M, Rangan A, Olsen NJ, Heitmann BL. Longitudinal association of nighttime sleep duration with emotional and behavioral problems in early childhood: results from the Danish healthy start study. J SLEEP. 2021.

  23. Román-Mata S, Puertas-Molero P, Ubago-Jiménez JL, González-Valero G. Benefits of physical activity and its associations with resilience, emotional intelligence, and psychological distress in university students from Southern Spain. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(12):4474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Acebes-Sánchez J, Diez-Vega I, Esteban-Gonzalo S, Rodriguez-Romo G. Physical activity and emotional intelligence among undergraduate students: a correlational study. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Cañabate D, Santos M, Rodríguez D, Serra T, Colomer J. Emotional self-regulation through introjective practices in physical education. Educ Sci. 2020;10(8):208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Alonso-Martínez AM, Ramírez-Vélez R, García-Alonso Y, Izquierdo M, García-Hermoso A. Physical activity, sedentary behavior, sleep and self-regulation in Spanish preschoolers during the COVID-19 Lockdown. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(2).

  27. Dealey RP, Stone MH. Exploring out-of-School Play and Educational Readiness. Early Childhood Educ J. 2018;46(2):201–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Carson V, Hunter S, Kuzik N, Wiebe SA, Spence JC, Friedman A, et al. Systematic review of physical activity and cognitive development in early childhood. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(7):573–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Reid GJ, Hong RY, Wade TJ. The relation between common sleep problems and emotional and behavioral problems among 2- and 3-year-olds in the context of known risk factors for psychopathology. J Sleep Res. 2009;18(1):49–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Sadeh A, Tikotzky L, Kahn M. Sleep in infancy and childhood: implications for emotional and behavioral difficulties in adolescence and beyond. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2014;27(6).

  31. Sivertsen B, Harvey AG, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Torgersen L, Ystrom E, Hysing M. Later emotional and behavioral problems associated with sleep problems in toddlers: a longitudinal study. JAMA pediatrics. United States: American Medical Association; 2015. pp. 575–82.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Hysing M, Sivertsen B, Garthus-Niegel S, Eberhard-Gran M. Pediatric sleep problems and social-emotional problems. A population-based study. Infant Behav Dev. 2016;42:111–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Troxel WM, Trentacosta CJ, Forbes EE, Campbell SB. Negative emotionality moderates associations among attachment, toddler sleep, and later problem behaviors. J Fam Psychol. 2013;27(1):127–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Conway A, Miller AL, Modrek A. Testing reciprocal links between trouble getting to sleep and internalizing behavior problems, and bedtime resistance and externalizing behavior problems in toddlers. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2017;48(4):678–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Winsper C, Wolke D. Infant and toddler crying, sleeping and feeding problems and trajectories of dysregulated behavior across childhood. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42(5):831–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Berger RH, Miller AL, Seifer R, Cares SR, LeBourgeois MK. Acute sleep restriction effects on emotion responses in 30- to 36‐month‐old children. J Sleep Res. 2012;21(3):235–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Gujar N, McDonald SA, Nishida M, Walker MP. A role for rem sleep in recalibrating the sensitivity of the human brain to specific emotions. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(1):115–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Schumacher AM, Miller AL, Watamura SE, Kurth S, Lassonde JM, LeBourgeois MK. Sleep Moderates the Association Between Response Inhibition and Self-Regulation in Early Childhood. Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology: the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53. 2017;46(2):222 – 35.

  39. Hall PA, Fong GT. Temporal self-regulation theory: a neurobiologically informed model for physical activity behavior. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9(117).

  40. Yoo S-S, Gujar N, Hu P, Jolesz FA, Walker MP. The human emotional brain without sleep–a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr Biology: CB. 2007;17(20):R877–R8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour: implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:413–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Huttenlocher PR. Neural plasticity: the effects of environment on the development of the cerebral cortex. Harvard University Press; 2002.

  43. St. Laurent CW, Rasmussen CL, Holmes JF, Cremone-Caira A, Kurdziel LB, Desrochers PC, Spencer RM. Associations of activity, sedentary, and sleep behaviors with cognitive and social-emotional health in early childhood. J Activity Sedentary Sleep Behav. 2023;2(1):7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Campbell SB. Behavior problems in preschool children: a review of recent research. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1995;36(1):113–49.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Telama R, Hirvensalo M, Yang X, Kankaanpää A, Tammelin T, Leskinen E, et al. Tracking of physical activity from early childhood through youth into adulthood. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46(5):955–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Cardon G, Van Cauwenberghe E, De Bourdeaudhuij I. What do we know about physical activity in infants and toddlers: a review of the literature and future research directions. Sci Sports. 2011;26(3):127–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Hamzelou J. Sleep’s role seems to change in toddlers. New Sci. 2020;247(3301):19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Williams KE, Berthelsen D, Walker S, Nicholson JM. A Developmental Cascade Model of behavioral sleep problems and emotional and attentional self-regulation across early childhood. Behav Sleep Med. 2017;15(1):1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Hesketh KD, Downing KL, Galland BC, Nicholson JM, Taylor R, Orellana L, et al. Protocol for the Let’s Grow randomised controlled trial: examining efficacy, cost-effectiveness and scalability of a m-Health intervention for movement behaviours in toddlers. BMJ open. 2022;12(3):e057521.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Altenburg TM, Wang X, van Ekris E, Andersen LB, Møller NC, Wedderkopp N, Chinapaw MJM. The consequences of using different epoch lengths on the classification of accelerometer based sedentary behaviour and physical activity. PLoS ONE. 2021;16(7):e0254721.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. McClain JJ, Abraham TL, Brusseau TAJ, Tudor-Locke C. Epoch length and accelerometer outputs in children: comparison to Direct Observation. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(12).

  52. Trost SG, Fees BS, Murray AD, Haar SJ, Crowe LK. Identification and validity of accelerometer cut-points for toddlers. Obesity. 2012;20(11):2317–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Hnatiuk J, Ridgers ND, Salmon J, Campbell K, McCallum Z, Hesketh K. Physical activity levels and patterns of 19-month-old children. 2012.

  54. Hinkley T, O’Connell E, Okely AD, Crawford D, Hesketh K, Salmon J. Assessing volume of accelerometry data for reliability in preschool children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2012;44(12):2436–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Sneddon P, Peacock GG, Crowley SL. Assessment of sleep problems in preschool aged children: an adaptation of the children’s sleep habits questionnaire. Behav Sleep Med. 2013;11(4):283–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire -Revised. : Pediatric Sleep Council; [14.07.2021]. Available from: https://www.babysleep.com/bisq/.

  57. Owens JA, Spirito A, McGuinn M. The children’s Sleep habits Questionnaire (CSHQ: psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep (New York NY). 2000;23(8):1043–51.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Group CPPR. A developmental and clinical model for the prevention of conduct disorder: the FAST track program. Dev Psychopathol. 1992;4(4):509–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Checklist FCB. (Child Behavior Checklist, 2022) [Available from: [online] Available at: <https://fasttrackproject.org/techrept/c/cbc/index.php.

  60. Healey DM, Halperin JM. Enhancing Neurobehavioral gains with the aid of games and Exercise (ENGAGE): initial open trial of a novel early intervention fostering the development of preschoolers’ self-regulation. Child Neuropsychol. 2015;21(4):465–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Vazou S, Mantis C, Luze G, Krogh JS. Self-perceptions and social–emotional classroom engagement following structured physical activity among preschoolers: a feasibility study. J Sport Health Sci. 2017;6(2):241–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Cliff DP, McNeill J, Vella SA, Howard SJ, Santos R, Batterham M, et al. Adherence to 24-Hour Movement guidelines for the early years and associations with social-cognitive development among Australian preschool children. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(Suppl 5):857.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. López-Gil JF, Oriol-Granado X, Izquierdo M, Ramírez-Vélez R, Fernández-Vergara O, Olloquequi J, García-Hermoso A. Healthy lifestyle behaviors and their association with self-regulation in Chilean children. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(16):5676.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Baum KT, Desai A, Field J, Miller LE, Rausch J, Beebe DW. Sleep restriction worsens mood and emotion regulation in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(2):180–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Palmer CA, Oosterhoff B, Bower JL, Kaplow JB, Alfano CA. Associations among adolescent sleep problems, emotion regulation, and affective disorders: findings from a nationally representative sample. J Psychiatr Res. 2018;96:1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Gruber R, Cassoff J. The interplay between sleep and emotion regulation: conceptual Framework empirical evidence and future directions. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2014;16(11).

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Let’s Grow is supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council (GNT 1162980). AD is supported by a Deakin University Post-Graduate Scholarship. KLD was supported by an Alfred Deakin Postdoctoral Research Fellowship during the data collection of this study. ES is supported by a Medical Research Future Fund, Investigator Grant (1194297). KDH is supported by a Heart Foundation Future Leader Fellowship (105929).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AD was responsible for the analysis and interpretation of the data. AD was also responsible for the writing of this manuscript. All co-authors (KD, ES and KH) contributed to the concept of the study, interpretation of the findings and provided expert input and support overall for the writing of this manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Abhigale F. L. D’Cruz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study utilises data from the Let’s Grow study, which was approved by the Deakin University Human Research Ethics Committee (2020-077). The collection of all data was carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided informed consent prior to participating in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

D’Cruz, A.F.L., Downing, K.L., Sciberras, E. et al. Are physical activity and sleep associated with emotional self-regulation in toddlers? a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 24, 61 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-17588-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-17588-2

Keywords