Skip to main content

Appealing dish names to nudge diners to more sustainable food choices: a quasi-experimental study

Abstract

Background

Promoting plant-rich diets, i.e., diets with significantly reduced amounts of animal products, including vegan and vegetarian, is a promising strategy to help address the dual environmental and health crises that we currently face. Appealing dish names could boost interest in plant-rich dishes by attracting diners’ attention to them. In this study, a systematic approach to naming plant-rich dishes with appealing descriptors was tested with a quasi-experimental design in four workplace, self-service, buffet-style cafeterias in Chicago, Sydney, São Paulo and Singapore.

Methods

Three different plant-rich dishes were tested at each site. Appealing names were generated systematically through a workshop and emphasized the dish ingredients, origin, flavor and/or the eating experience. Each test dish appeared once in a four-week menu cycle where menu options changed on a daily basis. The cycle was then repeated four times (six times in Chicago) with the total number of showings for each dish to be four (six in Chicago). The dish names alternated between basic and appealing across dish repetitions. For each dish, the food taken per plate was estimated by weighing the overall food taken and dividing it by the plate count in the cafeteria. Data was analysed as percentage change from baseline (i.e., the first showing of each dish that always had a basic name) with linear mixed effects analysis using the lme4 package in R.

Results

Overall, appealing dish names significantly increased the amount of food taken per plate by 43.9% relative to baseline compared to basic dish names (54.5% vs. 10.6% increase for appealing vs. basic names, respectively, p = .002). This increase corresponded to a 7% increase in actual grams of food taken per plate. Secondary analysis showed that the effect was site-specific to English-speaking countries only and that there was no substitution effect between plant-rich and meat dishes.

Conclusions

The study tested an approach to creating appealing dish names in a systematic way and indicates that, in some settings, appealing dish titles are a relatively easy, scalable, cost-effective strategy that the food services sector can adopt to shift food choices towards more plant-rich, sustainable ones.

Peer Review reports

Background

The dual threats of climate change and the rising prevalence of non-communicable diseases stand to diminish the quality of life and well-being of future generations [1,2,3]. Tackling these issues requires immediate action on a global scale. A critical component of reducing both climate change and non-communicable diseases is shifting our food system to be more plant-rich (i.e., towards diets with significantly reduced amounts of animal products, including vegan and vegetarian) and environmentally sustainable [4, 5]. In this context, the general population of countries or cultures that follow red meat-heavy diets need to shift to more plant-rich choices, as red meat production is a primary contributor to greenhouse gas emissions and diets high in processed meat are often related to adverse health outcomes [6, 7]. However, many of the proposed solutions to encourage changes in dietary choices are costly or politically difficult to implement (e.g., taxes on certain foods) or rely on effortful self-control on behalf of consumers which can be challenging to maintain [8, 9].

Appealing language, that is, the use of descriptors that evoke positive attitudes and grab attention, is a marketing tool proven to influence consumers’ choices. Research shows that words and language can influence many priority behaviors in the field of public health: from vaccination to smoking cessation [10, 11]. Notably, however, plant-rich, healthy dishes are often described in restaurants or social media using less appealing language than their meat-rich counterparts [12, 13]. This may lead them to be perceived as less flavorful or indulgent, thereby deterring consumers from selecting these options [12,13,14].

Past work has provided initial evidence that renaming plant-rich menu items with more appealing descriptors emphasizing tasty and enjoyable attributes can increase selection of these dishes [15]. In one study, for instance, researchers in the US found that, whilst keeping the same recipe, renaming “carrots” to “twisted citrus-glazed carrots” led to a 25% increase in people selecting carrots [16]. Most of the research on this topic to date has been conducted in the US and the UK. It is unclear, though, if this approach is effective in other global locations. Anecdotal evidence from the World Resources Institute partners shows that appealing names work differently in Asian countries due to large differences in diet structure and norms around naming of food and this needs to be further confirmed. In addition, impeding the scalability of this strategy is the lack of a validated, systematic approach for food service operators to use to transform plant-rich names to more appealing ones.

In the present study, we developed a turnkey strategy that food service operators could use to create appealing dish names in a systematic manner, and assessed its efficacy in shifting consumers’ food choices in a field setting across different global locations and cultures. We hypothesized that renaming plant-rich dishes with more appealing descriptors generated through this systematic approach would a) primarily increase the amount of plant-rich food taken compared to basic names and b) secondarily reduce the amount of meat taken. In addition, we hypothesized that the effect would be c) location-specific and less pronounced in the Asian site as well as d) dish-specific and more pronounced for soups that were offered in opaque containers and they had their visual and olfactory cues removed.

Methods

Naming structure development

Before the intervention, we created a systematic naming approach to be delivered through a workshop based on past work published by the World Resources Institute [17]. Specifically, this work has shown that attributes which highlight provenance, spotlight flavor and emphasize a food’s look and feel are the most effective attributes to make the dish name more interesting and increase the likelihood for someone to select it [17]. The workshop consisted of five exercises that emphasized the dish’s analogy to other popular dishes; the dish ingredients or source of ingredients; the cultural and regional origin or preparation method of the dish; the difference from other dishes by emphasizing the dish’s most exciting characteristic; and its sensory appeal/eating experience (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic Representation of the Workshop

Then the research team conducted the workshop at each one of the four study sites (Chicago, Sydney, São Paulo, Singapore) with the food service operators. This workshop generated the appealing and basic names for the test dishes. Appealing names were created only for plant-rich dishes (not for the control dishes or the meat dishes that had only basic names - descriptions for the control and meat dishes to be found at the “Timeframe and test dishes” section below -). Before the workshop, Chefs responsible for creating the cafeteria menus, filled out a form with the test dishes details. These were made available to the workshop attendees during the workshop. The details provided for each test dish included the original dish title, a photo of the dish, the ingredients of the dish, the cooking instructions and the story of the dish, if any. For each workshop exercise, the food service attendees were asked to generate for each dish as many words/phrases as possible within 3 minutes using a post-it note per word/phrase. At the end, all post-it notes for each dish were placed on an empty wall and the attendees had to choose which words/phrases best describes the dish and compile three different names. In that way, three appealing names were generated for each plant-rich dish. These names were then ranked by the research team based on criteria on how well they reflected the workshop exercises and goals: a) how well someone could understand the generated name; b) if the dish name sounded to include lots of different components; c) if it sounded like it is good for health, of high quality; d) if someone could tell the regional or cultural origin; and e) if it was associated with a particular occasion. The name with the greatest score for each dish was then selected as the final test name (Table 1). Basic names were created in the same way with the appealing names asking attendees to generate as many basic names as they could for each test dish (plant-rich and meat-rich) within 3 minutes using post-its. At the end, the workshop attendees placed all post-its for each name on the wall and chose the name that they believed best described the dish.

Table 1 Plant-rich test dishes along with their basic and appealing names

Study sites, cafeterias and population

The study had a quasi-experimental design. It was conducted in four workplace offices in different parts of the world (Sydney, Chicago, Singapore & São Paulo) with onsite self-service, buffet-style cafeterias. All food in these cafeterias (i.e., all meals, beverages, and snacks) was provided complimentary to employees. The criteria for the selected locations were 1) to represent different continents and cultures; 2) to have at least two cafeterias on campus so one could be used for the intervention and the other for the control measurements; 3) to display the menus in English to ensure the consistency of the naming workshop; and 4) to serve a variety of plant-rich and meat dishes so that diners had adequate choices. The seated headcount at the test locations was ~ 850–2500 employees. About 50–60% were females, the mean employee age range was 28–40 and the job functions included sales, engineering, business and support services (mostly finance, operations and human resources).

Timeframe and test dishes

The testing was conducted during lunch time at all sites from September to December 2019 (July to December in Chicago). Three plant-rich (vegan or vegetarian) menu items including main dishes, side dishes, composed salads and/or soups were tested at each site (Table 1). The dishes that were selected were expected to have medium popularity (based on past menu items) and had the potential for good appealing names to be created. The effect size that determined our sample size i.e., the number of total plant-rich dish showings to be tested based on four dish repetitions, was calculated based on a previous hypothetical online study conducted solely for this purpose (data not shown). Meat-based versions of the plant-rich test dishes were also tested to assess potential substitution effects (Table 2). Intervention cafeterias offered the test dishes once over a four-week menu cycle. During each menu cycle the menu choices changed daily and each cycle repeated four times a year (six times in Chicago only). This determined the total number of repetitions for each dish to be four (six in Chicago), meaning the plant-rich dishes were shown with their basic names two times (three in Chicago) and with their appealing names another two (three in Chicago). No changes to the dish preparation were made across dish repetitions and the only thing that changed during the study period was the dish title. The test dishes were new to the participants when first introduced.

Table 2 Meat-based test dishes along with their plant-rich dish pairs

Soups were tested both as test and control dishes to control for any seasonality or weather effect on dish choice. Control soups were offered at a different cafeteria in the same building to the intervention cafeteria. The cafeterias were on different floors in the building (except in São Paulo). All employees could access both cafeterias without restriction. The control soups were identical with the test soups at each location (except in São Paulo), appeared only with their basic name and were offered on the same day with the test soups. In São Paulo two different plant-rich soups were offered as the control cafeteria was on the same floor as the intervention cafeteria (intervention soup: collard greens vegetable soup; control soup: cauliflower cream with coconut).

Intervention

During the intervention, each menu item appeared four times in total (six in Chicago as we had the opportunity at this site to test for a longer period of time) across repeated menu cycles as described above. The only exception was the barley risotto with spinach, artichokes and sundried tomatoes in Sydney that was presented twice within each menu cycle instead of once and eight times in total. Plant-rich main dishes and soups, along with their paired meat-based dish (and control dish for soups), were tested on the same day. All dishes were presented on the menu with their basic name the very first time (baseline) and then the plant-rich test dishes were presented alternating between basic and appealing names in a random order. For example, at one location the naming of the test dishes could have been basic/appealing/basic/appealing while at another site it may have been basic/basic/appealing/appealing. This was done to remove any naming order or time/date effects from the effect of the name. In all cases, all dishes were presented with their basic names as many times as with their appealing names. The meat-based dishes and control soups were presented with their basic names for all dish repetitions on the menu. Food service operators were specifically asked to not talk the test dishes up or down to the cafeteria diners.

Measurements

For each test dish, the overall amount of food produced as well as any leftovers in the kitchen or the counters was weighed at the end of the service. From their difference the amount of food taken by the diners was calculated. In addition, the order at which the test dish appeared as well as the number of other items at the buffet line was recorded. The number of clean plates each cafeteria had available for diners to use during service and the number of clean plates left over at the end of the service were counted. From their difference the total number of plates taken by the diners was calculated. This was a proxy measure for how many diners served themselves from the cafeteria. The amount of food taken per plate was calculated by dividing the amount taken for each test dish by the number of total plates taken in the cafeteria. This measure was used in the analysis to account for differences in attendance at the cafeteria at each test day.

Statistical analysis

To test the main study hypothesis, i.e., that selection of plant-rich dishes (food amount taken per plate) at the intervention cafeteria would increase when appealing names were used compared to basic names, we opted a priori to pool data across all four sites instead of analysing each location individually. This approach allowed us to maximize statistical power given that the study design was constant across all four sites. To account for the clustered nature of these data, we employed a linear mixed effects model using the lme4 package in R, with random-effects terms for dish and location. In order to reduce the noise of the data, the results were analyzed as percentage change from baseline i.e., the aggregated data for all basic (or appealing) names for each dish were analyzed as percentage change from their very first presentation that had a basic name. The results remained consistent when analyzing the amount of food per plate as grams/plate (data for grams/plate could be seen in Table 3). We further repeated the analysis using different covariates: month of year, order of presentation, number of other items in the buffet line, and day of the week. None of these covariates reduced the effect to non-significant. All tests were two-sided.

A number of exploratory analyses were also conducted using mixed effects models with random effects terms for dish and/or location. First, we investigated our secondary hypothesis whether the selection (food amount taken per plate) of meat-based dishes was decreased when paired with plant-rich dishes with appealing names (with random effects terms for dish and location). Second, using a random effects term for dish, we tested for each site separately whether there were differences in the selection of plant-rich dishes (food amount taken per plate) between appealing and basic names. Third, we tested whether the effect was more prominent for specific dish types and, most importantly, for soups, which were the only options to be offered in opaque containers with closed lids, thus removing the influence of any visual and olfactory food cues on dish choice (with random effects term for location). Data was not analyzed separately for salads as there were only four showings of them. In addition, we tested whether the effect was stronger or weaker depending on the popularity of the dish at baseline. Finally, we tested whether food consumption at the control cafeterias significantly changed compared to baseline via a mixed effects model with a random effects term for dish (as there was only one control dish per site, we did not use a random effects term for location). All tests were two-sided.

Results

Overall, appealing dish names significantly increased the amount of food taken per plate by 43.9% relative to baseline where basic dish names were presented (54.5% vs. 10.6% increase for appealing vs. basic names, respectively, b = 39.32, SE = 12.20, t(45.69) = 3.22, p = .002; Fig. 2). In real terms, this corresponded to a 7% increase in food amount taken (27.6 g per plate vs. 25.8 g per plate for appealing names vs. basic names, respectively, b = 2.88, SE = 1.65, t(44.91) = 1.75, p = .087; for all results in actual grams see Table 3). The results remained significant when controlling for the number of other items served alongside the intervention dish, the relative order of the intervention dish (i.e., first, second, last, etc.), the day of the week and the month of the year.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Percentage change of the food amount taken per plate relative to baseline for the basic (n = 28) and appealing (n = 30) names of plant-rich dishes. The food amount taken was estimated by dividing the total amount of food taken for each dish by the total plate count from that lunch period. The food amount taken was then normalized to the first presentation of that dish (which always had a basic name)

Table 3 Amount of food taken per dish and percentage change for appealing and basic names

No evidence was found from secondary analysis to support a decrease in meat selection when plant-rich dishes were presented with appealing names compared to basic names. On the contrary, meat selection expressed as percentage change from baseline was slightly increased when plant-rich dishes had appealing names (20.3% increase vs. -12.8% decrease for appealing vs. basic plant-rich dish names, respectively, b = 25.30, SE = 12.32, t(35.43) = 2.05, p = .048). However, this difference disappeared when analyzing the grams taken per plate (42.1 g of meat/plate vs. 37.8 g of meat/plate for appealing vs. basic plant-rich dish names, respectively, b = 3.30, SE = 3.02, t(34.96) = 1.09, p = .283). In other words, this result was not consistent between the different measures of consumption (when analyzing percentage change the effect was barely significant, but when analyzing the grams taken the effect was not significant) and shall be interpreted with caution. The results did not change when covariates were included in the analysis (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Percentage change of the food amount taken per plate relative to baseline for meat dishes when the corresponding plant-rich dish names were either basic (n = 22) or appealing (n = 24). The food amount taken was estimated by dividing the total amount of food taken for each dish by the total plate count from that lunch period. The food amount taken was then normalized to the first presentation of that dish. Meat dishes were always presented with basic names

Secondary analysis revealed that the effect was site specific (Fig. 4). Specifically, the effect was statistically significant in the two sites where English was the native language for the majority of employees i.e., Chicago and Sydney. Appealing names increased the amount of food taken per plate relative to baseline by 74.3% in Chicago (131.5% vs. 57.2% increase for appealing vs. basic names, respectively, b = 93.34, SE = 28.52, t(14.18) = 2.27, p = .005) and by 19.5% in Sydney (39.2% vs. 19.7% increase for appealing vs. basic names, respectively, b = 26.92, SE = 11.63, t(11.96) = 2.31, p = .039). The effect was not significant in São Paulo (9.1% increase vs. -16.2% decrease for appealing vs. basic names, respectively, b = 17.19, SE = 28.43, t(8) = .61, p = .562) or in Singapore (− 8.2% decrease vs. -12.1% decrease for appealing vs. basic names, respectively, b = − 2.18, SE = 8.27, t(8) = −.26, p = .799). The results did not change when covariates were included in the analyses.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Percentage change of the food amount taken per plate relative to baseline for the basic and appealing names of plant-rich dishes by study location. Sample sizes: Chicago n = 18 (8 basic names, 10 appealing names), São Paulo n = 12 (6 basic names, 6 appealing names), Singapore n = 12 (6 basic names, 6 appealing names), Sydney n = 16 (8 basic names, 8 appealing names). The food amount taken was estimated by dividing the total amount of food taken for each dish by the total plate count from that lunch period. The food amount taken was then normalized to the first presentation of that dish (which always had a basic name)

A significant effect of appealing names was found for main dishes (b = 39.14, SE = 17.86, t(21.97) = 2.19, p = .039) and soups (b = 61.04, SE = 26.02, t(13.30) = 2.35, p = .035), resulting in significantly greater dish selection compared to the basic names (Fig. 5). Only four observations were available for salad dishes, therefore we did not run statistical tests on these dishes alone (although we noted the pattern was directionally consistent with our hypothesized effect). No significant effect was found between the appealing and basic names for side dishes (b = − 12.54, SE = 11.73, t(5) = − 1.07, p = .334; Fig. 5). It is, however, important to note that side dishes were only tested in São Paulo and Singapore, the two sites that showed no overall effect of the intervention. It should also be noted that this analysis did not have sufficient statistical power, suggesting the results shall be interpreted with caution.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Percentage change of the food amount taken per plate relative to baseline for the basic and appealing names of plant-rich dishes by dish type. Sample sizes: Main n = 28 (n = 14 for basic names, n = 14 for appealing names), Salad n = 4 (n = 2 for basic names, n = 2 for appealing names), Side n = 8 (n = 4 for basic names, n = 4 for appealing names), Soup n = 18 (n = 8 for basic names, n = 10 for appealing names). The food amount taken was estimated by dividing the total amount of food taken for each dish by the total plate count from that lunch period. The food amount taken was then normalized to the first presentation of that dish (which always had a basic name). Statistical tests were omitted for salads due to low sample size

In addition, we found that the effect of appealing names compared to basic names was stronger for dishes that were less popular at baseline i.e., dishes at -1SD on volume taken at baseline (b = − 1873.25, SE = 575.45, t(44.75) = − 3.26, p = .002.). Finally, there was no significant change in selection of control dishes over time (b = − 63.16, SE = 62.33, t(20.27) = − 1.01, p = .323).

Discussion

In this real-world study, we tested a naming approach to promote plant-rich dishes using appealing descriptions. Overall, our results showed that appealing dish names increased the amount of food of plant-rich dishes taken by diners at two of the four global workplace sites. In order to understand the applicability of this strategy to geographies and cultures beyond the US and the UK, we examined the effect of appealing dish names in different global locations including Asia, Pacific and Latin America. The effect appeared significant only at sites where English was the native language. In addition, when we tested for a substitution effect between plant-rich and meat dishes we did not find any; meaning people were not selecting a plant-rich dish in the place of a meat dish. While the intervention effect was modest, scaling the strategy across food service companies could have a tremendous impact on shifting diets towards more plant-rich choices as a 7% increase could correspond to many thousands of meals if applied broadly.

The findings agree with previously published research showing that appealing names can increase the selection or sale of plant-rich dishes. Specifically, naming the vegetable dishes served at US university cafeterias with indulgent or taste-focused descriptors led to significantly more people choosing the vegetable dish compared to basic or health-related descriptors [15, 16]. In another experiment conducted in the UK’s second largest grocery store chain onsite cafeterias, descriptors emphasizing the dish taste, origin and those making the dish sound more appealing significantly increased the sales of plant-rich dishes compared to basic names [18]. On the other hand, naming dishes as vegetarian has been shown to lead to decreased selection [19]. Although direct comparisons of the magnitude of the effects are difficult to make between studies due to heterogeneity in design and measurement, taken together these findings indicate that using appealing dish names at the point of decision show some promise as a means to promote more plant-rich food choices in food service.

Potential mechanisms that could explain why appealing names influence selection can be identified from existing research on advertising and mental simulation. For instance, appealing menu names may alter taste perceptions [20], trigger tempting mental imagery [21], trigger positive mental simulations of eating and evoke positive expectations (e.g., due to vivid recall and imagining of eating the food, including the taste and texture of the food, eating situations and hedonic enjoyment [22, 23]).

Although appealing descriptors can attract consumers to the dish, repeat selection of plant-rich dishes depends on whether or not dish expectations created by the appealing names are met after tasting. Moderation analysis has shown that tastier vegetable recipes drive stronger effects [15]. However, the type of expectation generated by appealing names was not examined in the current study and warrants further investigation. Nevertheless, making plant-rich dishes delicious in combination with appealing dish names holds promise as the recipe for success. In addition, renewing appealing names systematically could also keep diners interested in them as a diminishing effect might be observed with repeated name showings. Again, the validity of this hypothesis and identifying the optimal frequency for changing the names require further investigation.

A secondary finding from the present study was that the effect of appealing language was observed only at sites where English was the native language. This finding may reflect the close relationship between language and culture [24]. Past work has found that linguistic and cultural differences are important factors that affect the way people respond in multi-country interventions [25]. For example, a major driver for food choices in western countries is taste. Consumer research shows that taste is the top driver of purchase decisions (88%), a finding that has not changed over the past decade [26]. However, this is not the case in all cultures. In China for example, far greater importance is attached to foods that evoke a sense of honor and prestige [27]. In addition, a study [28] comparing motives for food choice in different Asian and Pacific countries showed much heterogeneity in food choice drivers. Particularly, the most important driver in Taiwanese and (ethnically Chinese) Malaysian consumers were health, natural content, weight control and convenience, in Japanese consumers was price and in New Zealand consumers was sensory appeal. In addition, the variable of ‘familiarity’, which was a key focus in our naming workshop, was rated as least important by all countries. This fact demonstrates that consumers from different cultures have different drivers when it comes to food choice [28]. In other words, because our naming approach for developing appealing names was tailored to western-centric cultures, its effectiveness did not seem to generalize well to other cultures. This suggests that when global initiatives and strategies are being designed, guidelines and tool attributes need to be adapted to different cultures before scaling across different geographies.

Another important aspect that may have muted the effectiveness of this approach is that people do not always read the names of dishes served from a buffet. Instead, they often rely on what they see and smell when choosing what to put on their plates. Visual cues have a strong effect on guiding food choices [29]. In the current study, the best way to increase internal validity and examine the isolated effect of appealing dish names was via soups. This was because visual and olfactory cues were removed as soups were offered in opaque pots with closed lids and people had to read the labels to understand the soup on offer. In the present study, in three out of the four locations (i.e., Chicago, Singapore and São Paulo), the increase in soup selection with the appealing names was larger than the increase in other types of dishes where consumers could see the dishes they were selecting. However, given that a significant effect was also observed for other types of dishes, no robust conclusions can be drawn. Nevertheless, this indicative evidence supports our hypothesis about appealing dish names attracting consumers to plant-rich dishes in the absence of other sensory cues.

No evidence was found that diners substitute meat dishes with plant-rich dishes even when the latter were presented with appealing names. Although appealing names increased the selection of plant-rich dishes, this increase was additive and not a replacement to the selection of meat dishes. This may increase the overall energy consumption from the meal and this is something that warrants further investigation.

Strength and limitations

Our study examined a systematic naming approach to improve the appeal of plant-rich dishes. To date, most of the existing research on this topic has been conducted in the US and the UK. For the first time, we examined how this approach performed across other geographies and cultures. In addition, most of the research into the role of appealing language on food choice has been conducted either online or in academic cafeterias or restaurant settings. This study examined the effect of appealing dish titles in a workplace dining environment. Workplaces are important settings to consider as people often spend a significant amount of time at work and regularly consume a meal there. Additional strengths of the study include the elimination of the influence of dish price, which is a major determinant of food selection as food was complimentary at the point of service in this study. Notably, it is important for future research to examine the interaction between appealing names and pricing as people might select in a different way when they have to pay. The repeated menu cycles also provided the opportunity to test the same dish across different time points, thus improving reliability of the findings, as well as studying dishes from different cuisines and different types i.e., mains, sides, salads, soups. Limitations of the study include the inability to implement a fully controlled experimental design given the applied nature of this research, although control dishes were used in every location. The quasi-experimental design provides low internal validity but, on the other hand, maximizes external validity. In addition, a test to verify whether the appealing names were considered appealing by the audience would be helpful to provide further confirmation for the efficacy of the workshop. Lastly, the sample size was not big enough for the secondary analysis performed, increasing the likelihood of type 2 error in our interpretations i.e., finding a false null result and no firm conclusions can be drawn. Conversely, power for the main analysis was adequate to detect significant results.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study indicates that a systematic approach to generating appealing names to promote plant-rich dishes in a real-life dining environment can be effective in influencing diners’ food choices. It also shows that the effectiveness of this strategy is influenced by the cultural and linguistic context and that adaptations must be made before applying the approach in different locations. Appealing dish names are a relatively easy, cost-effective, scalable strategy that food service providers could adopt to shift food choices. Applying this to workplace environments, as well as more broadly, could affect millions of meals on a daily basis and induce large scale shifts to more plant-rich dishes.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. NCD Countdown 2030 collaborators. NCD Countdown 2030: worldwide trends in non-communicable disease mortality and progress towards Sustainable Development Goal target 3.4. Lancet. 2018;392(10152):1072–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Garnett T. Plating up solutions. Science. 2016;353(6305):1202–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Prüss-Ustün A, van Deventer E, Mudu P, Campbell-Lendrum D, Vickers C, Ivanov I, et al. Environmental risks and non-communicable diseases. BMJ. 2019;364:l265.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. UN Sustainable Development Goals progress report. 2019. https://sustainabledevelopment.un.org/content/documents/24978Report_of_the_SG_on_SDG_Progress_2019.pdf.

  5. Willett W, Rockström J, Loken B, Springmann M, Lang T, Vermeulen S, et al. Food in the anthropocene: the EAT-lancet commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems. Lancet. 2019;393(10170):447–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Hemler EC, Hu FB. Plant-based diets for personal, population, and planetary health. Adv Nutr. 2019;10(Suppl 4):275–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Tilman D, Clark M. Global diets link environmental sustainability and human health. Nature. 2014;515(7528):518–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(2):1252–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Duckworth AL, Taxer JL, Eskreis-Winkler L, Galla BM, Gross JJ. Self-control and academic achievement. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70(1):373–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Maibach EW, Abroms LC, Marosits M. Communication and marketing as tools to cultivate the public's health: a proposed "people and places" framework. BMC Public Health. 2007;7(1):88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Ohlrogge AW, Suggs LS. Flu vaccination communication in Europe: what does the government communicate and how? Vaccine. 2018;36(44):6512–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Turnwald BP, Jurafsky D, Conner A, Crum AJ. Reading between the menu lines: are restaurants' descriptions of "healthy" foods unappealing? Health Psychol. 2017;36(11):1034–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Davis T, Papies EK. Pleasure vs. identity: more eating simulation language in meat posts than plant-based posts on social media #foodtalk. Appetite. 2022;175:106024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Raghunathan R, Walker Naylor R, Hoyer WD. The unhealthy = tasty intuition and its effects on taste inferences, enjoyment, and choice of food products. J Mark. 2006;70(4):170–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Turnwald BP, Bertoldo JD, Perry MA, Policastro P, Timmons M, Bosso C, et al. Increasing vegetable intake by emphasizing tasty and enjoyable attributes: a randomized controlled multisite intervention for taste-focused labeling. Psychol Sci. 2019;30(11):1603–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Turnwald BP, Boles DZ, Crum AJ. Association between indulgent descriptions and vegetable consumption: twisted carrots and dynamite beets. JAMA. Intern Med. 2017;177(8):1216–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Wise J, Vennard D. WRI commentary. 2019. https://www.wri.org/insights/its-all-name-how-boost-sales-plant-based-menu-items.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bacon L, Wise J, Attwood S, Vennard D. The language of sustainable diets: a field study exploring the impact of renaming vegetarian dishes on U.K. cafe menus (Technical note). Washington, D.C.: World Resources Institute; 2018. https://wriorg.s3.amazonaws.com/s3fs-public/language-sustainable-diets.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Krpan D, Houtsma N. To veg or not to veg? The impact of framing on vegetarian food choice. J Environ Psychol. 2020;67:101391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Elder R, Krishna A. The effects of advertising copy on sensory thoughts and perceived taste. J Consum Res. 2010;30(5):748–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Lockyer T. Would a restaurant menu item by any other name taste as sweet? Hos Rev. 2006;24(1):21–31.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Papies EK. Tempting food words activate eating simulations. Front Psychol. 2013;4:838.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Papies EK, Johannes N, Daneva T, Semyte G, Kauhanen L. Using consumption and reward simulations to increase the appeal of plant-based foods. Appetite. 2020;155:104812.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Thompson B, Roberts SG, Lupyan G. Cultural influences on word meanings revealed through large-scale semantic alignment. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4(10):1029–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kumar R, Chambers E 4th. Understanding the terminology for snack foods and their texture by consumers in four languages: a qualitative study. Foods. 2019;8(10):484.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. IFIC. Food and health survey. 2020. https://foodinsight.org/2020-food-and-health-survey/. Accessed 10 Nov 2021.

  27. China Internet Information Center. Chinese imperial cuisines. How Chinese dishes were named. http://www.china.org.cn/english/MATERIAL/26147.htm.

  28. Prescott J, Young O, O'Neill LO, Yau N, Stevens R. Motives for food choice: a comparison of consumers from Japan, Taiwan, Malaysia and New Zealand. Food Qual Pref. 2002;13(7-8):489–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. McCrickerd K, Forde CG. Sensory influences on food intake control: moving beyond palatability. Obes Rev. 2016;17(1):18–29.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the food service operators that helped with the study conduction and data collection.

Funding

This work was supported by the Food at Google Program. CH, SG and MB had a role in the conceptualization of the study, critically revised and approved the manuscript for submission.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AG contributed in the design of the work; the acquisition, analysis plan, and interpretation of data and have drafted the work; SA contributed in the conceptualization and design of the work; the interpretation of data and have substantively revised the work; JW contributed in the conceptualization and design of the work; and have substantively revised the work; EPF contributed in the conceptualization and design of the work; the interpretation of data and have substantively revised the work; PS contributed in the design of the work; the analysis plan, data analysis and interpretation of data and have drafted part of the work; SG contributed in the conceptualization of the work and have substantively revised the work; JU contributed in the conceptualization and design of the work; the interpretation of data and have substantively revised the work; CH contributed in the design of the work and have substantively revised the work; MB contributed in the conceptualization of the work and have substantively revised the work. All authors have read and approved the final article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna Gavrieli.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was reviewed by the World Resources Institute’s Human Subjects Protection Program (#WRI202100001). As the study did not include any human participants but only food and plate measurements on a cafeteria kitchen- and dining hall- level, the study was determined as “not human subjects research” and therefore did not require IRB review or oversight, according to US regulations summarized in the Revised Common Rule at 45 CFR 46.101(a). In addition, the project did not require informed consent to be obtained, as the US Regulations on informed consent apply only to non-exempt human subjects research, according to the Revised Common Rule at 45 CFR 46.116(a)(1). The WRI process is in accordance with this guidance and all methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

SA and JW contributed to this work while employed at the World Resources Institute, an environmental research organization. Their work at WRI focuses on researching and promoting plant based diets for sustainability benefits. The rest of the authors have no financial or non-financial competing interests to declare.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gavrieli, A., Attwood, S., Wise, J. et al. Appealing dish names to nudge diners to more sustainable food choices: a quasi-experimental study. BMC Public Health 22, 2229 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-14683-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-14683-8

Keywords