Skip to main content

Effects of food nutrition labels on the health awareness of school-age children

Abstract

Background

Overweight and obesity have been described as a global epidemic that seriously affects the health of adults and children. Front of Package (FOP) Nutrition Labeling can increase consumers’ awareness of unhealthy foods. The purpose of this study is to find effective deterrence and improve children’s health awareness via the FOP.

Methods

This study examined children’s health awareness of snack packaging using the four labels: guideline daily amounts (GDA), traffic light system (TLS), Apple label (designed in this study), and Warning label. This study recruited 343 children in the sixth grade, including 223 children living in cities and 120 children living in rural areas. First, 30 children in grades 3 to 6 selected 8 snacks that they often buy. Then, each snack was synthesized into these four labels according to their nutritional content for a total of 32 samples. Finally, a questionnaire was used to evaluate the health of snack packaging and the visibility of nutrition labels.

Results

Four results can be drawn: (1) GDA, Apple label and TLS can help children determine healthier snack choices, (2) black Warning label cannot induce children to make healthier choices, (3) children who often buy snacks have low health awareness, and (4) rural children have weak health awareness of snack packaging.

Conclusions

These results can provide a packaging label design, which can effectively improve children’s health awareness.

Peer Review reports

Background

The overweight and obesity risks have been described as a global epidemic affecting adults and children in both developed and developing countries [1,2,3,4,5]. In particular, foods with high edible sugar, fat and salt have been considered as the most important food factors for promoting non-conductive diseases associated with weight gain, obesity and diet [6]. Front of Package (FOP) Nutrition Labeling can effectively encourage the food industry to re-develop their products and develop new and healthier foods [7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. However, most research on the effectiveness of the FOP focuses on adults [12, 14]. Nutrition labels have no significant effects on children’s choice of food [15, 16]. Most children’s foods contain high sugar, sodium and fat content, and these foods are typically sold through cartoon characters in ads and their packaging [17, 18]. Children’s products usually promote entertainment and health with bright colors, cartoon characters, cute patterns, nutrition promotion, natural food images (such as fruit pictures), and descriptions of physical activities (implying the power or intensity of product consumption) [19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28]. Children are extremely susceptible to these marketing strategies. In addition, food packaging (such as name, shape, color, flavor, and characters) generally regarded by children as “interesting” is more praised than the taste of “uninteresting” food [28, 29]. In other words, when children’s favorite cartoon patterns are printed on the packaging, or the color of the packaging attracts their attention, these may cause children to disregard the health results of the food. In addition, there is evidence that urban-rural gap factors can also affect children’s health [30,31,32,33,34,35]. Increasing the educational level of women in rural areas may have a greater impact on reducing child malnutrition [34, 36, 37]. Therefore, family environment and education seem to potentially affect children’s health awareness.

Front of Package (FOP) Nutrition Labeling improves consumers’ ability to correctly identify healthy products and encourage people to choose more healthy foods [38,39,40,41,42,43]. However, children may not be familiar with FOP. Many studies have investigated the effectiveness of the FOP [44,45,46,47,48]; there is evidence that the possible explanation for the lack of health perceptions of most children with FOP is that their evaluations are based on previous preconceived perceptions of product and label design, rather than relying on nutritional information [34, 49] point out that familiarity with the labeling system may affect consumers’ willingness to use these products. In short, consumers will not choose unfamiliar nutrition labels; this is especially important for children because they rarely use nutritional information to guide their food choices [50]. The most common forms of FOP include guideline daily amounts (GDA), traffic light system (TLS), and Warning labels. GDA uses symbols to display the number of specific nutrients or calories per serving, providing a semi-directive assessment of nutritional quality. Based on GDA, TLS adds traffic light colors (red, yellow, and green) to indicate the level of nutrients in food [12]. A significant amount of evidence shows that TLS seems to be more acceptable than GDA to consumers; this may be due to the color form enabling consumers to identify and use the information easily and more quickly [51]. These colorful labels can help avoid the consumption of unhealthy foods [52, 53]. In addition, black Warning labels [34, 40, 54, 55] indicate that the content of key nutrients (such as salt, sugar, and saturated fat) is higher than the standard, which can warn consumers that these foods contain a large amount of ingredients that cause obesity, thereby helping to prevent consumers from buying unhealthy products [40, 56, 57]. On the contrary, the way TLS classifies low/medium/high nutrient content conveys contradictory information, that is, the product may contain a high content of one nutrient and low content of other nutrients [58]. Food packaging with low nutrient content information (the label is green) can easily mislead people into thinking that these products are healthy [59,60,61,62,63,64].

There are still many controversies about the promotional effects of the nutritional content noted on these food labels. This study questions whether these labels are effective in deterring children from buying unhealthy foods. In addition, children are easily tempted by food marketing as attractive label patterns (such as fruits or cartoons) may easily attract children’s attention. Therefore, this study compares the health perception and impact of snack packaging containing four FOP forms on children: GDA, Apple label (designed in this study), TLS, and Warning label. These results may provide an FOP improvement plan and a new marketing strategy for children’s food.

Methods

Participants

Three hundred forty-three sixth-grade Taiwanese children (158 males, 185 females, M = 11.49 years old, SD = 0.50 years old) (Table 1) were recruited to conduct health assessments based on different packaging labels. Among them, 223 children live in cities (108 males and 115 females, M = 11.48 years old, SD = 0.50 years old) and come from Tainan City’s Dawan Elementary School. In addition, 120 children live in rural areas (50 males and 70 females, M = 11.52 years old, SD = 0.50 years old) and come from Puli Elementary School in Nantou County.

Table 1 Participant information, including: gender, age, and frequency of snack consumption in urban and rural areas

Material

Label settings

Figure 1 shows four kinds of nutrition labels: GDA, Apple label, TLS, and Warning label. All the labels were located on the left or right under the front of the package. The GDA, Apple label, and TLS display the content of each serving and the percentage of the recommended daily intake, while the Warning labels only display warning text for low, medium, and high nutrition. The Apple label designed by this study is based on the appearance of the TLS label changed to a fruit pattern.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Nutrition labels on the front of the package: a GDA: Colored in black and white and showing the number of specific nutrients or calories per serving, b Apple label (designed in this study): Combining GDA and TLS features plus apple-patterned outlines; c TLS: Based on GDA, TLS adds traffic light colors (red, yellow, and green) to indicate nutrient levels, and d Warning label: Black octagon with nutrients showing exceeding standard contents

Packages

Thirty children in grades 3 to 6 (9 males, 21 females, M = 11.47 years old, SD = 1.01 years old) chose eight kinds of snacks that are most popular with Taiwanese children (see the Additional file 1): Kola Nuts, Guaiguai (5 flavors), Fruit Jelly (Guaiguai soft candies), Lay’s Potato Chips (original flavor), Scientific Noodles, Jinsha Chocolate, Cheetos (cheese), and Pocky (chocolate).

Figure 2 shows examples of the positions and sizes of the four labels displayed on the packaging. Each package uses a commercially available original image, and synthesizes four nutrition labels on the front package. All the labels ware located at the bottom of the front packaging, and their exact location (left or right) depends on the original packaging design. All the packaging images were presented in the same size.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Snack packaging contains these four examples of labels on the bottom left or right: a “Kola Nut” with GDA label; b “Lay’s Potato Chips” with Apple label; c “Kuai Kuai” with TLS label, and d “Science Noodles” with Warning label

Questionnaire

The content of the questionnaire was divided into three parts: (1) personal information, (2) packaging health evaluation, and (3) label visibility evaluation. The evaluation scale uses a 7-point Likert scale (1 = completely unhealthy, 7 = completely healthy). In order to avoid the psychological burden of children caused by the large number of pictures, these samples were evaluated by 5 groups (see Table 2). Each group judged 8 pictures. Groups 1 to 4 evaluated the images containing four labels (GDA, Apple label, TLS, and Warning label) and the visibility of the four labels. The last group evaluated the original packaging without any label, and did not need to denote the visibility of the label. Samples between groups were not repeated.

Table 2 The content of the questionnaire includes personal information and evaluations of packaging healthiness and label visibility. The sample types and order of appearance were divided into five groups for healthiness and visibility assessment

Procedure

Before the experiment, children were asked to sit in front of a 19-in. screen. The researchers explained the contents of the experiment to the children. At the beginning of the experiment, each package picture was displayed in the center of the screen with a white background. The sample has 40 pictures in total. During the experiment, no information about packaging were provided to the children. All the children used computers to complete tasks under the supervision of the researcher. If children had questions, the researcher helped them.

Data analysis

First, the data was analyzed using univariate analysis. The “healthiness” of the package and the “visibility” of the label were dependent variables. The “nutrition label” (GDA, Apple label, TLS, Warning label, and No label (standard)), the “consumption frequency” of snacks (Type 1, Type 2, and Type 3), and the “residential area” (urban and rural) were the independent variables. If the ANOVA results significantly differed, post hoc tests were analyzed by Scheffe. Then, a paired sample T-test was used to compare potentially significant factors.

Finally, linear regression analysis was performed to predict children’s health awareness of the five labels on the packaging. The “consumption frequency”, “residential area”, and “gender” were set as dependent variables. The “healthiness” and “visibility” were the independent variables. The scatter plot examined the linear relationship between the response variables and the standardized residuals.

Results

ANOVA results

Health evaluation of snack packaging

Table 3 shows the overall ANOVA results; different nutrition labels, consumption frequency, and residential area affect children’s health awareness of snack packaging (F[4, 1586] = 4.77, p = 0.001; F[2, 1586] = 22.95, p = 0.000; F[1, 1586] = 47.34, p = 0.000, respectively). The following further examines the health awareness of these three factors on packaging through post hoc tests and T-test comparisons.

Table 3 The overall ANOVA results of different health awareness of packaging among the nutrition labels, consumption frequency, and residential area

Differences between the nutrition labels

The results of the post hoc test showed that GDA, Apple label, TLS and Warning labels can be grouped into one group, while Warning label and no label comprise another group. The comparison results of the T test (Fig. 3a) found that no label (score = 3.58), compared with GDA (score = 3.19), Apple label (score = 3.18) and TLS (score = 3.19) has significant differences (p = 0.003, p = 0.003, and p = 0.004, respectively). However, there is no significant difference between Warning label (score = 3.38) and no label packaging (p = 0.409). Because the main function of FOP is to prevent unhealthy snacks, the Warning label (GDA and TLS) has a lower score than the no label, indicating that these snacks have been successfully promoted as unhealthy. The health prevention of Warning label is worse than the other three labels, and has almost the same effect as no label.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean and T-test evaluation results of children’s health awareness of packaging and visibility of labels on the nutrition labels, consumption frequency, and residential area

Differences of consumption frequency

According to the results of post hoc tests, the consumption Type 1 and Type 2 belong to one group, while the consumption Type 3 is another group. The comparison results of the T test (Fig. 3b) showed that children who often buy snacks (score = 3.72) have lower health awareness (all p < 0.000), compared to children who belong to consumption Type 1 and Type 2 (score = 3.06 and score = 3.24, respectively). This means that children who often buy snacks are less aware of the health implications of snacks, and other children agreed that these are unhealthy snacks.

Differences of residential area

Children living in urban areas (score = 3.07) have lower health ratings of snack packaging compared to rural children (score = 3.66) (p = 0.000) (Fig. 3c), indicating that urban children have a stronger health awareness of snack packaging.

Visibility of the label on the snack package

Table 4 shows the overall ANOVA results found that different nutrition labels, consumption frequency, and residential area affect the visibility of children’s snack packaging (F[31348] = 9.87, p = 0.001; F[21348] = 3.26, p = 0.039; F[11348] = 21.25, p = 0.000, respectively). The following further examines the visibility of these three factors on packaging through post hoc tests and T-test comparisons.

Table 4 The overall ANOVA results of different visibility of the label among the nutrition label, consumption frequency, and residential area

Differences between the nutrition labels

The results of the post hoc test revealed that the GDA, Apple, and TLS tags are classified into one group, and the Warning label is another group. The comparison results of the T test (Fig. 3d) showed that the Warning label (score = 3.82) significantly differs from the GDA (score = 4.23), Apple label (score = 4.35) and TLS (score = 4.53) (p = 0.01; p = 0.000 and p = 0.000, respectively).

Differences in consumption frequency

According to the results of post hoc tests, the consumption Type 1 and Type 2 belong to one group, while the consumption Type 3 is another group. The comparison results of the T test (Fig. 3e) revealed that children who meet the consumption Type 3 (score = 3.52) consider that nutrition labels have a higher degree of visibility (all p < 0.000), compared to children who belong to consumption Type 1 and Type 2 (score = 3.17 and score = 3.13, respectively). This means that children who often buy snacks do notice the existence of nutrition labels, albeit they still buy the snacks. It may be that they have significantly low health awareness.

Differences of residential area

Children living in urban areas (score = 4.06) are less likely to pay attention to nutrition labels on packaging i.e., these appear less visible compared to children in rural areas (score = 4.56) (p < 0.000) (Fig. 3f).

Regression results

Tables 5 and 6 display the results of the “healthiness” and “visibility” coefficients for GDA, Apple Label, TLS, and Warning label in the gender, consumption frequency, and residential area variables. Figures 4 and 5 show the scatter plots of these four labels, consumption frequency, and residential area between visibility and the standardized residuals, respectively. These scatter plots illustrate the linear relationships between the response variables and the standardized residuals.

Table 5 The regression results of “healthiness” coefficients for GDA, Apple label, TLS, and Warning label in the gender, consumption frequency, and residential area variables
Table 6 The regression results of “visibility” coefficients for GDA, Apple label, TLS, and Warning label in the gender, consumption frequency, and residential area variables
Fig. 4
figure 4

The scatter plots of “healthiness” for the four labels, consumption frequency, and residential area between healthiness and the standardized residuals

Fig. 5
figure 5

The scatter plots of “visibility” for the four labels, consumption frequency, and residential area between healthiness and the standardized residuals

Prediction of healthiness

All of the labels (p = 0.002, p = 0.000, p = 0.001, and p = 0.001, respectively) have strongly significant differences in consumption frequency. GDA, Apple Label, and TLS (p = 0.003, p = 0.002, and p = 0.001, respectively) showed significant differences in the residential area, but not the Warning label (p = 0.128). Nevertheless, there is no significant difference in gender. These regression results show that the consumption frequency is a better predictor of the healthiness of snack packaging than other factors, regardless of the nutrition label used in the packaging design. However, the gender factor did not help to predict the children’s health awareness for snack packaging.

Prediction of visibility

Both the Apple label and TLS exhibit significant differences in the consumption frequency and residential area (Apple label: p = 0.017 and p = 0.012; TLS: p = 0.036 and p = 0.010). However, GDA and Warning label do not have any significant factor. Also, there was no significant difference in gender. These results suggest that the higher color rendering of the Apple label and TLS makes them more predictable for package visibility compared to the other two labels.

Discussion

GDA, apple label, and TLS can effectively improve children’s health awareness

Front of Package (FOP) Nutrition Labeling was developed to enable consumers to choose healthy foods. This study proves that snack packaging with FOP is better than non-labeled snack packaging, as it can effectively improve children’s health awareness. This study unanimously agrees that past studies believe that indicating FOP on snack packaging can indeed change consumers’ health perception and their food choices [37, 39,40,41,42,43]. Because this visual cue highlights compliance with specific nutritional standards, it lets children know whether these products are healthy or unhealthy. This study cannot completely agree with previous studies that most children lack health awareness of FOP [34]. A possible explanation is that these sixth-grade participants are older. Children in this period might engage in logical thinking based on specific examples and have the ability to deal with consumption information [65]. Therefore, they could indeed perceive and process the nutritional information and health implications of these labels.

GDA, TLS and Apple label have the same effect of conveying nutritional information. Among them, the visibility of GDA is slightly lower than that of Apple label and TLS because GDA uses black and white labels. In contrast, the visual cue is not higher than the use of multi-color TLS and Apple labels. Packaging focuses on visual quality [28]. In the eyes of children, colorless GDA is regarded as “not interesting.” Because interesting packaging is usually better in regard to evaluation [28, 29]. In addition, the color information of TLS can be clearly conveyed to children. Because red can link warning and danger meanings to attract children’s attention, and effectively and quickly raise children’s health awareness [52, 53, 66,67,68].

Furthermore, snack packaging labels use fruit patterns to increase children’s attention and effectively convey the message that these products are unhealthy [19,20,21, 24]. Moreover, the health alertness of the Apple label can be effectively the same as that of GDA and TLS because the advantages of the Apple label’s pattern combine the color distinction of TLS and daily nutritional information of GDA. Graphic design is used by many businesses; they put cartoon or game characters on the packaging when selling snacks to increase children’s preference and brand attraction, leading children to buy unhealthy and high-calorie products [20]. Indeed, children are susceptible to these marketing strategies; therefore, the label design of traffic light colors and attractive shapes can effectively improve children’s health awareness.

The controversy that warning label has no effective health warning

The Warning label did not achieve effective prompting in this study, and its effect is equivalent to “no label.” It is further found that the Warning label is less visible than the other three labels; this result is contrary to previous studies, namely that the Warning label can effectively deter consumers from choosing unhealthy products [34; 40). The display of the black Warning label is completely opposite to that of the other three nutrition labels. The information presented inside the Warning label is simple and only highlights unhealthy components [57].

This study speculates on three reasons. The first reason is the black of high color rendering of the Warning label, which only reveals the high content of information, but does not display other data. There is still insufficient information to provide more detailed information about “health”, which prevents children from being aware of the meaning of the warning. The second reason is the fact that children have regarded snacks as unhealthy, making snacks have no effect on Warning labels, a view which is consistent with [69], who found that soda water is the only category that has no warning effect. Similar studies, such as [57] also found that Warning labels only affect half of the participants, and did not produce any relevant effects on the remaining participants because consumers may often ignore the information on the label of unhealthy products [70]. The last reason may be based on different cultural perceptions. Since Taiwanese snack labels rarely exhibit the Warning label design, many children do not recognize this label. In Chile, the Warning label has been implemented in people’s basic food [34, 54, 55]. As this label is familiar and recognized by local people, Warning labels may not be suitable for Taiwanese snacks.

Children who often buy snacks have low health awareness

Children who often buy snacks have significantly lower health awareness. This study speculates that the reason may be due to insufficient health education in the family or incorrect parenting methods, which leaves children with limited health knowledge [34, 36, 37]. In general, consumers’ food choices are usually affected by product health considerations [71]. When consumers are able to obtain sufficient health awareness resources, their food choices tend to favor healthy products, thereby reducing impulsive eating behavior [72,73,74]. In fact, children have fewer intellectual resources in many fields compared to adults [75, 76], and their understanding of product prices and the value of goods and services are not mature enough [77, 78]; therefore, with limited cognitive abilities and experience, children often lack the requisite knowledge for recognizing the health implications of food.

In addition, advertisements of unhealthy products seriously affect children’s correct healthy diet and nutritional concepts. Children are susceptible to external interference (for example, TV commercials, celebrities, cartoon characters, etc.). Food promotion directly affects children’s nutritional knowledge, preferences, purchasing behavior, consumption patterns, and diet [79]. In order to better sell snacks, many businesses use promotional activities to increase consumers’ willingness to buy. For example, TV commercials often use animation to effectively attract young children and enhance their perception of food pleasure [80]. When children see a favorite cartoon character or TV star advertising a certain snack, the children will not care whether or not the snack is healthy. Instead, these advertisements prompt them to associate snacks with pleasant things; this means that in the face of unhealthy food, consumers who prefer a certain orientation ignore the detailed nutritional information on the product [79, 81, 82]. Therefore, a healthy eating attitude is important to support the choice of healthy products. Consumers with health goals will understand the nutritional information before considering the product [83], which is the correct dietary concept.

Children living in rural areas have weak health awareness on snack packaging

This study confirms that the health awareness of children living in cities is indeed higher than that in rural areas. Past studies have shown that urban children are healthier than rural children [30,31,32,33,34,35]. While rural areas are usually not the direct cause of health literacy gaps, this gap still exists. Many of the children who grew up in the countryside were brought up by their elders who may be particularly ignorant of health awareness, so that rural children rarely have health concepts. In addition, income may also be one of the variables that affect the way children buy food. Because low-income people usually have less nutritional knowledge than middle- and high-income people, they face more obstacles in identifying and choosing healthy foods [35]. However, many factors are still involved in the weak health awareness of rural children. The factor of family education may be one of the potential impacts on rural areas [34, 36, 37]. Due to research limitations, this study did not investigate the educational background of children’s parents or explore their health knowledge; this may be a topic in future research.

Conclusions

Front of Package (FOP) Nutrition Labeling can identify unhealthy foods for children and increase their alertness to unhealthy foods. This study compares GDA, TLS, Apple label, and Warning label on the packaging on the front of snacks, and examines children’s health awareness. The results of the study include three conclusions: (1) rural children’s health awareness is weak on snack packaging; (2) children who often buy snacks have low health awareness; (3) GDA, TLS and Apple labels can help children determine healthier food choices.

Although this study was not conducted in a real retail environment, these snack products have always been favorites for children in Taiwan. The possible limitations of this study lie in three items: (1) This study cannot fully understand the family background, eating habits, parental rearing styles, and food types of each child. (2) Because, the factors that affect children’s unhealthy diet involve a very wide range, the use of labelled snack packaging composite images to allow children to make judgments may be subjective and may not fully represent the opinions of all children. Judging from the current results, it was possible to find the effectiveness of the designed Apple label for health reminders; and (3) This research result is limited to the snack packaging category and cannot be fully applied to other categories.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article [and its supplementary information files].

References

  1. De Onis M, Blössner M, Borghi E. Global prevalence and trends of overweight and obesity among preschool children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92:1257–64.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Gupta N, Goel K, Shah P, Misra A. Childhood obesity in developing countries: epidemiology, determinants, and prevention. Endocr Rev. 2012;33(1):48–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lobstein T, Baur L, Uauy R, IASO international obe- sity task force. Obesity in children and young people: a crisis in public health. Obes Rev. 2004;1:4–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Han JC, Lawlor DA, Kimm SY. Childhood obesity. Lancet. 2010;375(9727):1737–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Zobel EH, Hansen TW, Rossing P, Scholten BJV. Global changes in food supply and the obesity epidemic. Curr Obes Rep. 2016;5:449–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Cominato L, Di Biagio GF, Lellis D, Franco RR, Mancini MC, de Melo ME. Obesity prevention: strategies and challenges in Latin America. Curr Obes Rep. 2018;7(2):97–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Couste NL, Martos-Partal M, Martinez-Rios E. The power of a package. Product claims drive purchase decisions. J Advert Res. 2012;52:364–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Wirtz JG, Ahn R, Song R, Wang Z. Selling or selling out? A content analysis of children’s snack packages and implications for advertising practitioners and educators. The Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication: AEJMC Conference. Washington, DC; 2013.

  9. Vyth EL, Steenhuis IH, Roodenburg AJ, Brug J, Seidell JC. Front-of-pack nutrition label stimulates healthier product develop-ment: a quantitative analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Young L, Swinburn B. Impact of the pick the Tickfood information programme on the salt content of food inNew Zealand. Health Promot Int. 2002;17:13–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bonsmann SS, Celemin LF, Grunert KG. Food labelling to advance better education for life. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2010;64(3):S14–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hawley KL, Roberto CA, Bragg MA, Liu PJ, Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. The science on front-of-package food labels. Public Health Nutr. 2013;16(3):430–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Hodgkins C, Barnett J, Wasowicz-Kirylo G, Stysko-Kunkowska M, Gulcan Y, Kustepeli Y. Understanding how consumers categorise nutritional labels: a consumer derived typology for front-of-pack nutrition labelling. Appetite. 2012;59:806–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Graham DJ, Heidrick C, Hodgin K. Nutrition label viewing during foodselectiontask: front-of-package labels vsnutrition factslabels. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2015;115(10):1636–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Ares G, Arrúa A, Antúnez L, Vidal L, Machín L, Martínez J. Influence of label design on children's perception of two snack foods: comparison of rating and choice-based conjoint analysis. Food Qual Prefer. 2016;53:1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Dodds P, Wolfenden L, Chapman K, Wellard L, Hughes C, Wiggers J. The effect of energy and traffic light labelling on parent and child fast food selection: a randomised controlled trial. Appetite. 2014;73:23–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Chapman K, Nicholas P, Banovic D, Supramaniam R. The extent and nature of food promotion directed to children in Australian supermarkets. Health Promot Int. 2006;21(4):331–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Giménez A, de Saldamando L, Curutchet MR, Ares G. Package design and nutritional profile of foods targeted at children available in supermarkets in Monte video. Uruguay: Cadernos de Saúde Pública; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Buijzen M, Van Reijmersdal EA, Owen LH. Introducing the PCMC model: an investigative framework for young people’s processing of commercialized media content. Commun Theory. 2010;20:427–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Folkvord F, Veling H, Hans H. Targeting implicit approach reactions to snack food in children: effects on intake. Health Psychol. 2016;35(8):919–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Harris J, Speers S, Schwartz M, Brownell K. US food company branded advergames on the internet: Children’s exposure and effects on snack consumption. J Child Media. 2012;6:51–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Hémar-Nicolas V, Gollety M. Using brand character when targeting children: what for? An exploration of managers’ and children’s viewpoints. Young Consum. 2012;13(1):20–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Hémar-Nicolas V, Putri Hapsari H, Angka S, Olsen A. How cartoon characters and claims influence children's attitude towards a snack vegetable: an explorative cross-cultural comparison between Indonesia and Denmark. Food Qual Prefer. 2021;87:104031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. McNeal JU, Ji MF. Children’s visual memory of packaging. J Consum Mark. 2003;20(5):400–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Pires C, Agante L. Encouraging children to eat more healthily: the influence of packaging. J Consum Behav. 2011;10(3):161–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. The differential susceptibility to media effects model. J Commun. 2013;63(2):221–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Zeghache N. The impact of packaging colour on children’s brand name memorization (7–12 years old). Int J Retail Distrib Manag. 2014;42(11/12):1053–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Boyland EJ, Halford JCG. Television advertising and branding. Effects on eating behaviour and food preferences in children. Appetite. 2013;62:236–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Elliott C. Packaging fun: analyzing supermarket food messages targeted at children. Can J Commun. 2012;37(2):303–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Menon P, Ruel MT, Morris SS. Socioeconomic differentials in child stunting are consistently larger in urban than in rural areas. Food Nutr Bull. 2000;21(3):282–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Ruel MT, Garrett JL, Morris SS, Maxwell D, Oshaug O, Engle P, et al. Urban challenges to food and nutrition security: a review of food security, health, and caregiving in the cities. In: Food Consumption and Nutrition Division Discussion Paper 51. Washington, D.C.: International Food Policy Research Institute; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Smith LC, Ramakrishnan U, Ndiaye A, Haddad L, Martorell R. The importance of women.S status for child nutrition in developing countries. Research report 131. Washington, D.C: International Food Policy Research Institute; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  33. von Braun J, McComb J, Fred-Mensah B, Pandya-Lorch R. Urban food insecurity and malnutrition in developing countries: trends, policies, and research implications. Washington, D.C.: International Food Policy Research Institute; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Lima M, Aresb G, Deliza R. How do front of pack nutrition labels affect healthfulness perception of foods targeted at children? Insights from Brazilian children and parents. Food Qual Prefer. 2018;64:111–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Van de Poel E, O'Donnell O, Van Doorslaer E. Are urban children really healthier ? Soc Sci Med. 2007;65(10):1986–2003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Deolalikar A. Malnutrition and hunger. Copenhagen consensus 2008 perspective paper. Copenhagen: Copenhagen Consensus Centre; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  37. EUFIC. Global update on nutrition Labelling. The 2017 edition. Brussels: EUFIC; 2017. https://www.eufic.org/images/uploads/files/GUNL-2017-exsummary.pdf. Accessed 1 May 2020

    Google Scholar 

  38. Headey D. Developmental drivers of nutritional change: a cross-country analysis. World Dev. 2013;42:76–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Antúnez L, Giménez A, Maiche A, Ares G. Influence of interpretation aids on attentional capture, visual processing, and understanding of front-of-package nutrition labels. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2015;47:292–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Arrúa A, Curutchet MR, Rey N, Barreto P, Golovchenko N, Sellanes A. Impact of front-of-pack nutrition information and label design on children's choice of two snack foods: comparison of warnings and the traffic-light system. Appetite. 2017;116:139–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Feunekes GIJ, Gortemaker IA, Willems AA, Lion R, Van Den Kommer M. Front-of-pack nutrition labelling: testing effectiveness of different nutrition labelling formats front-of-pack in four European countries. Appetite. 2008;50:57–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hagmann D, Siegrist M. Nutri-score, multiple traffic light and incomplete nutrition labelling on food packages: effects on consumers’ accuracy in identifying healthier snack options. Food Qual Prefer. 2020;83:103894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Van Herpen E, Van Trijp HCM. Front-of-pack nutrition labels. Their effect on attention and choices when consumers have varying goals and time constraints. Appetite. 2011;57(1):148–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Borgmeier I, Westenhoefer J. Impact of different food label formats on healthiness evaluation and food choice of consumers: a randomized controlled study. BioMed Central Public Health. 2009;9(1):184.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Crosetto P, Muller L, Ruffieux B. Helping consumers with a front-of-pack label: numbers or colors? Experimental comparison between guideline daily amounts and traffic light in a diet-building exercise. J Econ Psychol. 2016;55:30–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Méjean C, Macouillard P, Péneau S, Lassale C, Hercbergg S, Castetbon K. Association of perception of front-of-pack labels with dietary, lifestyle and health characteristics. PLoS One. 2014;9(3):1–11.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Watson WL, Kelly B, Hector D, Hughes C, King L, Crawford J, et al. Can front-of-pack labelling schemes guide healthier food choices? Australian shoppers’ responses to seven labelling formats. Appetite. 2014;72:90–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Talati Z, Pettigrew S, Ball K, Hughes C, Kelly B, Neal B, et al. The relative ability of different front-of-pack labels to assist consumers discriminate between healthy, moderately healthy, and unhealthy foods. Food Qual Prefer. 2017;59:109–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Grunert KG, Wills JM. A review of European research on consumer response to nutrition information on food labels. J Public Health. 2007;15:385–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Zucchi ND, Fiates GMR. Analysis of the presence of nutrient claims on labels of ultra-processed foods directed at children and of the perception of kids on such claims. Rev Nutr. 2016;29(6):821–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Siegrist M, Leins-Hess R, Keller C. Which front-of-pack nutrition label is the most efficient one? The results of an eye-tracker study. Food Qual Prefer. 2015;39:183–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Koenigstorfer J, Groeppel-Klein A, Kamm F. Healthful food decision making in response to traffic light color-coded nutrition labelling. J Public Policy Mark. 2014;33(1):65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Ellis RM, Ellis RCT. Impact of a traffic light nutrition tool in a primary school. J R Soc Promot Heal. 2007;127:13–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Ministerio de Salud (MINSAL). Decreto número 13, de 2015. Santiago de Chile: Ministerio de Salud; 2015. https://www.leychile.cl/Navegar?idNorma=1078836. Accessed 2 June 2020

    Google Scholar 

  55. Schnettler B, Ares G, Sepúlveda N, Bravo S, Villalobos B, Hueche C, et al. How do consumers perceive reformulated foods after the implementation of nutritional warnings? Case study with frankfurters in Chile. Food Qual Prefer. 2019;74:179–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Ministry of Health. Decreto número 13, de 2015. Santiago de Chile: Ministerio de Salud; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Ares G, Aschemann-Witzel J, Curutchet MR, Antúnez L, Machín L, Vidal L, et al. Nutritional warnings and product substitution or abandonment: policy implications derived from a repeated purchase simulation. Food Qual Prefer. 2018;65:40–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Black A, Rayner M. Just read the label: understanding nutrition information in numeric, verbal and graphic formats. London: HMSO; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  59. European Dairy Association. European Dairy Association refutes proposed traffic light labelling 2017. https://reurl.cc/ZOvlDp. Accessed 10 June 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Lähteenmäki L. Claiming health in food products. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;27:196–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Sims J, Mikkelsen L, Gibson P, Warming E. Claiming health: front-of-package labelling of children's food. Oakland: Prevention Institute; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Andrews M, Mark N, Gary B, Steven C. Household food security in the United States. FANRR-8. Washington, DC: USDA, Economic Research Service; 2000.

  63. Neeley SM, Petricone B. Children’s (mis) understanding of nutritional information on product packages: seeking ways to help kids make healthier food choices. Adv Consum Res. 2006;33:556.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Cabrera M, Machín L, Arrúa A, Antúnez L, Curutchet MR, Giménez A, et al. Nutrition warnings as front-of-pack labels: Influence of design features on healthfulness perception and attentional capture. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(18):3360–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Wei XQ. Teach children to use money. Taipei: Times Culture; 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Gorton D, Ni MC, Chen MH, Dixon R. Nutrition labels: a survey of use, understanding and preferences among ethnically diverse shoppers in New Zealand. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12:1359–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Kim WK, Kim J. A study on the consumer’s perception of front-of-pack nutrition labeling. Nutr Res Pract. 2009;3:300–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Campos S, Doxey J, Hammond D. Nutrition labels on pre-packaged foods: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2011;14(8):1496–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Machín L, Cabrera M, Curutchet MR, Martínez J, Giménez A, Ares G. Consumer perception of the healthfulness of ultra-processed products featuring different front-of-pack nutrition labeling schemes. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2017;49:330–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Balasubramanium SK, Cole C. Consumers’ search and use of nutrition information: the challenge and promise of the nutrition labeling and education act. J Mark. 2002;66:112–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Aschemann-Witzel J, Hamm U. Do consumers prefer foods with nutrition and health claims? Results of a purchase simulation. J Mark Commun. 2010;16(1–2):47–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Shiv B, Fedorikhin A. Heart and mind in conflict: the interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. J Consum Res. 1999;26(3):278–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Zimmerman FJ, Shimoga SV. The effects of food advertising and cognitive load on food choices. BioMed Central Public Health. 2014;14(1):342–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Trendel O, Werle COC. Distinguishing the affective and cognitive bases of implicit attitudes to improve prediction of food choices. Appetite. 2016;104:33–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Haugtvedt CT. Handbook of consumer psychology. New York: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis Group; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Mau G, Schuhen M, Steinmann S, Schramm-Klein H. How children make purchase decisions: behaviour of the cued processors. Young Consum. 2016;17(2):111–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Damay C. What is the meaning of ‘price’ and ‘being expensive’ for children? Young Consum. 2008;9(3):179–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Damay C, Guichard N. The role of price in the socialisation of children as consumers. Rech Appl Mark. 2016;31(1):64–84.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Dixon H, Niven P, Scully M, Wakefield M. Food marketing with movie character toys: effects on young children's preferences for unhealthy and healthier fastfood meals. Appetite. 2017;117:342–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Chandon P, Wansink B. Does food marketing need to make us fat? A review and solutions. Nutr Rev. 2012;70(10):571–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Angka S, Hémar-Nicolas V, Hapsari HP, Olsen A. How packaging colours and claims influence children's vegetable attitude and intake – an exploratory cross-cultural comparison between Indonesia and Denmark. Food Qual Prefer. 2020;79:103795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Raghunathan R, Naylor RW, Hoyer WD. The unhealthy = tasty intuition and its effects on taste inferences, enjoyment, and choice of food products. J Mark. 2006;70(4):170–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Bialkova S, van Trijp HC. An efficient methodology for assessing attention to and effect of nutrition information displayed front-of-pack. Food Qual Prefer. 2011;22(6):592–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from the National Science Council, Taiwan to Ching-Yi Wang (110–2221-E-468-008) and Dengchuan Cai (110–2622-E-224-014) for helping experiment implementation fee.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

C-Y W is responsible for the entire research planning, ERP data interpretation and article writing. C-J H is responsible for collecting literature. All authors have read and approved the manuscript. D C is responsible for all contacts and assists in correcting the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dengchuan Cai.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The experimental standards of the study were approved by the China Medical University & Hospital Research Ethics Center of Taiwan (CRREC-109-027). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to participation. For participants under the age of 16 years, informed consent have been obtained from a parent and/or legal guardian. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wang, CY., Hsu, CJ. & Cai, D. Effects of food nutrition labels on the health awareness of school-age children. BMC Public Health 22, 1249 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13613-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13613-y

Keywords