Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Why patients want to take or refuse to take antibiotics: an inventory of motives

Abstract

Background

Inappropriate use of antibiotics is a worldwide issue. In order to help public health institutions and each particular physician to change patterns of consumption among patients, it is important to understand better the reasons why people accept to take or refuse to take the antibiotic drugs. This study explored the motives people give for taking or refusing to take antibiotics.

Methods

Four hundred eighteen adults filled out a 60-item questionnaire that consisted of assertions referring to reasons for which the person had taken antibiotics in the past and a 70-item questionnaire that listed reasons for which the person had sometimes refused to take antibiotics.

Results

A six-factor structure of motives to take antibiotics was found: Appropriate Prescription, Protective Device, Enjoyment (antibiotics as a quick fix allowing someone to go out), Others’ Pressure, Work Imperative, and Personal Autonomy. A four-factor structure of motives not to take antibiotics was found: Secondary Gain (through prolonged illness), Bacterial Resistance, Self-defense (the body is able to defend itself) and Lack of trust. Scores on these factors were related to participants’ demographics and previous experience with antibiotics.

Conclusion

Although people are generally willing to follow their physician’s prescription of antibiotics, a notable proportion of them report adopting behaviors that are beneficial to micro-organisms and, as a result, potentially detrimental to humans.

Peer Review reports

Background

Antibiotics consumption is on the rise in most countries, especially in countries forming the BRICS (Brazil, Russia, India, China and South-Africa) group [1]. Inappropriate use of antibiotics is a worldwide issue that concerns developed countries as well as developing countries. This issue can, nevertheless, arise differently from one part of the world to another, depending on the level of economic development and local cultures [2]. Irresponsible antibiotic use may have detrimental effects—increasing antibiotic resistance (the public health level), and causing side effects (the patient level), either directly through gastrointestinal side effects and allergic reactions or indirectly by changing the nature of the gut flora [3, 4]. In order to help public institutions and each particular physician to change patterns of consumption among patients, it is important to understand better the reasons why people (a) accept to take antibiotics when they are prescribed, (b) self-medicate themselves when denied the expected prescription, or (c) refuse to take the antibiotic drugs that have been duly prescribed [5].

Prescription by a physician is certainly not the only reason to take antibiotics: Patients consume many over-the-counter pharmacological substances, and antibiotics are just one of them. In France, antibiotics are available only on a physician’s prescription, but they may be borrowed from family members or they can be bought on the Internet. The reasons for antibiotic use by patients have, until now, not been examined in a systematic way. Studies about patients’ expectations regarding antibiotic prescription converge on the view that physicians tend to overestimate patients’ expectations [6, 7]. Studies about patients’ level of knowledge about antibiotic use converge on the view that it is quite poor, and, in particular, that the erroneous belief that antibiotics are indicated in cases of viral infections has been widespread [8,9,10,11]. Recent studies suggest, however, that patients around the world are now better informed [12,13,14,15,16,17].

The present study

Although instructive, these studies do not tell much regarding the psychological motives that govern patients’ behaviors regarding antibiotics use. As shown in previous studies on patient’s motives for attending or refusing to attend health centers [18] for undergoing/refusing to undergo surgery [19] or for donating organs [20], these psychological motives are likely to form a complex net, and the nature and impact of some of them may be totally unexpected. The goal of the present study was, therefore, to explore, in systematic way, the motives people evoke when they take antibiotics or when they refuse to take them.

Method

Participants

The participants were a convenience sample of adults enrolled during daylight hours by two trained research assistants. Participants were approached in different public areas of Toulouse, France (e.g., the campus, post offices, schools, markets). Seven hundred fifty persons were approached, and 418 (56%) agreed to participate. All the participants who agreed to take part in the study had been prescribed antibiotics in the past by their physician. Most of the time, they have decided to take them but sometimes, they have decided not to take them or to discontinue the treatment. All participants provided informed consent. Their demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the participants. Mean scores observed for each factor as a function of participants’ characteristics

Material

Two separate questionnaires were created (a) a 60-item questionnaire listing reasons for which the person has taken antibiotics in the past, and (b) a 70-item questionnaire listing reasons for which the person has sometimes refused to take antibiotics. A list of 100 items was created on the basis of previous literature on antibiotics consumption [10, 11, 13, 15, 21,22,23] and on motivation [24, 25]. This list was shown to a focus group of four adults who were members of the public. They reformulated 48 items judged as ambiguous and suggested 22 additional items based on their personal views. This augmented list was then presented to another focus group who suggested 8 additional items.

The common wording of all items – “One of the reasons why I have been led to take (to refuse to take) antibiotics was” – was chosen to reflect the fact that several motives can be operating at the same time or at different times for the same person [25]. A 15-point scale was printed following each sentence. The two extremes of the scales were labeled “Never happened for this motive” (1) and “Frequently happened” (15). The questionnaire is shown in Additional file 1.

Procedure

Participants answered individually in a quiet room. Half of the participants were presented with the reason-to-take-antibiotics items first and then with the reason-to-refuse items. The other participants were presented with the items in the reverse order. The questionnaires took approximately 50 min to complete. Then, participants were presented with a questionnaire regarding their demographics and personal experience with antibiotics. The research adhered to the legal requirements of the study country: informed consent was obtained and participants’ anonymity was respected.

Data analyses

Mean scores of the reason-to-take items and of the reason-not-to-take items were computed. Two separate exploratory factor analyses were conducted, one on each set of items. They showed that 24 reason-to-take items and 34 reason-not-to-take items did not load (correlation < .30) on any factor or loaded on more than one factor. They were removed from the analyses, and a second set of factor analyses was conducted. Six interpretable factors (68% of the variance) with eigen-values ranging from 1.11 to 14.43 were observed in the reason-to-take condition, and four interpretable factors (66% of the variance) with eigen-values ranging from 1.45 to 14.88 were observed in the reason-not-to-take condition. Varimax rotations were performed. Ten mean factor scores were computed. A series of forward linear stepwise regression analyses was conducted with the demographic characteristics as the independent variables and the ten scores as the dependent variables.

Results

One hundred and forty seven males and 271 females aged 18–85 years participated in the study. The mean scores of the reason-to-take items ranged from 2.21 to 9.75 (out of 15). Main results of the first exploratory factor analysis are shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Results of the second factor analysis on the agree-to-take items. Means and standard deviations. Cronbach’s alpha. Only four items for each factor – the ones with the highest loadings – are shown

The first factor (21% of the variance) was labelled Appropriate prescription since it loaded on items expressing the idea that antibiotics were prescribed by a qualified physician and that this prescription looked reasonable to the participants’ eyes. The mean of the four items with the highest loadings was 8.46--SD (standard deviation) = 4.89--, the highest value observed. The second factor—Protective device (11% of the variance)—expressed the idea that antibiotics can protect the body from bacterial invasion, M (mean) = 5.78, SD = 3.71. The third factor—Enjoyment (11%)—expressed the idea that antibiotics were considered as a quick fix allowing someone to go out and celebrate the week-end as usual (M = 4.17, SD = 3.60). The fourth factor—Others’ pressure (10%)—expressed the idea that antibiotics were taken mainly in order to reassure close relatives (M = 4.41, SD = 3.53). The fifth factor—Work imperative (8%)—expressed the idea that antibiotics were taken mainly to be able to achieve important work (M = 6.99, SD = 4.13). Finally, the sixth factor—Personal autonomy (7%)—expressed the idea that through the taking of antibiotics one can shorten one’s dependence upon others (M = 4.51, SD = 3.86).

The mean scores of the reason-to-refuse items ranged from 2.29 to 10.61 (out of 15). Main results of the second factor analysis are shown in Table 3. The first factor (34% of the variance) was labelled Secondary Gain since it loaded on items expressing the idea that through prolonged illness one can benefit from increased social support and one may also be able to control more easily one’s social environment (M = 3.92, SD = 4.27). The second factor—Bacterial Resistance (14% of the variance)—expressed the idea that the irresponsible use of antibiotics may facilitate the process of bacterial resistance (M = 6.41, SD = 3.87). The third factor—Self-defense (10%)—expressed the idea that the body was able to defend itself against the infection, in particular when it was not severe (M = 8.92, SD = 3.77). Finally, the fourth factor—Lack of trust (8%)—expressed the idea that one may not always be fully confident in the prescriber’s competence (M = 5.09, SD = 3.61).

Table 3 Results of the second factor analysis on the refuse-to-take items. Means and standard deviations. Cronbach’s alpha. Only four items for each factor – the ones with the highest loadings – are shown

Table 1 shows the relationship between participants’ characteristics and scores on each factor of motives and Table 4 shows the results from the stepwise regression analyses. Appropriate prescription was significantly associated with gender—ß (beta) = .16--and number of children (ß = −.13). Protective device was only associated with change of treatment (ß = .18). Enjoyment was associated with age (ß = −.14), number of therapies (ß = .15) and concerns with public health issues (ß = −.10). Work imperative was associated with gender (ß = .13), number of antibiotic treatment in the past year (ß = .09) and change of treatment (ß = .13). Secondary gain was only associated with age (ß = .17). Bacterial resistance was associated with personal experience of inefficacy (ß = .15), conviction that antibiotics are in general useless (ß = .13) and expressed concerns about resistance (ß = .27). Self-defense was associated with personal experience of inefficacy (ß = .13) and expressed concerns about resistance. Finally, Lack of trust was similarly associated with personal experience of inefficacy (ß = .15) and expressed concerns about resistance (ß = .18).

Table 4 Main Results From the Stepwise Linear Regression Analyses

Discussion

The most strongly endorsed motive to agree to take antibiotics, especially among females and patients with children, was that they had been prescribed by a physician. This reason was, however, associated with the idea that the physician’s prescription was at the same time judged appropriate and reasonable. This means that, if in general people were willing to follow the prescriber’s recommendations, they were unwilling to do so blindly.

The second most strongly endorsed motive, especially among females, and among people who had experienced trouble with antibiotics in the past, was work pressure. In addition, the persons invoking this kind of motive tended more than others to take antibiotics on a regular basis. Antibiotics may thus be viewed by some people as a way to enhance performance at work.

The third most strongly endorsed motive, especially among people who had experienced trouble with antibiotics, was related to the fear and suffering engendered by the infection. It is logical that people in bad health who have experienced unsuccessful treatment are willing to take, more than others (and at times to ask for), antibiotics to protect themselves; that is, to keep themselves able to be well and to perform well in their environment.

Three other kinds of motives were also found, although they were less strongly endorsed than the others: close relatives’ concerns, personal autonomy, and enjoyment. The first two motives were related to family and social life but the third one is more concerning. People who more frequently than others endorsed enjoyment-type motives tended to discount the severity of bacterial resistance. They were younger and reported having taken antibiotics more than twice the past year. This suggests that a small, but not negligible segment of the sample (18%) considers that antibiotics are just consumption goods that can be freely used.

The most strongly endorsed motive to refuse to take antibiotics was that one’s body was seen as not severely endangered by the infection and, as a result, would be able to defend itself successfully. This result was consistent with findings by Jonsson and Haraldsson [26]. This kind of motive was endorsed especially by people who, more than others thought that antibiotics are ineffective.

The second most strongly endorsed motive to refuse was directly related to concern about bacterial resistance. This result was consistent with findings by Finkelstein et al. [23], but this commendable vision seemed to have its limitations: it was expressed especially by people who (a) had experienced troubles with treatment with antibiotics, (b) thought that antibiotics are generally ineffective, and (c) did not hesitate to stop treatment inappropriately.

Two other motives to refuse antibiotics were found: (a) the presence of secondary gain associated with prolonged illness, especially among older people, and (b) lack of trust in the prescriber, especially among people who had had troubles with past treatment with antibiotics and at the same time reported behaving in a way that is paradoxical because it was potentially dangerous for themselves (i.e., keeping antibiotics after treatment for later use).

Limitations

The study has at least two limitations. First, motives were assessed through self-reports. Participants’ responses were, however, clearly structured: If they had consciously decided to misreport their motives, responses would have been given in a more or less random way and, as a result, no clear factor structure could have been found. Now that the complete structure of motives is known, it will be possible, in future studies, conducted in collaboration with physicians, to contact people who have recently been prescribed antibiotics, to ask them whether they have taken these antibiotics, and, using a shortened six-item (or four-item) version of the questionnaire, to assess the reasons why they have taken them (or not taken them or discontinued the treatment). Second, the sample was conducted in a single site in France. Its results must, therefore, be generalized with care to other populations in the country, namely to those who live in rural settings. In addition, the two models of motives have been issued from exploratory factor analyses. They need to be confirmed on other samples, using confirmatory factor analysis, and measurement invariance has to be assessed (men vs. women, young vs. aged, often sick or not).

Conclusions

People are generally willing to follow their physician’s prescription of antibiotics. In our study, however, Appropriate prescription, although the leading motive, was not rated as highly as could have been expected: its mean rating was located only slightly above the center of the response scale. This implies that people would be willing to take antibiotics if instructed to do so and at the same time, for example: (a) told that antibiotics will attenuate their physical suffering or (b) reassured that, owing to taking antibiotics, they will be able more quickly to achieve an important task.

Although most people seemed to be aware that bacterial resistance was a big public health issue, a minority (about 21%) did not agree with this view, and, what is more concerning, they were mostly among those who reported that, when they are ill, they do not hesitate to use antibiotics simply in order to go out and have fun with friends. It should be explained to these people that when ill, the best they could do is to stay at home and try not to contaminate large groups of people.

Although it is certainly a good thing that people sometimes are unwilling to take antibiotics, there seemed to be a gap between the wisdom or altruism of their reasons and what they reported regarding their behavior. They were aware more than others of the public health issue and also of their body’s capacity to defend itself against infections, but at the same time they also tended more than others to report behaviors that were at variance with their motives. In particular they did not hesitate to stop treatment before it had been completed; that is, to do what would facilitate mutations and adaptations in microorganisms. In fact, they seemed to be essentially acting out of previous negative personal experience with antibiotics because their statements regarding bacterial resistance as a big public health issue were more rhetorical than grounded in even minimal understanding (see also Napolitano et al. [13]). In other words, even people who express a willingness to take antibiotics only if really needed must be educated about the mechanisms by which micro-organisms adapt to human defenses [27].

Finally, one out of five participants expressed lack of trust in physicians and treatment with antibiotics. Unfortunately, these people, more frequently than others, reported behaviors that were potentially more dangerous to themselves (e.g., shortening duration of treatment) than anything physicians could recommend or prescribe in these circumstances.

Overall, a notable proportion of people report adopting behaviors that are more beneficial to micro-organisms than to humans and other animals; that is, behaviors that are likely to increase bacterial resistance. If taught that bacterial resistance is a planetary health concern, these people would certainly not be surprised, and a huge majority would agree. They would even be likely to report behaviors such as shortening the duration of treatment and using past-prescribed antibiotics as proofs of their good intentions. As a result, they must be taught that their behavior is counterproductive, and that their small actions have global consequences.

In summary, in each instance of consultation involving prescription of antibiotics--and particularly if physicians have detected erroneous beliefs, physicians must remind patients that antibiotics help fight dangerous micro-organisms, that temporary isolation is often the best way to limit contagion, that stopping treatment before completion is exactly what helps micro-organisms to become stronger, and that inappropriate action by a minority can affect the whole human population. This information must, however, not be delivered in a confrontational way: As stressed by the promoters of motivational interviewing [28], motivation to act in a determined way can only be elicited from the patient; it cannot be imposed from outside.

Abbreviations

BRICS:

Brazil, Russia, India, China and South-Africa

M:

Mean

SD:

Standard deviation

ß:

Beta

References

  1. Van Boeckel TP, Gandra S, Ashok A, Caudron Q, Grenfell BT, Levin SA, et al. Global antibiotic consumption 2000 to 2010: an analysis of national pharmaceutical sales data. Lancet Infect Dis. 2014;14(8):742–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Ayukekbong JA, Ntemgwa M, Atabe AN. The threat of antimicrobial resistance in developing countries: causes and control strategies. Antimicrob Resist Infect Control. 2017;6:47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. McNulty CA, Johnson AP. The European antibiotic awareness day. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2008;62:853–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Carabotti M, Scirocco A, Maselli MA, Severi C. The gut-brain axis interactions between enteric microbiota, central and enteric nervous systems. Ann Gastroenterol. 2015;28(2):203–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Fischer MA, Stedman MR, Lil J, Vogeli C, Shrank WH, Brookhart MA, et al. Primary medication non-adherence: analysis of 195,930 electronic prescriptions. J Gen Intern Med. 2010;25(4):284–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Coenen S, Francis N, Kelly M, Hood K, Nuttall J, Little P, et al. Are patient views about antibiotics related to clinician perceptions, management and outcome? A multi-country study in outpatients with acute cough. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e76691.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Cartwright A. Patients and their doctors. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul; 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Eurobarometer (2001). Europeans, science and technology. Retrieved at https://ec.europa.eu/research/press/2001/pr0612en-report.pdf

  9. Ferech M, Coenen S, Malhotra-Kumar S, Dvorakova K, Hendrickx E, Suetens C, et al. European surveillance of antimicrobial consumption (ESAC): outpatient antibiotic use in Europe. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2006;58(2):401–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. McNulty CA, Boyle P, Nichols T, Clappison P, Davey P. Don't wear me out--the public's knowledge of and attitudes to antibiotic use. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2007;59(4):727–38.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Van den Eng J, Marcus R, Hadler JL, Imhoff B, Vugia DJ, Cieslak PR, et al. Consumer attitudes and use of antibiotics. Emerg Infect Dis. 2003;9(9):1128–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Alzoubi K, Al-Azzam S, Alhusban A, Mukattash T, Al-Zubaidy S, Alomari N, et al. An audit on the knowledge, beliefs and attitudes about the uses and side-effects of antibiotics among outpatients attending two teaching hospitals in Jordan. East Mediterr Health J. 2013;19(5):478–84.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Napolitano F, Izzo MT, Di Giuseppe G, Angelillo IF. Public knowledge, attitudes, and experience regarding the use of antibiotics in Italy. PLoS One. 2013;8(12). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084177.

  14. Ling OA, Hassali MA, Al-Haddad MS, Syed Sulaiman SA, Shafie AA, Awaisu A. Public knowledge and attitudes towards antibiotic usage: a cross-sectional study among the general public in the state of Penang, Malaysia. J Infect Dev Ctries. 2011;5(5):338–47.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Panagakou SG, Spyridis N, Papaevangelou V, Theodoridou KM, Goutziana GP, Theodoridou MN, et al. Antibiotic use for upper respiratory tract infections in children: a cross-sectional survey of knowledge, attitudes, and practices (KAP) of parents in Greece. BMC Pediatr. 2011;11:60. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2431-11-60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Parimi N, Pinto Pereira LN, Prabhakar P. Caregivers' practices, knowledge and beliefs of antibiotics in pediatric upper respiratory tract infections in Trinidad and Tobago: a cross-sectional study. BMC Fam Pract. 2004;5:28. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2296-5-28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Yu M, Zhao G, Stålsby Lundborg C, Zhu Y, Zhao Q, Xu B. Knowledge, attitudes, and practices of parents in rural China on the use of antibiotics in children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Infect Dis. 2014;14:112. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2334-14-112.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Kpanake L, Dassa K, Mullet E. Why most Togolese patients do not seek care for malaria in health facilities: a theory-driven inventory of reasons. Psychol Health Med. 2009;14(4):502–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Zounon O, Hans-Moevi Akué A, Cohovi Quenum G, Sorum PC, Mullet E. Why people in Benin are reluctant to undergo amputations? A systematic inventory of motives. J Health Psychol. 2016;21(11):2753–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Guedj M, Muñoz Sastre MT, Mullet E. Donating organs: a theory-driven inventory of motives. Psychol Health Med. 2011;16(4):418–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Emslie MJ, Bond CM. Public knowledge, attitudes and behaviour regarding antibiotics--a survey of patients in general practice. Eur J Gen Pract. 2003;9(3):84–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Faber MS, Heckenbach K, Velasco E, Eckmanns T. Antibiotics for the common cold: expectations of Germany's general population. Euro Surveill. 2010;15(35):pii:19655.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Finkelstein JA, Dutta-Linn M, Meyer R, Goldman R. Childhood infections, antibiotics, and resistance: what are parents saying now? Clin Pediatr. 2014;53(2):145–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Apter MJ, editor. Motivational styles in everyday life: a guide to reversal theory. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Mullet E, Kpanake L, Zounon O, Guedj M, Munoz Sastre MT. Putting reversal Theory’s model of four domains of experience in the hot seat. J Motiv Emot Pers. 2014;2:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Jónsson H, Haraldsson RH. Parents’ perspectives on otitis media and antibiotics: a qualitative study. Scand J Prim Health Care. 2002;20(1):35–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Goossens H, Ferech M, Vander Stichele R, Elseviers M. ESAC project group. Outpatient antibiotic use in Europe and association with resistance: a cross-national database study. Lancet. 2005;365(9459):579–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing: preparing people to change. New York: Guilford Press; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Angélique Barrau, Joan Sesbastian Cardona Gallego, Caroline Dangas, Marion Froment, Hélène Lauriol, and Vega Andrea Izaguirre, for their help in gathering the data.

Funding

This study was funded by University of Toulouse Jean Jaurès and Canada Research Chairs program (Grant number: 950–230745). The funding bodies had no role in the design of the study, collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in writing the manuscript or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Availability of data and materials

All data collected is available and can be accessed by contacting the corresponding author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to the overall study concept and design. AB and MTMS managed the data collection process. AB, MTMS and EM conducted the statistical analysis. All authors contributed to interpretation of the data. MTMS, LK and EM wrote the first draft of the manuscript. PCS and AB provided editorial feedback on the manuscript drafts. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lonzozou Kpanake.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval for the study was granted by the Ethics and Work Unit, École Pratique des Hautes Études (France). All participants signed a consent form before completing this survey, and responses were anonymous.

Consent for publication

The authors have obtained consent to publish from the participants.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Questionnaire. (PDF 305 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bagnulo, A., Muñoz Sastre, MT., Kpanake, L. et al. Why patients want to take or refuse to take antibiotics: an inventory of motives. BMC Public Health 19, 441 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-6834-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-6834-x

Keywords