Skip to main content

Relationships between parent feeding behaviors and parent and child characteristics in Brazilian preschoolers: a cross-sectional study

Abstract

Background

Eating habits formed in early childhood are influenced by parental feeding behaviors, warranting investigation of predictors and correlates of parent feeding. We aimed to describe relationships between parental feeding practices and parent and child characteristics in a sample of Brazilian preschoolers.

Methods

Four hundred and two parents of preschoolers enrolled in private schools of São Paulo and Campinas, Brazil, completed a Brazilian version of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire, as well as questions about parental attitudes, child food intake, other obesity-associated behaviors, and socioeconomic and demographic characteristics. We ran bivariate logistic regression models examining associations between independent variables and each feeding practice. Next, we ran multiple logistic regression models predicting each parental feeding practice.

Results

Greater ‘Restriction for Weight Control’ and ‘Restriction for Health’ were associated with lower maternal education (OR = 2.42 (CI 95% 1.07–5.48) and 2.79 (CI 95% 1.25–6.22), respectively), and with higher concern about child overweight (OR = 2.46, CI 95% 1.64–3.69 for ‘Restriction for Weight Control’, only), while greater ‘Pressure’ was associated with greater concern about child underweight (OR = 2.30, CI 95% 1.53–3.47) and lower maternal BMI (OR = 0.94, CI 95% 0.88–1.00). Greater use of ‘Emotion Regulation/ Food as Reward’ was associated with lower maternal education (OR = 2.22, CI 95% 1.05–4.71). In analyses of positive feeding practices, lesser use of ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ and ‘Monitoring’ was associated with greater intake of less healthy foods in children (OR = 1.53 (CI 95% 1.01–2.32) and OR = 1.94 (CI 95% 1.27–2.97), respectively), and greater use of screen devices (OR = 1.59 (CI 95% 1.04–2.44) and OR = 1.57 (CI 95% 1.03–2.39), respectively). Lesser use of ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ was additionally associated with higher maternal BMI (OR = 1.09, CI 95% 1.03–1.16), and lesser use of ‘Monitoring’ with lesser perceived parent responsibility for child feeding (OR = 1.68, CI 95% 1.12–2.52).

Conclusions

Our results demonstrate diverse socioeconomic, anthropometric and behavioral correlates of parent feeding in a large Brazilian sample of parents of preschoolers.

Peer Review reports

Background

Eating habits formed in preschool years are likely to persist into later life such that early unhealthy food patterns could have implications for health throughout the life course [1, 2]. These habits are influenced by internal factors, e.g. the child’s own taste and food preferences, and by external factors, such as peers, media, and parents. Parents play a unique role in early childhood because they act as providers, enforcers and role models for children, who are still highly dependent on them [3, 4]. It is particularly important to study parental feeding practices in the preschool years since communication between parent and child becomes more nuanced than at younger ages, while responding to children’s food demands becomes more complex [5].

Parents use a variety of strategies to influence when, what, and how much children eat, and these may have maximal impact when children are young, and in constant contact with their parents [6]. Parental feeding practices are an attempt to maintain or modify children’s eating habits and weight status. Some of these efforts achieve the parent’s intended goals. For example, the use of feeding practices such as making healthy foods available, and modelling the consumption of healthy food, have been shown to facilitate healthy eating behaviors in young children [7]. Additionally, a recent prospective observational study suggested that monitoring a child’s intake of high energy-dense foods at age 2 was associated with a healthier weight status one year later [8]. Kröller & Warschburger (2008) found that mothers with higher education reported using more monitoring regarding children’s food intake [9].

On the other hand, certain behaviors may have unintended consequences. Coercive feeding practices, such as ‘Restriction’, may promote overeating in children, increasing the risk for subsequent problems with eating and energy balance [10]. Restricting children’s food intake is correlated with parental knowledge and overvaluation of the child’s weight and body shape [11], and has been associated with several demographic characteristics of parents, including lower maternal age and education [12]. Pressuring the child to eat, usually during mealtimes, is also a common practice, which may override children’s internal cues of hunger and satiety [1], and associate with eating beyond satiety, since internal satiety cues are ignored [1, 13], increased intake of unhealthy foods in early childhood [9, 14], and a negative atmosphere at mealtime [4].

Given the potential relevance of parental feeding practices in young children for future nutrition disorders, it is necessary to investigate potential predictors of these practices across different contexts and environments. Child overweight is increasing in developing countries such as Brazil, with national surveys showing an increase of 160% from 1989 to 2006 (9.4% per year) [15], accompanied by large increases in availability and variety of food products promoting ultra-processed food consumption [16]. Since parents are the first nutritional educators for their children, the investigation of parental feeding practices is essential, yet currently understudied, in Brazil.

The present study therefore aimed to test associations between multiple parental feeding practices and diverse parent and child characteristics in a large sample of preschoolers enrolled in Brazilian private schools. To ensure coverage of a wide range of influential feeding strategies we used a validated Brazilian version [17] of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire (CFPQ) [18]. As parental feeding practices have been associated with a number of parent characteristics such as maternal age [6, 12], education [12], weight status [6, 12], socioeconomic status [6], and child characteristics such as gender [14], child age [12, 14], weight status [1], daily screen time [14] and food intake [14, 17], we included each of these factors as independent variables.

In accordance with the literature, our primary hypotheses were that the use of potentially negative and non-nutritive feeding practices (‘Restriction for weight control’, ‘Restriction for health’, ‘Pressure’, ‘Emotion regulation/ food as reward’) would be associated with lower parental educational levels, higher parental weight status, less healthy eating habits in children (e.g. greater intake of ultra-processed foods), and higher child weight status. We additionally hypothesized that for positive feeding practices (‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ and ‘Monitoring’), lower use would be associated with lesser parental education, less healthy eating habits (e.g. lesser intake of healthier foods such as fruits and vegetables), and higher child weight status. We expected parental restriction to be predicted by parents’ concerns about child overweight, and parental pressure to eat to be predicted by concerns about child underweight.

Methods

Study design

This research was conducted as part of a broader cross-sectional study, the ‘Estudo de Práticas Alimentares’ (EPA), which aimed to adapt and validate the CFPQ for a Brazilian sample [17], as well as to investigate relationships with parent and child characteristics.

Participants

Participants were middle- and high-income parents of 2-to-5-year-olds recruited from April through June 2014 from private schools in the cities of São Paulo and Campinas. Children with diseases that were related to nutrition and/or could influence parental feeding practices (e.g. cystic fibrosis, diabetes or autism), children who had a younger sibling who was participating (in order to avoid sample unit duplication), and not being within the eligible age group were excluded.

To estimate the sample size, we based power calculations on an effect of maternal weight status (overweight i.e. ≥25.0 Kg/m2 versus non-overweight i.e. < 25.0 Kg/m2), assuming a proportion of 65 and 45% for the use of more coercive parental feeding practices among overweight and non-overweight mothers, respectively. After an addition of 10% for anticipated losses, this analysis yielded a minimum required sample size of 310 respondents to achieve a probability of 0.05 for type I error and 0.20 for type II error.

Procedures

The first phase of the EPA involved validation of the CFPQ in Brazilian preschool [17] and school-aged children [19]. This resulted in a slightly modified, reduced instrument, with a new factor structure (6 factors and 43 items), that was used for the current study. The validated instrument showed good validity and reliability (‘Restriction for Weight Control’: α = 0.84; ‘Restriction for Health’: α = 0.88; ‘Emotion Regulation/Food as Reward: α = 0.74; ‘Pressure’: α = 0.72; ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’: α = 0.83; ‘Monitoring’: α = 0.86) [17]. Before participating in the survey, caregivers each gave written consent. Survey packets containing questionnaires and instructions requesting completion within two weeks by a parent were left in each classroom. In one of the schools, the questionnaires were administered and completed by parents before a parents and teachers meeting. More details on the data collection procedure are described elsewhere [17]. This research received ethical approval from the Federal University of São Paulo (UNIFESP) ethics committee.

Measures

The Brazilian version of the CFPQ includes the following factors: ‘Restriction for Weight Control’ (RWC, the degree to which a parent restricts their child’s food intake in order to limit weight gain – 7 items), ‘Restriction for Health’ (RH, the degree to which a parent restricts their child’s food intake with the motivation of promoting healthy eating rather than influencing body weight – 5 items), ‘Emotion Regulation/Food as Reward’ (ER/FR, the degree to which a parent uses food as reward for desired behavior in their child, or to regulate emotion – 6 items), ‘Pressure’ (P, the degree to which a parent pressures the child to eat – 4 items), ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ (HEG, parents’ facilitation of a healthy eating environment, including teaching, modeling and child’s involvement in food intake – 16 items), and ‘Monitoring’ (M, the degree to which the parent keeps track of their child’s consumption of unhealthy foods – 4 items). Responses are given on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘never’ to ‘always’ for frequency items, and from ‘totally disagree’ to ‘totally agree’ for statements [17]. For each CFPQ sub-scale, we summed the response values of each item.

Information about socioeconomic and demographic characteristics, as well as parent and child anthropometric measures, were reported by the parents (376 (93.54%) mothers and 26 (6.46%) fathers). Parental attitude scales were derived from the Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ) [20]. ‘Concern about Child Overweight’ (3 items) and ‘Perceived Responsibility for Child Feeding’ (3 items) scales were taken directly from the CFQ. ‘Concern about Child Underweight’ (3 items) was adapted from the CFQ by changing the words ‘overweight’ to ‘underweight’, and ‘diet’ to ‘eat more’ [18]. Responses were given on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘never’ to ‘always’, and scale scores were generated by calculating the mean of each factor (Table 1).

Table 1 Demographic and anthropometric characteristics of children (n = 402)

Dietary data regarding child food intake for the last seven days was gathered using a Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) specially designed for this population, based on the foods most frequently consumed in the Brazilian population, and incorporating ultra-processed foods known to be associated with obesity [21]. Responses were given on a 5 point scale with the following options: 1 = did not consume, 2 = consumed 1–2 times/week, 3 = consumed 3–4 times/week, 4 = consumed 5–6 times/week, 5 = consumed every day. Using this data we created variables representing frequencies of intake for traditional foods (i.e. beans, meat and eggs, fruits, milk and dairy, vegetables and grains) and ultra-processed foods (i.e. artificial juice, breakfast cereal, chips, chocolate milk, crackers/biscuits/cakes with and without stuffing, dairy desserts, fast food, ice-cream/popsicle, instant noodles, processed meat, soft drink and sugary snacks) [22]. For each food category, scores for all food items were summed, divided by the total number of items, and then transformed to a 1–100 scale. Additionally, we asked parents to provide information about child’s daily screen time using the following response options: 1 = no use, 2 = less than 2 h, 3 = 3–4 h, 4 = 5–6 h, 5 = more than 6 h.

Data treatment and statistical analysis

In order to a) choose an approach that was robust to the non-normal distributions of parent feeding behavior scores (confirmed using Kolmogorov-Smirnov testing), b) facilitate interpretation of the studied associations by quantifying the risk of defined levels of each parent and child characteristic being associated with defined quantities of parental feeding behavior, and c) promote comparison between factors by presenting associations using a shared scale for risk magnitude, we chose to dichotomize our multiple independent and dependent variables and apply logistic regression.

For CFPQ sub-scales (dependent variables), we implemented a median split, and considered higher (above median) scores on negative/non-nutritive feeding practices (i.e. RWC: > 25.00 (of range 0–96.43; RH: > 70.00 (of range 0–100); P: > 62.50 (of range 0–100); ER/FR: > 12.50 (of range 0–75), and lower (below median) scores on positive feeding practices (i.e. HEG: < 87.50 (of range 46.87–100) and M: < 98.75 (of range 0–100)), as indicating higher obesity risk.

For maternal weight status we split the sample into those who were Overweight/Obese (i.e. BMI ≥ 25) and those who were normal-weight. Child weight status was determined based on World Health Organization (WHO) guidelines, such that those children with z-score < − 3 were designated ‘Extremely underweight’, those with z-score ≥ − 3 but < − 2 ‘Underweight’, those with z-score ≥ − 2 but < + 1 ‘Normal weight’, those with z-score ≥ + 1 but < + 2 ‘Overweight’, those with z-score ≥ + 2 but < + 3 ‘Obese’ and those with z-score ≥ + 3 ‘Extremely obese’ [23]. We then split the sample into those who were Overweight/ Obese/ Extremely obese) vs. those who were Normal weight/ Underweight/ Extremely underweight.

For parental attitude scales, we based dichotomization on the median for each scale, i.e., a score < 15.0 for Perceived Responsibility for Child Feeding, which corresponded to a mean response of Never, Seldom, Half of the time, or Most of the time across the scale items a score, a score ≥ 7.0 for Concern about Child Overweight, which corresponded to a mean response of ‘Concerned’, ‘Fairly concerned’ or ‘Very concerned’ across the scale items, and a score ≥ 9.0 for Concern about Child Underweight, which corresponded to a mean response of ‘Fairly concerned’ or ‘Very concerned’ across the scale items.

For the FFQ data (independent variables), we implemented cut-off values based on dual consideration of the data distribution and the associated response values. For ultra-processed foods, we used a cut-off value of 33rd centile (i.e score ≥ 1.69), considering values equal or above it as indicating ‘frequent consumption’, (approximately corresponding to consumption of specified items from the specified ultra-processed food categories, on average, 1–2 times a week or more), and values below indicating ‘infrequent consumption’ (approximately corresponding to no consumption of any ultra-processed item within the last 7 days) (Table 1). For traditional foods we chose a cut-off of 66th centile with scores equal to or under 66th centile (i.e. score ≤ 4.83) considered as indicating, on average, ‘infrequent consumption’ (approximately corresponding to consumption of specified items from the specified food categories, on average, 5–6 times a week or less), and scores above 66th centile considered as indicating ‘frequent consumption’ (approximately corresponding to consumption of each item 7 days a week). This centile-based approach insured that we had sufficient numbers in each group for our logistic regression models to be valid.

We then ran a series of bivariate logistic regression analyses examining associations between each independent variable and each parental feeding practice. Next, to establish which variables were associated with parental feeding practices, independently of other confounders, we ran multiple logistic regression models for each parental feeding practice using the Stepwise Forward entering method. A criterion of p ≤ 0.20 was used to identify independent variables for initial entry into these models, then, after using the Stepwise Forward enter method, only variables with p-value less than 0.05 were considered significant and retained for the final model [24]. All data were analyzed using Stata version 14.0 [25].

Results

From the total of 46 contacted schools, 14 agreed to participate in the study. We distributed 996 questionnaires and 448 (44.98%) of them were retained for analysis. Of the 548 other questionnaires, 526 were not returned, 18 had missing data on the CFPQ, and 4 had numerous incomplete answers including essential items concerning family characteristics. The final sample size was 402. A more detailed description of the excluded participants is given elsewhere [17].

Descriptive analysis (Table 1) revealed that the majority of parents were highly educated, with only 10% of the respondents classifying their education as less than college. The vast majority of questionnaires were completed by the mother of the child (93.54%) and, according to calculated BMI, mothers were mostly normal weight (76%). Parents reported greater concern about their child becoming overweight (51.62%), than about their child becoming underweight (38.65%).

Table 2 shows the results of bivariate logistic regression analyses for all independent and dependent variables. All variables with p ≤ 0.20 were deemed eligible to enter the final model, shown in Table 3. In addition to the dichotomized variables, in order to confirm that our results were not unduly influenced by our analysis choice, we also re-conducted analyses using continuous independent variables where possible (i.e. for child and mother weight, and child and mother age). Results were unchanged with the exception of associations that emerged between ‘Pressure’ and maternal BMI (OR = 0.94 (CI 95% 0.88–0.96, p = 0.034), and ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ and maternal BMI (OR = 1.10 (CI 95% 1.03–1.17, p = 0.002)). We therefore included this additional analysis in Table 2 and considered maternal BMI for the multiple logistic regression models.

Table 2 Results of bivariate logistic regression models showing relationships between each parent and child characteristics, and each parent feeding practice (n = 402)
Table 3 Results of multiple logistic regression models showing significant associations between parent and child characteristics for each parent feeding practice

Final multiple logistic regression models for each CFPQ factor, adjusted for child sex, are presented in Table 3. In analyses of negative/non-nutritive feeding practices, both ‘Restriction for Weight Control’ and ‘Restriction for Health’ were associated with lower maternal education (RWC: OR = 2.42 (CI 95% 1.07–5.48, p = 0.034), RH: OR = 2.79 (CI 95% 1.25–6.22, p = 0.013). ‘Restriction for Weight Control’ was additionally associated with greater concern about child overweight (OR = 2.46 (CI 95% 1.64–3.69, p < 0.001)). Parents that used more ‘Pressure’ were 2.3 times more likely to be concerned about child underweight and lower mother BMI (OR = 0.94 (CI 95% 0.88–1.00, p = 0.038)). Greater use of the feeding practice ‘Emotion Regulation/ Food as Reward’ was associated with lower maternal education (OR = 2.22 (CI 95% 1.05–4.71, p = 0.038)).

For positive feeding practices, lesser use of ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ was associated with infrequent consumption of traditional foods (OR = 1.53 (CI 95% 1.01–2.32, p = 0.046)), while, lesser use of ‘Monitoring’ was associated with greater consumption of ultra-processed foods (OR = 1.94 (CI 95% 1.27–2.97, p = 0.002)). The lesser use of both of these feeding practices were associated with screen time > two hours per day (HEG: OR = 1.59 (CI 95% 1.04–2.44, p = 0.034), M: OR = 1.57 (CI 95% 1.03–2.39, p = 0.036)). Finally, lower scores on ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ were associated with greater maternal BMI (OR = 1.09 (CI 95% 1.03–1.16, p = 0.004)) and lower use of ‘Monitoring’ was associated with lower perceived responsibility for child feeding (OR = 1.68 (CI 95% 1.12–2.52, p = 0.012)).

Discussion

This study of parental feeding practices in a large sample of Brazilian preschoolers revealed that a number of both positive and negative/non-nutritive feeding behaviors were significantly associated with selected parent and child characteristics, partially confirming our specified hypotheses. Below we discuss results specifically relating to our hypotheses.

For our first hypothesis, that the use of negative/non-nutritive feeding practices would be associated with lower parental educational levels, less healthy eating habits, and higher child weight status, results were partly consistent. Specifically, these practices were indeed used more often by parents with lower education levels. In fact, less educated mothers were almost 2.42 times more likely to employ ‘Restriction for Weight Control’ and almost 2.8 times likely to use ‘Restriction for Health’. This association of greater restriction with lower parental education is consistent with a longitudinal study in the US [26] and a cross-sectional study in the UK [27], and suggests that educational interventions promoting effective feeding practices may benefit from targeting mothers with lower education levels. Less educated parents also had a greater tendency to use food to regulate emotions or as reward, as has been found in other studies (use of food as reward: [28, 29]; emotional feeding [30]). Since instrumental feeding has been associated with negative nutritional outcomes in children [10, 31], attempts to curtail the use of these strategies among less educated mothers may be beneficial. The association with only mother education, not father education, may reflect that, in Brazil, the mother is still the main parent responsible for feeding and educating children at home [32]. Wealthier mothers in Brazil with a higher educational level and monthly income have more access to information and care of their children, as demonstrated in a nationally representative study [33]. Contrary to our first hypothesis, we did not find any associations between negative/non-nutritive feeding practices and less healthy eating habits in children. Notably, greater use of ‘Restriction for Weight Control’ was associated with greater child weight using bivariate logistic regression, but this effect did not remain in multiple logistic regression models including other influential variables.

Our second hypothesis, that for healthier feeding practices (‘Monitoring’, ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’), lower use would be associated with lower maternal education, less healthy eating habits and higher child weight status, was partially confirmed. Lower use of ‘Monitoring’ did not show an association with lower maternal education. However, the use of practices such as serving as a model for healthy eating and teaching about the nutritional value of foods (captured in the ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ factor) has previously been associated with parental knowledge about these topics [34] and could be emphasized in nutritional education programs. In addition, parents with lower scores on both ‘Monitoring’ and ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ reported that their children consumed fewer healthy (traditional) foods. This was also demonstrated in a recent study, which found that lower use of ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ was related to lesser intake of fruits and vegetables, as well as greater intake of unhealthy snacks and beverages [35]. It is well-established that healthy eating promoting parental practices such as modelling and providing fruits and vegetables at home – both of which were encompassed within our ‘Healthy Eating Guidance’ factor – increases preschoolers’ consumption of healthier foods [4, 6, 7, 36]. Further, in our own data, lower use of ‘Monitoring’ was associated with greater consumption of ultra-processed foods. Consistent with this finding, ‘Monitoring’ has repeatedly been positively associated with healthier outcomes, such as lower consumption of sweet drinks and non-core foods [35], and negatively associated with unhealthy eating [37]. It is possible that parents who reported higher ‘Monitoring’ levels also perceived themselves as more responsible for child feeding, suggesting a greater engagement with child nutrition [28]. Contrary to our hypotheses, we did not find any association between positive feeding practices and lower child weight. This may have been because the preschoolers in our sample had not been exposed to these beneficial strategies long enough to have an impact on their weight status. Longitudinal study designs are necessary to test for such long-term impacts. Notably, studies examining positive, rather than negative, feeding practices are scarce and our results therefore contribute to the wider literature on parent feeding by suggesting these practices may help to decrease children’s ultra-processed food intake [22]. In Brazil, to our knowledge, there are no reports of potential consequences of a variety of positive feeding behaviors among preschoolers; the present study therefore contributes unique information that could be particularly relevant for development of health interventions in Brazil.

We also found support for our third hypothesis, that parental feeding strategies would be predicted by parents’ concerns about child weight. Specifically, we found that greater concern about child overweight increased the likelihood of using ‘Restriction for Weight Control’. This is consistent with other work suggesting that parents who restrict their child’s food intake may do so in response to parental concerns about child overweight [1, 5, 38,39,40], and confirms that this relationship is also present in the Brazilian population. This is likely due to increased recognition of the dangers of overweight, and the presence of stigma associated with excessive weight, across most modern societies. According to Damiano et al. (2016), maternal lack of knowledge about parenting strategies and overevaluation of child’s body shape and weight would also be predictors in the use of restrictive feeding practices [11]. A previous study using the CFPQ also found that parental concern about child overweight was related to higher restriction of child’s eating for both weight and health reasons [41]. In addition, we found that greater parental concern about child underweight was positively associated with pressuring. Consistent with this finding, pressuring has been previously associated with both lower child weight [6, 40], and higher concern about child undereating and underweight [40]. Parental concern about their child undereating has also been associated with identification of their child as being a picky or fussy eater [1, 5]. Notably, our scale assessing concern about underweight also included one item assessing concern about undereating. Our findings are therefore consistent with a parental response interpretation whereby concern is a response to a child who eats less than the parent deems suitable, and is picky about which items he or she eats. Importantly, parental perception of a small appetite may not necessarily indicate that the child actually eats too little for optimal health; it could instead reflect difficulty on the part of parents in assessing appropriate portion sizes for preschoolers, or a lack of security regarding their child’s ability to self-regulate their food intake [5].

Finally, regarding the last hypothesis, we did not find any association between greater risk of using negative/non-nutritive feeding practices among overweight/obese mothers – instead, we found that greater pressure was associated with lower maternal weight. The former finding could be driven by mothers with lower weight being more conscious of low intake and weight in children, and, conversely, by mothers with higher weight being more aware of increased risk of overweight in their children and therefore abstaining from this practice [42]. We also found that lower ‘Healthy eating guidance’ was associated with greater maternal weight. This corroborates the results of another study finding that the lack of positive practices, of ‘Modelling’ and ‘Teaching about nutrition’ were associated with greater maternal weight [28], and suggests a potential route for familial transmission of obesity risk.

Our use of a validated measure to assess parental feeding practices in a relatively large sample of preschoolers is a strength of this study, as is our use of a Brazilian population. Only a small number of studies have thus far investigated parental feeding practices in Brazil and, since parents’ feeding practices may be influenced by cultural norms and other sociocultural factors, investigating them across different populations is essential [28, 37]. Our study also had some limitations. First, the data is cross-sectional and thus causal relations cannot be inferred, so interpretation of the results should be made with caution. Second, the sample was relatively well educated. The fact that we observed differences in negative feeding practices by education suggests that, despite this limitation, we did have sufficient variance to detect some important education-related effects. However the lack of variability in education could also have limited our ability to see significant education-related differences in positive feeding practices. Our findings may not replicate in lesser educated Brazilian families. Third, our use of sample-based median splits to dichotomize the parent feeding variables, although statistically prudent due to non-normality, does not allow us to infer practical or theoretical significance of scoring ‘high’ or ‘low’. To rigorously establish clinically-meaningful cut-offs would require reference to a much larger evidence base including longitudinal and experimental studies that could conclusively attribute negative child outcomes to certain levels of the named parent feeding strategies. The literature does not currently allow such an endeavor. Fourth, participants could have answered in a socially desirable way to questions regarding their child food intake and their behaviors – a disadvantage common to all parent-report survey research. The FFQ we used here was not validated previously. Dietary assessment in young children is challenging in general, and relies on dietary information provided by the parents, which is prone to error. Notwithstanding, FFQs have been shown to be effective at ranking children according to their dietary patterns [43]. In addition, all the obtained information was self-reported by parents, which could have led to recall errors. Anthropometric data for parents and children was reported rather than measured; this may have somewhat limited our ability to detect reliable effects of parent and child weight. However, the majority (about 70%) of the anthropometric information provided by parents was derived from pediatrician/medical reports or measured at home (data not shown), and parents of children of this age group are likely to be more aware of their child’s growth data due to contact with health professionals for checks on development. Finally, since our main focus was to analyze variables associated with and potentially predictive of parental feeding practices (which are subjective variables), self-reported weight and height information were arguably more directly relevant to the use of these practices at home.

Conclusions

To summarize, our results suggest that lower use of positive feeding practices is associated with child characteristics such as unhealthy food intake and excessive screen time, and parental lack of responsibility about child feeding. In contrast, characteristics such as parental education may influence the use of negative/non-nutritive feeding practices, which could lead to unhealthy health or weight outcomes in children. Parental concern about child weight status (under- and overweight) also predicted negative/non-nutritive feeding practices. Since preschool children are undergoing rapid evolution in their eating habits and emotional relationships with food, which may go on to predict their eating and weight gain in later childhood and adulthood, interventions informed by the risk factors we identified here may be beneficial for the Brazilian population. For example, an educational intervention highlighting potential consequences of negative/non-nutritive feeding practices and describing how to implement positive feeding practices, could be targeted at lesser educated or obese mothers. Such an intervention could help to promote healthy eating habits and prevent future nutritional disorders.

Abbreviations

BMI:

Body Mass Index

CFPQ:

Comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire

CFQ:

Child feeding questionnaire

ER/FR:

Emotion regulation/food as reward

FFQ:

Food frequency questionnaire

HEG:

Healthy eating guidance

M:

Monitoring

P:

Pressure

RH:

Restriction for Health

RWC:

Restriction for weight control

References

  1. Jansen PW, Roza SJ, Jaddoe VW, Mackenbach JD, Raat H, Hofman A, et al. Children’s eating behavior, feeding practices of parents and weight problems in early childhood: results from the population-based generation R study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:130.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler PJ. Children’s food preferences: a longitudinal analysis. J Am Diet Assoc. 2002;102:1638–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Wardle J, Roza SJ, Jaddoe VW, Mackenbach JD, Raat H, Hofman A, et al. Understanding the aetiology of childhood obesity: implications for treatment. Proc Nutr Soc. 2005;64:73–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Zeinstra GG, Koelen MA, Kok FJ, van der Laan N, de Graaf C. Parental child-feeding strategies in relation to Dutch children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Public Health Nutr. 2010;13:787–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ek A, Sorjonen K, Nyman J, Marcus C, Nowicka P. Child behaviors associated with childhood obesity and parents’ self-efficacy to handle them: confirmatory factor analysis of the lifestyle behavior checklist. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12:36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Kröller K, Warschburger P. Maternal feeding strategies and child’s food intake: considering weight and demographic influences using structural equation modeling. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2009;6:78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Blissett J, Bennett C, Fogel A, Harris G, Higgs S. Parental modelling and prompting effects on acceptance of a novel fruit in 2–4-year-old children are dependent on children’s food responsiveness. Br J Nutr. 2016;115:554–64.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Rodgers RF, McLean SA, Paxton SJ. Longitudinal relationships among internalization of the media ideal, peer social comparison, and body dissatisfaction: implications for the tripartite influence model. Dev Psychol. 2015;51:706–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kröller K, Warschburger P. Associations between maternal feeding style and food intake of children with a higher risk for overweight. Appetite. 2008;51:166–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Farrow CV, Haycraft E, Blissett JM. Teaching our children when to eat: how parental feeding practices inform the development of emotional eating--a longitudinal experimental design. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;101:908–13.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Damiano SR, Hart LM, Paxton SJ. Correlates of parental feeding practices with pre-schoolers: parental body image and eating knowledge, attitudes, and behaviours. Appetite. 2016;101:192–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Gevers DWM, Raaijmakers LGM, Bessems KMHH, Teuscher D, Kremers SPJ, van Assema P. Restrictive rules of Dutch mothers regarding their children’s dietary intake between meals. Eat Behav. 2015;18:62–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Orrell-Valente JK, Hill LG, Brechwald WA, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. “Just three more bites”: an observational analysis of parents’ socialization of children’s eating at mealtime. Appetite. 2007;48:37–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bost KK, Wiley AR, Fiese B, Hammons A, McBride B. Associations between adult attachment style, emotion regulation, and preschool Childrenʼs food consumption. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014;35:50–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Silveira JAC, Colugnati FAB, Cocetti M, Taddei JAAC, Silveira JAC, Colugnati FAB, et al. Secular trends and factors associated with overweight among Brazilian preschool children: PNSN-1989, PNDS-1996, and 2006/07. J Pediatr. 2014;90:258–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Martins APB, Levy RB, Claro RM, Moubarac JC, Monteiro CA. Participação crescente de produtos ultraprocessados na dieta brasileira (1987-2009). Rev Saúde Pública. 2013;47:656–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Warkentin S, Mais LA, Latorre MRDO, Carnell S, Taddei JAAC. Validation of the comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire in parents of preschool children in Brazil. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:603.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Musher-Eizenman D, Holub S. Comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire: validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32:960–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Mais LA, Warkentin S, Latorre MRDO, Carnell S, Taddei JAAC. Validation of the comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire among Brazilian families of school-aged children. Front Nutr. 2015;2:1–9.

  20. Birch LL, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R, Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the child feeding questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36:201–10.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Louzada MLC, Martins APB, Canella DS, Baraldi LG, Levy RB, Claro RM, et al. Ultra-processed foods and the nutritional dietary profile in Brazil. Rev Saude Publica. 2015;49:38.

  22. Mais LA, Warkentin S, Vega JB, Latorre MRDO, Carnell S, Taddei JAAC. Sociodemographic, anthropometric and behavioural risk factors for ultra-processed food consumption in a sample of 2–9-year-olds in Brazil. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21:77–86.

  23. World Health Organization. WHO | The WHO Child Growth Standards. Geneva: World Health Organization: WHO; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Kleinbaum DG, Klein M. Logistic Regression: a self-learning text. New York: Springer; 2010.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  25. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2015.

  26. Rifas-Shiman S, Sherry B, Scanlon K, Birch L, GIllman M, Taveras E. Does maternal feeding restriction lead to childhood obesity in a prospective cohort study? Arch Dis Child. 2011;96:265–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Blissett J, Haycraft E. Are parenting style and controlling feeding practices related? Appetite. 2008;50:477–85.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Musher-Eizenman DR, de Lauzon-Guillain B, Holub SC, Leporc E, Charles MA. Child and parent characteristics related to parental feeding practices. A cross-cultural examination in the US and France. Appetite. 2009;52:89–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Adnan N, Muniandy ND. The relationship between mothers’ educational level and feeding practices among children in selected kindergartens in Selangor, Malaysia: a cross-sectional study. Asian J Clin Nutr. 2012;4:39–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Saxton J, Carnell S, van Jaarsveld CHM, Wardle J. Maternal education is associated with feeding style. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:894–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Blissett J, Haycraft E, Farrow C. Inducing preschool children’s emotional eating: relations with parental feeding practices. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92:359–65.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Costa FS, Pino DL, Friedman R. Caregivers’ attitudes and practices: influence on childhood body weight. J Biosoc Sci. 2011;43:369–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Silveira JAC, Colugnati FAB, Poblacion AP, Taddei JAAC. Association between socioeconomic and biological factors and infant weight gain: Brazilian demographic and health survey – PNDS-2006/07. J Pediatr. 2015;91:284–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Hart LM, Damiano SR, Cornell C, Paxton SJ. What parents know and want to learn about healthy eating and body image in preschool children: a triangulated qualitative study with parents and early childhood professionals. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:596.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Haszard JJ, Skidmore PM, Williams SM, Taylor RW. Associations between parental feeding practices, problem food behaviours and dietary intake in New Zealand overweight children aged 4–8 years. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18:1036–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Wardle J, Carnell S, Cooke L. Parental control over feeding and children’s fruit and vegetable intake: how are they related? J Am Diet Assoc. 2005;105:227–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Arredondo EM, Elder JP, Ayala GX, Campbell N, Baquero B, Duerksen S. Is parenting style related to children’s healthy eating and physical activity in Latino families? Health Educ Res. 2006;21:862–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Birch LL, Fisher JO, Davison KK. Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;78:215–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Webber L, Cooke L, Hill C, Wardle J. Associations between children’s appetitive traits and maternal feeding practices. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110:1718–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Dinkevich E, Leid L, Pryor K, Wei Y, Huberman H, Carnell S. Mothers’ feeding behaviors in infancy: do they predict child weight trajectories? Obesity. 2015;23:2470–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. de Lauzon-Guillain B, Musher-Eizenman D, Leporc E, Holub S, Charles MA. Parental feeding practices in the United States and in France: relationships with Child’s characteristics and Parent’s eating behavior. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:1064–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kaur H, Li C, Nazir N, Choi WS, Resnicow K, Birch LL, et al. Confirmatory factor analysis of the child-feeding questionnaire among parents of adolescents. Appetite. 2006;47:36–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Jarman M, Inskip HM, Ntani G, Cooper C, Baird J, Robinson SM, et al. Influences on the diet quality of pre-school children: importance of maternal psychological characteristics. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18:2001–10.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participating schools, parents and children for their valuable contribution. We also thank the CAPES for scholarships for LAM and SW, and the Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa (CNPq) for a productivity scholarship for JAACT.

Funding

Funding for SW and LAM was provided by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES), Brasília (DF), Brazil. Funding for SC was provided by the National Institutes of Health (NIH). Funding for JAACT was provided by the Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa (CNPq).

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Data collection was performed at schools in Campinas and São Paulo, SP, Brazil. SW contributed to the study design, participated in the data gathering and entering, performed the data analysis and interpretation, and wrote the article. LAM contributed to the study design, participated in the data gathering and entering, and contributed to data analysis and interpretation, and writing the article. MRDOL suggested and supervised the data analysis and interpretation, and reviewed the article. SC contributed to conceptualization and writing of the article. JAACT selected the study design, supervised the data gathering and data entering, and reviewed the article. All authors approved the contents of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sarah Warkentin.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This research received ethical approval from the Federal University of Sao Paulo (UNIFESP) ethics committee under the protocol number 20221013.9.0000.5505. The mother or the father of the child provided written informed consent for participation prior to data collection.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Warkentin, S., Mais, L.A., Latorre, M.d.R.D.d. et al. Relationships between parent feeding behaviors and parent and child characteristics in Brazilian preschoolers: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 18, 704 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5593-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5593-4

Keywords