Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

The relative importance of work-related and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support on global perceived stress in a cross-sectional population-based sample

Abstract

Background

High levels of perceived stress have a negative bearing on health and well-being, and stress is a major public health issue. According to the Stress Process Model, stressors are socially patterned and combine to produce strain. Despite this, most studies on stress have focused on work-related stressors leaving non-work determinants under-investigated. The aim of the present study was to determine the relative importance of work-related and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support for the overall perceived stress level.

Methods

Self-reported data were drawn from the 2017 population-based health survey “How are you?” conducted in the Central Denmark Region (N = 32,417). Data were linked with data drawn from national administrative registers. Work- and non-work-related stressors assessed included major life events, chronic stressors and daily hassles. Perceived social support was assessed using a single question. Overall perceived stress was assessed by the 10-item Perceived Stress Scale. We conducted dominance analyses based on a multiple linear regression model to determine the most important explanatory variables of overall perceived stress. Analyses were weighted and adjusted.

Results

Work- and non-work-related stressors along with perceived social support explained 42.5% of the total variance (R2) in overall perceived stress. The most important explanatory variables were disease, perceived social support and work situation. The stratified analyses produced slightly varying results (“dominance profiles”) of perceived stress between subgroups. Work situation was the most important explanatory variable in the employed group. However, adding non-work-related explanatory variables to the analysis tripled the explained variance.

Conclusions

The overall level of perceived stress can be statistically explained by a combination of work- and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support both at population level and in subgroups. The most important explanatory variables of overall perceived stress are disease, perceived social support and work situation. Results indicate that public health strategies aiming to reduce stress should take a comprehensive approach and address a variety of stressor domains rather than focus on a single domain.

Trial registration

The study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency (r. no. 2012-58-0006) and registered in the Central Denmark Region (r. no. 1-16-02-593-16).

Peer Review reports

Background

Stress is a major public health issue and has long been a topic attracting much interest across a wide range of research fields [1,2,3]. A particularly active area of research has been work-related stress [2, 4, 5]. This area has predominantly been inspired by the Demand-Control-Support [6] and Effort-Reward-Imbalance [7] models, focusing on the interplay between the psychosocial work environment and workers’ mental health. With the exception of work-life balance, most research in this area has analysed the workplace as a closed system where high levels of stress among employees are linked primarily to occupational stressors thereby leaving non-work determinants under-investigated [3, 8].

In the present study, we examine the relative importance of work-related and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support for statistically explaining the overall level of perceived stress. As a theoretical framework for the study, we combine two pivotal stress research models: the psychological Transactional Stress Model [9] and the sociological Stress Process Model [10]. In doing so, we focus on stress as a mental or emotional reaction to socially patterned exposures.

We use the definition of stress by Lazarus and Folkman in which stress is a state of emotional strain and pressure arising in an individual when demands in the environment are perceived as “taxing or exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her wellbeing” [9]. Key concepts are cognitive appraisal and coping. While appraisal mechanisms (i.e., primary, secondary and re-appraisal) affect the extent to which the situation is perceived as stressful, coping process (i.e., how the individual handle the stressful situation) determine whether the stress will be alleviated or continue. High levels of perceived stress have a negative impact on health and well-being [1]. Furthermore, perceived stress increases the risk of unemployment [11], and long-term exposure to stressors is a risk factor for developing mental and physical diseases including depression [12,13,14,15] and anxiety disorders [16], substance abuse [17, 18], coronary heart disease [19, 20], type 2 diabetes [21, 22] and metabolic syndrome [23]. Consequently, high levels of perceived stress are associated with an increased risk of premature death [24].

While the Transactional Stress Model focuses on cognitive processes that occur in the interaction between the individual and the environment, the Stress Process Model introduced by Pearlin and colleagues [10] focuses on social phenomena that cause stress (termed stressors). Pearlin recognized that stressors are socially patterned and rarely act in isolation but combine in an additive or multiplicative manner to produce strain [10]. Thus, the Stress Process Model complements the Transaction Stress Model and points to social causes of stress that could potentially be changed.

The Stress Process Model has served as a paradigm for research into an expanding range of stressors [25]. Pearlin argued strongly that stress research should address the full array of stressors found in a person’s life rather than single stressors in isolation [25, 26]. There is, however, no clear definition of “the full array of stressors”, but Pearlin points towards both chronic strains and life events [26], and Wheaton adds daily hassles to the list [27]. Despite this, the understanding of the complexity of the stress process remains deficient in the empirical literature [28]. According to a review of trends in stress research, literature on specific stressors has become more abundant and research has become increasingly segmented [29]. This trend may result in a return to pre-Stress Process Model approaches addressing one stressor at a time and thus ignoring the additive or multiplicative effect of stressors.

One of the gains of the extensive sociological, psychological and epidemiological research into stressors is a conceptual division of the stress continuum into life events, chronic stressors and daily hassles [26, 27]. “Life events” are sudden, discrete occurrences such as the death of a near relative or being laid off from work unexpectedly. “Chronic stressors” are enduring strains such as discrimination, permanently compromised health or conflicting demands of multiple social roles. “Daily hassles” include episodic – expected or unexpected – minor events which, as they accumulate, may constitute stressors such as a parent and a child struggling over bedtime or delays in morning traffic. In addition, these very diverse stressors vary in respect to duration, severity and the context in which they occur. It is acknowledged that these different types of stressors all contribute to the overall burden of stress in the population [27]. Thus, the measures used for exposure to stressors should tap into a broad spectrum from discrete life events to chronic stressors and daily hassles. The consequences of chronic or recurring stressors may be particularly severe when they surface within major social domains, such as breadwinning, job and family [30]. A better understanding of domain-specific stressors and their relative contribution to the overall perceived burden of stress is important for effective public health strategies [31, 32]. Prevention and reduction of stress at population level and in target groups facing special needs should be based on empirical knowledge of the main sources of stress.

The other major component of the Stress Process Model is resources available to cope with the challenges experienced [33]. Central to this component is access to social support from one’s surroundings. In particular, empirical research has established that it is not the actual availability of support that promotes coping but its perceived availability [28, 34]. It has been theorised that social support buffers the effects of stressors [10, 26, 35]. However, a review indicates that the stress-buffering effects of social support are “less dramatic and consistent” than the direct effects of social support on mental health [36]. This underscores the relevance of investigating the direct effect of perceived social support on the overall stress burden on par with stressors within various social domains.

In this study, we return to a comprehensive approach to stress suggested by Pearlin [30] by investigating the relative importance of perceived stressors occurring within major social domains and perceived social support as explanatory variables of perceived stress in a general adult population.

Aim

The aim of the study was to determine the relative importance of work-related and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support as explanatory variables of the overall perceived stress level. The study examines perceived stressors and perceived social support at population level and in socio-demographic subgroups in order to generate knowledge for public health strategies.

Methods

Study design and data collection

Self-reported data were drawn from the 2017 population-based health survey “How are you?” conducted in the Central Denmark Region. In 2017, approximately 23% of the Danish population were living in the Central Denmark Region, and the demographic composition (gender, age and educational attainment) of the population was similar to that of the total Danish population [37]. The survey included a representative population sample of 52,000 citizens aged 16 years and above drawn from the Danish Civil Registration System [38]. Participants were invited to complete a web-based or postal questionnaire [39]. Those who failed to respond to the survey received up to four reminders. The response rate was 62% (32,417), and 80% of the respondents completed the web questionnaire. The study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency (r. no. 2012-58-0006) and the Central Denmark Region (r. no. 1–16–02-593-16). Each participant received written information about the purpose of the survey, and informed consent was obtained from all subjects. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations along with the approval. Data from the survey were linked with national administrative registers using the unique personal identification number assigned to all Danish citizens [38]. Register data included age, gender, ethnic background and labour market status.

Variables

Outcome (Perceived Stress Scale) and exposure (perceived stressors and perceived social support) were selected according to the Transactional Stress Model in which a person’s assessment of a potential stressor as benign, neutral or threatening serves as the link between loads and demands in the environment and the emotional strain experienced by the person [9]. The selected stressors include a wide range of life domains in order to examine how much each contributes to the global stress level. Furthermore, in the Stress Process Model, stressors and social support constitute the major components causing stress [10].

Perceived stress scale

The level of perceived stress was assessed by the 10-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) [40]. Based on Lazarus’ stress model [41, 42], the PSS is a global measure of stress in which stress relies upon the person’s perception of the stressor as stressful or not. The ten items ask how often in the past month life was appraised as unpredictable, uncontrollable and overloaded (e.g., “In the last month, how often have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly?”) [40]. The items were scored from 0 to 4 (“never”, “almost never”, “sometimes”, “fairly often” or “very often”). An increasing sum score (range 0 to 40) indicates an increasing perceived stress level [40]. PSS does not have cut-off values for determining high or low levels of perceived stress [40]. However, a number of studies have proposed cut-off values ranging from 15 to 18 for high levels of perceived stress [11, 24, 43, 44]. PSS has satisfying psychometric properties [45, 46] and has been used in a number of population-based studies (e.g. [11, 43, 47,48,49]). In the present study, Cronbach’s α indicated that the scale had a high internal consistency (α = 0.87).

Perceived stressors

Work-related and non-work-related perceived stressors were assessed using nine questions covering seven categories from the Life Event Questionnaire [50]. The selection of questions was inspired by The Danish National Birth Cohort [51,52,53]. The questions cover major life events, chronic stressors and daily hassles. Perceived stressors were assessed asking “Within the past 12 months, have you felt burdened by some of the following things?” The questionnaire covers work situation, financial circumstances, housing conditions, relationship with partner, relationship with family and friends, own disease, disease among close relatives, deaths among close relatives and other types of burdens. The response categories were “no”, “yes, a little”, “yes, partly” or “yes, a lot”. In the present study, all four response categories of each variable were used in the analyses.

The survey also includes a more elaborate questionnaire on work that covers quantitative demands, job influence, satisfaction and recognition, work-life conflicts, physical and emotional wear and tear, and physical load [54]. However, a previous study found that including specific aspects of psychosocial work conditions did not provide additional information than a single global question [55]. In line with this, preliminary analyses using the more elaborate questionnaire did not alter our results. Thus, in order to keep the model simple, the single question about work situation was preferred.

Perceived social support

Perceived social support was assessed using a single item: “Do you have anyone to talk to if you have problems or need support?” The response format was “yes, always”, “yes, mostly”, “yes, sometimes” or “no, never or almost never”. In the present study, all four response categories of each variable were used in the analyses. The question was inspired by The MOS Social Support Instrument [56].

Socio-demographic variables

Age and gender were assessed using a combination of self-reported and register data. Ethnic background was defined using the Danish Civil Registration System [38]. Educational attainment was self-reported and categorised as low (primary school, no further education), medium (upper secondary education, vocational education and/or short higher education) or high (bachelor’s degree or higher level of education) according to the Danish version of the International Standard Classification of Education [57]. Students were categorised according to their expected graduation level. Labour market status was assed using a combination of self-reported data and data from the Danish Register for Evaluation of Marginalization [58].

Data analysis

We conducted dominance analysis, a relatively new computer-intensive method, to determine the most important explanatory variables of perceived stress in the population and in stratified analyses (i.e., gender, age, educational attainment and labour market status) [59, 60]. The analyses were based on a multiple linear regression model with PSS as the dependent variable and the nine different stressors and perceived social support as explanatory variables. Dominance analysis is a method of investigating importance and ranking of explanatory variables (“dominance profiles”) according to how much each variable contributes to the total variance of the dependent variable in a model [59]. The method is particularly suitable when explanatory variables are intercorrelated, as may be expected in this study. Dominance analysis is an ensemble method based on estimation of all possible regression models (all subset regressions) [61]. The average increase in total variance explained by the model (R2) when adding a variable to all possible sub-models quantifies the importance of the explanatory variable. Dominance analysis is used for decomposition of the total R2, but it has rarely been used in stress research [62]. The dominance analyses in the present study consisted of 1023 (2^10–1) regression models containing all possible combinations of explanatory variables. In addition to the ten explanatory variables, gender, age, ethnic background and educational attainment were included in all models.

Perceived stress and the perceived stressors and social support are collected at the same time, and are likely to affect each other. This is foreseen since stressors are socially patterned and combine to produce strain [10]. This does not pose a problem, since we use dominance analysis to determine the relative importance of perceived stressors and perceived social support as explanatory variables of PSS [60].

To reduce sampling and non-response bias, weights constructed by Statistics Denmark using a model-based calibration approach and including socio-demographic characteristics, income, social benefits and healthcare utilisation were applied [39, 63].

Prior to the analyses, data were screened for missing values. The percentage of missing values was acceptable (0–13%). Missing PSS items ranged from 4.3% (item 1) to 5.1% (item 8). If one, two or three items of the PSS scale were missing, the mean of the available items was used to calculate the scale score [64]. If responses to more than three items were missing, the PSS score was regarded as missing. Hence, 1392 observations (4.3%) were excluded.

For stressors, missing values were treated as “no” if respondents had answered at least one of the nine stressor questions. Hence, 26,799 respondents (83%) had complete stressor data, 3462 respondents (11%) had partially complete stressor data and 2156 respondents (7%) were missing. A similar approach was adopted in the “How are you?” health survey with regards to measures of chronic diseases [65].

PSS was obtained in 95.7% of the respondent (Table 1), and stressors were obtained in 93.3% of the respondents (Table 2). Respondents with missing PSS or stressors were more likely to be 16–24 years old, have an ethnic background other than Danish and an unknown educational attainment or work situation (results not shown).

Table 1 Characteristics of respondents with perceived stress scale (PSS) score (N = 31,025)
Table 2 Prevalence of stressors and perceived social support (N = 30,261)

Stata/SE v16.1 (StataCorp, College Station, TX) was used to prepare the data and perform the descriptive and inferential analyses including dominance analysis using the community-contributed extension DOMIN [66].

Results

Descriptive analyses

The mean perceived stress level in the population was 12.2 with a range from 0 to 40 (Table 1). The highest stress levels were found among women, 16–24-year-olds, respondents with an ethnic background other than Danish, respondents with a low educational attainment and respondents who were students or outside the labour marked (unemployed, receiving cash or sickness benefits, or receiving a disability pension). The three groups outside the labour market had the highest perceived stress levels of all subgroups.

The prevalence of stressors varied from 42.2% having been burdened by disease during the past 12 months to 13.0% having experienced other types of burdens (Table 2). Absence of perceived social support was found in 4.9%. For all stressors, the mean PSS increased with increasing intensity of the stressor. However, for perceived social support those who never or almost never had social support did not report a higher intensity of the stressor than those who sometimes had social support.

Main findings

Work- and non-work-related stressors along with perceived social support explained 42.5% of the variance in overall perceived stress level in the adjusted analysis (Table 3 and Fig. 1). The three explanatory variables explaining most of the variance in PSS were disease (9.5%), perceived social support (5.9%) and work situation (4.9%) (Table 3 and Fig. 1). Adding the non-work-related stressors and perceived social support to the work-related stressor increased the explained variance from 12.1% (work situation 4.9% and sociodemographic characteristics 7.2%) to 42.5%.

Table 3 Unadjusted and adjusted dominance analyses (“dominance profiles”) for all respondents and subgroups stratified by gender, age, educational attainment and employment status (N = 29,860). Explanatory variables contributing with more than 10% of the adjusted R2 are stated in bold
Fig. 1
figure 1

Adjusted dominance analyses (“dominance profiles”) for all respondents (N = 29,541). Rank of stressors and social support based on average increase in total variance explained by the model (R2) when adding a variable to all possible sub-models. Contribution to total variance is indicated next to the bar in absolute and relative (in parentheses) terms. Sociodemographic characteristics were included in all models

Stratified analyses

The explanatory variables explained 41.9 and 42.1% of the variance in PSS in men and women, respectively (Table 3). In both men and women, the most important explanatory variables were disease, perceived social support and work situation. In the analyses stratified by age group, the explanatory variables explained 42.8, 44.8 and 34.0% of the variance in PSS, respectively (Table 3). In the young age group (16–24 years), the most important explanatory variables were disease, relationship with family and friends and perceived social support. In the middle-aged group (25–64 years), the most important explanatory variables were disease, work situation and perceived social support. In the elderly group (65 years and above), the most important explanatory variables were disease and perceived social support.

In the analyses stratified by educational attainment, the explanatory variables explained 38.7% of the variance in PSS in the low, 42.6% in the medium and 41.9% in the high educational groups (Table 3). In the low educational attainment group, the most important explanatory variables were disease and perceived social support. In the medium educational attainment group, the most important explanatory variables were disease, perceived social support and work situation. Finally, in the high educational attainment group, the most important explanatory variables were disease, work situation and perceived social support.

In the analyses stratified by employment status, the explanatory variables explained 41.6% for students, 38.4% for employed, 46.1% for unemployed, 51.8% for respondents receiving cash or sickness benefits, 42.6% for respondents receiving disability pension, 43.9% for respondents receiving early retirement pension and 33.3% for respondents receiving retirement pension (Table 3). The top three explanatory variables included disease in all seven subgroups and perceived social support in five subgroups. Among students, the most important explanatory variables were disease, perceived social support, relationship with family and friends and financial circumstances. Among employed respondents, the most important explanatory variables were work situation, perceived social support and disease. Among unemployed respondents, it was disease, work situation, financial circumstances and relationship with family and friends. Among those receiving cash or sickness benefits, it was disease, relationship with family and friends and financial circumstances. Among those receiving disability pensions, it was disease, perceived social support and relationship with family and friends. Furthermore, among those receiving early retirement pensions it was disease, perceived social support and relationship with family and friends. Finally, among those receiving retirement pensions, it was disease and perceived social support.

Discussion

Main findings and stratified analyses

We applied a comprehensive approach to stress, investigating the relative importance of work- and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support on overall perceived stress in a representative adult population and in sociodemographic subgroups. As recommended in a number of previous studies [3, 8, 11, 31, 32], we investigated both work- and non-work-related explanatory variables of stress. Most noteworthy, the overall perceived stress level was statistically explained by a combination of work- and non-work-related perceived stressors and perceived social support. The most important explanatory variables of perceived stress were disease, perceived social support and work situation. In fact, including non-work-related stressors and perceived social support along with work-related stressors more than tripled the explained variance in perceived stress compared with work-related stressors alone.

The stratified analyses (including different subgroups) resulted in slightly varying “dominance profiles” of perceived stress. However, disease was the most important explanatory variable of perceived stress in 14 of the 15 subgroups. Perceived social support was among the top three explanatory variables in 13 subgroups, and work situation was among the top three explanatory variables in seven subgroups (only relevant in eight subgroups). Other important explanatory variables in the stratified analyses were relationship with family and friends, and financial circumstances. Work situation was the single most important explanatory variable in the employed group, and the second most important explanatory variable in the unemployed group. However, even in the employed group adding non-work-related stressors and perceived social support tripled the explained variance.

Importantly, although a specific stressor explains only a small part of the overall variation in stress level, it may well play an important role in combination with other stressors [32]. This may also hold true for stressors with low contribution to the explained variance in the dominance analyses since stressors rarely occur in isolation. The combined effect of several stressors can therefore have a negative effect on the stress level. In addition, a stressor can be part of a chain of stressors triggered by a primary stressor (stress proliferation), or there may be a spill over of stressors across roles or life domains (conflicts between work and family, etc.) [10], which also demonstrates the importance of examining multiple stressors simultaneously.

Our results are in accordance with the Stress Process Model, which finds that stressors combine to produce strain [10], and with previous studies reporting that combinations of stressors from both work and non-work domains contribute to mental health [3, 8] and perceived stress in particular [11, 31, 32, 67, 68]. Our results add important knowledge to the vast majority of studies on stress that have focused on different aspects of the work situation [2, 5, 13, 20, 62, 69, 70]. A review concluded that since the majority of studies do not include non-work domains, they may overlook the contribution from non-work predictors [3]. Furthermore, Marchand and colleagues concluded that including both work and non-work domains is necessary to avoid erroneous conclusions about the relationship between work and mental health [8]. For example, recent years have seen an increased focus on the implications of managing work and family caregiving roles (child caregiving, elder caregiving or “sandwiched” caregiving) with a special focus on gender differences [71]. In line herewith, our results – both at population level and in subgroups – indicate that in order to understand the complex interaction of stressors, both non-work-related stressors and perceived social support should be addressed along with the work situation. Thus, our results are in favour of a comprehensive approach to understanding the relationship between perceived stress, work- and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support, and therefore also of developing public health strategies and interventions aiming at reducing perceived stress at the population level and in high-risk groups.

Explanatory variables

The three most important explanatory variables of perceived stress in this study were disease, perceived social support and work situation. An American study of older adults identified loneliness, neighbourhood and financial strain as particularly important predictors of perceived stress [31]. Different groups of respondents may have different “dominance profiles”of perceived stress, which is indicated in our study.

Disease as a stressor has received far less attention than stress as a risk factor for developing diseases. Even so, an understanding of the pathway from disease to stress is beginning to emerge [72]. This pathway includes loss of control and uncertainty leading to a feeling of threat, which may lead to hopelessness and stress if coping mechanisms are inadequate. This may be the reason why disease is the most important explanatory variable of overall perceived stress in our study. Four pathways from chronic disease to stress have been suggested [72]: 1) Disease often causes pain, reduces physical and mental functioning and can be life threatening, making it difficult to meet the demands of everyday life. 2) The process of diagnosis and treatment in itself gives rise to concern and uncertainty and can be both financially burdensome and time consuming. 3) Patients may have concerns whether possible harms outweigh potential benefits of treatment and may experience adverse drug interactions if treated for multiple conditions. 4) Treatment can disrupt social roles and relationships and cause lower self-efficacy and self-worth, thereby affecting mental well-being negatively.

Lack of social support is a well-known predictor of poor mental health [28, 33, 34, 73]. Non-work social support can come from a number of sources including a partner, close relatives, friends, neighbours, etc. A study of workplace-perceived social support finds that job performance was strengthened by perceived supervisor and co-worker support [74]. Our results are in line with a Danish study reporting that a low level of private-life social support increases the risk of symptoms of depression [73] and a review concluding that social support is associated with mental health [34]. Even though we do not distinguish between work- and non-work-related social support, our results are in accordance with a recent meta-analysis showing that a low level of support at work increases the risk of stress-related mental disorders [2]. However, it remains to be elucidated how social support produces positive mental health outcomes [36].

The work situation is a well-known stressor [2, 4, 5], which is also confirmed in our study. Different aspects of the work situation have been identified as predictors of stress, e.g. effort-reward imbalance, high job demands, organisational justice, social support, emotional demands and decision authority [2]. A study reports that stress from work-related strain may be transferred from parents to children [75]. This may also hold true for transfer to one’s partner.

Although The Stress Process Model has served as a paradigm for stressor research since the 1980s, a lack of understanding remains of how work- and non-work-related stressors, coping, social support and personal resources interact with each other [25]. Gaining a deeper understanding of this interaction is mandatory to develop effective strategies against stress encompassing people’s entire life situation [31, 32]. An American study found that similar combinations of chronic stressors and life events may result in different levels of perceived stress (multifinality) and that different combinations of stressors may produce similar levels of perceived stress (equifinality) [32].

Strengths and limitations

The present study has several strengths. We use a combination of self-reported and register data from a representative population sample, the sample size is large and the response rate was reasonably high. Register data are of high validity and accuracy. Overall perceived stress was assessed using the PSS, which has satisfying psychometric properties. Explanatory variables of perceived stress covered a wide range of sources. Furthermore, the dominance analysis allows us to rank the importance and contribution of explanatory variables and eases the interpretation of several regression coefficients simultaneously [61, 76]. Finally, data are weighted, and analyses are adjusted for a number of factors, which is expected to reduce residual confounding.

The study also has limitations. Although the study has a theoretical framing, it is not possible to draw causal conclusions from its results due to the cross-sectional nature of the data. Instead, we investigate the ability to statistically explain the variation in perceived stress levels using perceived stressors and perceived social support. As such, the study can be seen as part of a more comprehensive research effort to develop and refine explanatory models that can ultimately be tested in a causal setup (cf. the distinction between explanatory and predictive modelling presented by Shmueli [77]). In this study we focused on perceived stressors within nine life domains. Although comprehensive, our model does not include several areas of central importance to human existence and relevant to our understanding of stress and health. Examples of such areas are rest and restitution, loneliness, sleep, nutrition, physical activity and sexual relationships. We therefore need to develop richer, more complex models to capture even more aspects of the stress process.

The major risk with conducting a dominance analysis is not identifying and using the correct model [60]. Furthermore, omission of variables that belong in the model can bias comparisons. In this study both the model and the variables are carefully chosen. The dominance analysis is based on a multiple linear regression model, and the explanatory variables are chosen according to our theoretical framework.

Non-respondents are more likely to be 16–24-years and of an ethnic background other that Danish. Therefore, non-response bias cannot be excluded. Perceived stress, stressors and social support are assessed within different time frames, which may weaken the variance explained by the explanatory variables. This may partly explain why the dominance analyses explain only around 40% of the total variance. In the 16–24-year-old group, educational level is based on assumptions about future educational level resulting in a larger proportion having a low and medium-high educational level compared with the 25–64-year-old group.

Conclusions

The results of this population-based study demonstrate that the overall perceived stress level can be statistically explained by a combination of work- and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support. This is the case both at population level and in stratified subgroups. The three most important explanatory variables of perceived stress in the dominance analysis are disease, perceived social support and work situation. Hence, the study indicates that we should address a variety of stressor domains rather than focus on a single domain like work-related stressors. Thus, our results point towards a return to a comprehensive approach to stress as suggested by Pearlin and colleagues [10].

Work situation is the most important explanatory variable of perceived stress in the employed group. However, even in this group, adding the non-work-related explanatory variables increased the explained variance noticeably, suggesting that work-related stressors only capture part of the total amount of stressors affecting perceived stress. Thus, even among those employed, stress preventions should not focus solely on work-related stressors.

This study provides novel insight into the determinants of overall perceived stress level applicable to public health strategies aiming to reduce stress both at population level and in high-risk subgroups. Stratified subgroup analyses revealed varying “dominance profiles” profiles suggesting a need for target group-specific stress reduction strategies.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analysed during the present study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. O’Connor DB, Thayer JF, Vedhara K. Stress and health: a review of psychobiological processes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2020;72:663–88.

  2. van der Molen HF, Nieuwenhuijsen K, Frings-Dresen MHW, de Groene G. Work-related psychosocial risk factors for stress-related mental disorders: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2020;10(7):e034849. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2019-034849.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Beauregard N, Marchand A, Blanc M-E. What do we know about the non-work determinants of workers’ mental health? A systematic review of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):439. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-11-439.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Limonic L, Lennon MC. Work and unemployment as stressors. In: Wright ER, Scheid TL, editors. A handbook for the study of mental health: social contexts, theories, and systems. 3rd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2017. p. 224–38. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316471289.015.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  5. Dewe P, Cooper C. Work and stress: a research overview. London: Routledge; 2020. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429331015.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  6. Karasek R, Theorell T. Healthy work: stress, productivity and the reconstruction of working life. New York: Basic Books; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Siegrist J. Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward conditions. J Occup Health Psychol. 1996;1(1):27–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.1.1.27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Marchand A, Durand P, Haines V 3rd, Harvey S. The multilevel determinants of workers’ mental health: results from the SALVEO study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2015;50(3):445–59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-014-0932-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping: springer publishing company; 1984.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Pearlin LI, Menaghan EG, Lieberman MA, Mullan JT. The stress process. J Health Soc Behav. 1981;22(4):337–56. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136676.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Maehlisen MH, Pasgaard AA, Mortensen RN, Vardinghus-Nielsen H, Torp-Pedersen C, Boggild H. Perceived stress as a risk factor of unemployment: a register-based cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):728. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-5618-z.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Liu RT, Alloy LB. Stress generation in depression: a systematic review of the empirical literature and recommendations for future study. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(5):582–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Bonde JPE. Psychosocial factors at work and risk of depression: a systematic review of the epidemiological evidence. Occup Environ Med. 2008;65(7):438–45. https://doi.org/10.1136/oem.2007.038430.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA. Stressful life events and major depression: risk period, long-term contextual threat and diagnostic specificity. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1998;186(11):661–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-199811000-00001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Assari S, Lankarani MM. Stressful life events and risk of depression 25 years later: race and gender differences. Front Public Health. 2016;4:49.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Niedhammer I, Malard L, Chastang J-F. Occupational factors and subsequent major depressive and generalized anxiety disorders in the prospective French national SIP study. BMC Public Health. 2015;15(1):200. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-1559-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Spencer RL, Hutchison KE. Alcohol, aging, and the stress response. Alcohol Res Health. 1999;23(4):272–83.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Keyes KM, Hatzenbuehler ML, Grant BF, Hasin DS. Stress and alcohol: epidemiologic evidence. Alcohol Res. 2012;34(4):391–400.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Steptoe A, Kivimäki M. Stress and cardiovascular disease: an update on current knowledge. Annu Rev Public Health. 2013;34(1):337–54. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031912-114452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sara JD, Prasad M, Eleid MF, Zhang M, Widmer RJ, Lerman A. Association between work-related stress and coronary heart disease: a review of prospective studies through the job strain, effort-reward balance, and organizational justice models. J Am Heart Assoc. 2018;7(9):e008073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Kelly SJ, Ismail M. Stress and type 2 diabetes: a review of how stress contributes to the development of type 2 diabetes. Annu Rev Public Health. 2015;36(1):441–62. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031914-122921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Harris M, Oldmeadow C, Hure A, Luu J, Loxton D, Attia J. Stress increases the risk of type 2 diabetes onset in women: a 12-year longitudinal study using causal modelling. PLoS One. 2017;12(2):e0172126. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172126.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Kou W-c, Bratzke LC, Oakley LD, Kuo F, Wang H, Brown RL. The association between psychological stress and metabolic syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2019;20(11):1651–64.

  24. Prior A, Fenger-Grøn M, Larsen KK, Larsen FB, Robinson KM, Nielsen MG, Christensen KS, Mercer SW, Vestergaard M. The association between perceived stress and mortality among people with multimorbidity: a prospective population-based cohort study. Am J Epidemiol. 2016;184(3):199–210. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwv324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Aneshensel CS. Sociological inquiry into mental health: the legacy of Leonard I. Pearlin. J Health Soc Behav. 2015;56(2):166–78. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022146515583992.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Pearlin LI. The sociological study of stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1989;30(3):241–56. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136956.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Wheaton B, Montazer S. Stressors, stress, and distress. In: Scheid TL, Brown TN, editors. A handbook for the study of mental health: social contexts, theories, and systems. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2009. p. 171–99. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511984945.013.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  28. Brown LL, Mitchell UA, Ailshire JA. Disentangling the stress process: race/ethnic differences in the exposure and appraisal of chronic stressors among older adults. J Gerontol B. 2018;75(3):650–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Wheaton B, Young M, Montazer S, Stuart-Lahman K. Social stress in the twenty-first century. Handbook of the sociology of mental health. In: Handbooks of sociology and social research. 2nd ed. New York: Springer Science + Business Media; 2013. p. 299–323.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Pearlin LI, Schieman S, Fazio EM, Meersman SC. Stress, health, and the life course: some conceptual perspectives. J Health Soc Behav. 2005;46(2):205–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/002214650504600206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Scott SB, Jackson BR, Bergeman CS. What contributes to perceived stress in later life? A recursive partitioning approach. Psychol Aging. 2011;26(4):830–43. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023180.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Scott SB, Whitehead BR, Bergeman CS, Pitzer L. Combinations of stressors in midlife: examining role and domain stressors using regression trees and random forests. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2013;68(3):464–75. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs166.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Glanz K, Schwartz MD. Stress, coping, and health behavior. In: Health behavior and health education: theory, research, and practice. 4th ed. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2008. p. 211–36.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Brown RL, Ciciurkaite G. Understanding the connection between social support and mental health. In: Wright ER, Scheid TL, editors. A handbook for the study of mental health: social contexts, theories, and systems. 3rd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2017. p. 207–23. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316471289.014.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  35. Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985;98(2):310–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Thoits PA. Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. J Health Soc Behav. 2011;52(2):145–61. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022146510395592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Statistics Denmark. FOLK1 - Population 2017. Available from: https://www.statistikbanken.dk/statbank5a/default.asp?w=1920. Accessed 22 Sept 2020.

  38. Pedersen CB. The Danish civil registration system. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):22–5. https://doi.org/10.1177/1403494810387965.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Christensen AI, Lau CJ, Kristensen PL, Johnsen SB, Wingstrand A, Friis K, et al. The Danish National Health Survey: Study design, response rate and respondent characteristics in 2010, 2013 and 2017. Scand J Public Health. 2020:1403494820966534. https://doi.org/10.1177/1403494820966534.

  40. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385–96. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Lazarus RS. Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Lazarus RS. Psychological stress and coping in adaption and illness. In: Lipowski ZJ, Lipsitt DR, Whybrow PC, editors. Psychosomatic medicine: current trends and clinical applications. New York: Oxford Univ. Press; 1977. p. 14–26.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Pedersen AB, Baggesen LM, Ehrenstein V, Pedersen L, Lasgaard M, Mikkelsen EM. Perceived stress and risk of any osteoporotic fracture. Osteoporos Int. 2016;27(6):2035–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00198-016-3490-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Christiansen J, Larsen FB, Lasgaard M. Do stress, health behavior, and sleep mediate the association between loneliness and adverse health conditions among older people? Soc Sci Med. 2016;152:80–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.01.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Lee EH. Review of the psychometric evidence of the perceived stress scale. Asian Nurs Res (Korean Soc Nurs Sci). 2012;6(4):121–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anr.2012.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Nielsen MG, Ornbol E, Vestergaard M, Bech P, Larsen FB, Lasgaard M, et al. The construct validity of the perceived stress scale. J Psychosom Res. 2016;84:22–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2016.03.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Stubbs B, Vancampfort D, Veronese N, Schofield P, Lin PY, Tseng PT, Solmi M, Thompson T, Carvalho AF, Koyanagi A. Multimorbidity and perceived stress: a population-based cross-sectional study among older adults across six low- and middle-income countries. Maturitas. 2018;107:84–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.maturitas.2017.10.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Graff S, Prior A, Fenger-Gron M, Christensen B, Glumer C, Larsen FB, et al. Does perceived stress increase the risk of atrial fibrillation? A population-based cohort study in Denmark. Am Heart J. 2017;188:26–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ahj.2017.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Gawlik KS, Melnyk BM, Tan A. Associations between stress and cardiovascular disease risk factors among million hearts priority populations. Am J Health Promot. 2019;33(7):1063–6. https://doi.org/10.1177/0890117119847619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Norbeck JS. Modification of life event questionnaires for use with female respondents. Res Nurs Health. 1984;7(1):61–71. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.4770070110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Sondergaard C, Olsen J, Friis-Hasche E, Dirdal M, Thrane N, Sorensen HT. Psychosocial distress during pregnancy and the risk of infantile colic: a follow-up study. Acta Paediatr. 2003;92(7):811–6. https://doi.org/10.1080/08035250310003857.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Tegethoff M, Greene N, Olsen J, Meyer AH, Meinlschmidt G. Maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy and placenta weight: evidence from a national cohort study. PLoS One. 2010;5(12):e14478.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Tegethoff M, Greene N, Olsen J, Schaffner E, Meinlschmidt G. Stress during pregnancy and offspring pediatric disease: a national cohort study. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119(11):1647–52. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.1003253.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Kristensen TS, Hannerz H, Høgh A, Borg V. The Copenhagen Psychosocial Questionnaire--a tool for the assessment and improvement of the psychosocial work environment. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2005;31(6):438–49. https://doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.948.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Munch-Hansen T, Wieclaw J, Agerbo E, Westergaard-Nielsen N, Bonde JP. Global measure of satisfaction with psychosocial work conditions versus measures of specific aspects of psychosocial work conditions in explaining sickness absence. BMC Public Health. 2008;8(1):270. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-8-270.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Sherbourne CD, Stewart AL. The MOS social support survey. Soc Sci Med. 1991;32(6):705–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(91)90150-B.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Eurostat. International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) 2018 [updated 28-08-2018]. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/International_Standard_Classification_of_Education_(ISCED). Accessed 22 Sept 2020.

  58. Hjollund NH, Larsen FB, Andersen JH. Register-based follow-up of social benefits and other transfer payments: accuracy and degree of completeness in a Danish interdepartmental administrative database compared with a population-based survey. Scand J Public Health. 2007;35(5):497–502. https://doi.org/10.1080/14034940701271882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Azen R, Budescu D. The dominance analysis approach for comparing predictors in multiple regression. Psychol Methods. 2003;8(2):129–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.8.2.129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Budescu D. Dominance analysis: a new approach to the problem of relative importance of predictors in multiple regression. Psychol Bull. 1993;114(3):542–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Luchman J. Determining subgroup difference importance with complex survey designs: an application of weighted dominance analysis. Surv Pract. 2015;8(5):1–10. https://doi.org/10.29115/SP-2015-0022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Sun J-W, Bai H-Y, Li J-H, Lin P-Z, Zhang H-H, Cao F-L. Predictors of occupational burnout among nurses: a dominance analysis of job stressors. J Clin Nurs. 2017;26(23–24):4286–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.13754.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Särndal C-E, Lundström S. Estimation in survey with nonresponse; 2005. https://doi.org/10.1002/0470011351.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  64. Hawthorne G, Hawthorne G, Elliott P. Imputing cross-sectional missing data: comparison of common techniques. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39(7):583–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01630.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Larsen FB, Pedersen MH, Friis K, Glumer C, Lasgaard M. A latent class analysis of multimorbidity and the relationship to socio-demographic factors and health-related quality of life. A national population-based study of 162,283 Danish adults. PLoS One. 2017;12(1):e0169426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Luchman JN. DOMIN: Stata module to conduct dominance analysis. Statistical Software Components S457629, Boston College Department of Economics, revised 19 Feb 2021. 2013. https://ideas.repec.org/c/boc/bocode/s457629.html.

  67. Evans D, Mallet L, Flahault A, Cothereau C, Velázquez S, Capron L, et al. The importance of both workplace and private life factors in psychological distress: a large cross-sectional survey of French railway company employees. Soc Psychiatry Psych Epidemiol. 2012;48:1211–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Hasselberg K, Jonsdottir IH, Ellbin S, Skagert K. Self-reported stressors among patients with exhaustion disorder: an exploratory study of patient records. BMC Psychiatry. 2014;14(1):66. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-14-66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Nilsen C, Andel R, Fritzell J, Kåreholt I. Work-related stress in midlife and all-cause mortality: can sense of coherence modify this association? Eur J Pub Health. 2016;26(6):1055–61. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckw086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Pandey A, Quick JC, Rossi AM, Nelson DL, Martin W. 11 stress and the workplace: 10 years of science, 1997–2007. In: The handbook of stress science: biology, psychology, and health; 2010. p. 137.

    Google Scholar 

  71. DePasquale N, Polenick CA, Davis KD, Moen P, Hammer LB, Almeida DM. The psychosocial implications of managing work and family caregiving roles: gender differences among information technology professionals. J Fam Issues. 2017;38(11):1495–519. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513X15584680.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Garrido MM, Hash-Converse JM, Leventhal H, Leventhal EA. Stress and chronic disease management. In: The handbook of stress science: biology, psychology, and health; 2011. p. 487–500.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Madsen IE, Jorgensen AF, Borritz M, Nielsen ML, Rugulies R. Is the association between high strain work and depressive symptoms modified by private life social support: a cohort study of 1,074 Danish employees? BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):698. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-698.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Kim HJ, Hur W-M, Moon T-W, Jun J-K. Is all support equal? The moderating effects of supervisor, coworker, and organizational support on the link between emotional labor and job performance. BRQ Bus Res Q. 2017;20(2):124–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brq.2016.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Almeida DM, Davis KD. Workplace flexibility and daily stress processes in hotel employees and their children. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci. 2011;638(1):123–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716211415608.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Yu TC, Zhou H, Suh K, Arcona S. Assessing the importance of predictors in unplanned hospital readmissions for chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Clinicoecon Outcomes Res. 2015;7:37–51. https://doi.org/10.2147/CEOR.S74181.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Shmueli G. To explain or to predict? Stat Sci. 2010;25(3):289–310.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The “How are you?” health survey was funded by the Central Denmark Region. The present study (based on secondary data analysis) was funded by the Velliv Foreningen (grant number 19–0176).

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

FBL and JBS conceptualized the study, acquired funding, performed analyses and authored the manuscript. ML and MVW contributed to the manuscript, and read and approved the final manuscript. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jes Bak Sørensen.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency (r. no. 2012-58-0006) and registered in the Central Denmark Region (r. no. 1-16-02-593-16).

Each participant received written information about the purpose of the survey, and informed consent was obtained from all subjects. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations along with the approval.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Correlation table.

Pairwise correlation between perceived stressors, perceived social support and perceived stress (PSS) (*significance level p = 0.01, Bonferroni adjusted).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sørensen, J.B., Lasgaard, M., Willert, M.V. et al. The relative importance of work-related and non-work-related stressors and perceived social support on global perceived stress in a cross-sectional population-based sample. BMC Public Health 21, 543 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10594-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10594-2

Keywords