Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

A targeted promotional DVD fails to improve Māori and Pacific participation rates in the New Zealand bowel screening pilot: results from a pseudo-randomised controlled trial

Abstract

Background

New Zealand’s Bowel Screening Pilot (BSP) used a mailed invitation to return a faecal immunochemical test. As a pilot it offered opportunities to test interventions for reducing ethnic inequities in colorectal cancer screening prior to nationwide programme introduction. Small media interventions (e.g. educational material and DVDs) have been used at both community and participant level to improve uptake. We tested whether a DVD originally produced to raise community awareness among the Māori population would have a positive impact on participation and reduce the proportion of incorrectly performed tests (spoiled kits) if mailed out with the usual reminder letter.

Methods

The study was a parallel groups pseudo-randomised controlled trial. Over 12 months, all Māori and Pacific ethnicity non-responders four weeks after being mailed the test kit were allocated on alternate weeks to be sent, or not, the DVD intervention with the usual reminder letter. The objective was to determine changes in participation and spoiled kit rates in each ethnic group, determined three months from the date the reminder letter was sent. Participants and those recording the outcomes (receipt of a spoiled or non-spoiled test kit) were blinded to group assignment.

Results

2333 Māori and 2938 Pacific people participated (11 withdrew). Those who were sent the DVD (1029 Māori and 1359 Pacific) were less likely to participate in screening than those who were not (1304 Māori and 1579 Pacific). Screening participation was reduced by 12.3% (95% CI 9.1–15.5%) in Māori (13.6% versus 25.9%) and 8.3% (95% CI 5.8–10.8%) in Pacific (10.1% versus 18.4%). However, spoiled kit rates (first return) were significantly higher among those not sent the DVD (33.1% versus 12.4% in Māori and 42.1% versus 21.9% in Pacific).

Conclusion

The DVD sent with the reminder letter to BSP non-responders reduced screening participation to an extent that more than offset the lower rate of spoiled kits.

Trial registration

Australia and New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry ACTRN12612001259831. Registered 30 November 2013.

Peer Review reports

Background

Waitematā District Health Board (DHB) is responsible for a population of approximately 600,000 people in the north and west of Auckland, New Zealand (NZ). Waitematā DHB conducted a pilot of a Bowel Screening programme (BSP) from 2012 to 2017 to inform the potential introduction of an organised colorectal cancer screening programme in NZ [1]. The BSP mailed an immunohistochemical Faecal Occult Blood (iFOBT) self-test kit to all male and female Waitematā residents aged 55–74 years on the BSP population register, which was based on the National Health Index (a unique identifier given to all users of publicly funded health services). The pilot pathway comprised a pre-invitation letter, mailed self-test kit four weeks later, self-test completion, and return of results (via the participant’s general practitioner if positive). Non-participation resulted in a reminder letter being sent four weeks after the iFOBT was sent. For Māori, Pacific and Asian ethnicity invitees, a phone call follow-up was made concurrently with the reminder letter.

Strategies to optimise ethnic-specific participation in screening programmes are of international interest. Lessons on successful strategies from well-established screening programmes such as breast and cervical screening have been applied to colorectal cancer screening [2, 3]. A variety of interventions for colorectal cancer screening, in both organised and opportunistic settings, have been examined across patient, provider, organisation and policy intervention levels [4,5,6]. Interventions intended to increase participation at the patient level have emphasised the provision of ‘small media’ (health education or promotional materials e.g. brochures, booklets, video/DVD) in the context of screening invitation and follow up of non-participation.

The rationale for small media intervention is to act on social cognitive variables such as knowledge or attitude, aiming to reduce barriers such as risk perception or fear of the test [7]. While there is good evidence that small media does improve knowledge and facilitates a more favourable attitude to screening tests, there is equivocal evidence of the link between this and improved screening participation [4]. Many studies have demonstrated that some educational material has a positive impact on screening participation (variable range of effect depending on the kind of intervention and the setting) [2, 3]. There is also evidence that the provision of more detailed risk or test based information reduces screening participation (although informed consent may be increased) [8].

The BSP offered an opportunity to test whether a targeted bowel cancer screening health promotion DVD would raise participation and/or reduce the rate of incorrectly performed tests (referred to henceforth as the spoiled kit rate). The DVD focused on the indigenous Māori population and was developed locally for community awareness raising activities. This study examines its impact on Māori and Pacific screening participation when utilised as an adjunct to the usual non-participation reminder letter.

Methods

DVD development

A locally produced six-minute promotional DVD was developed as a tool for Māori community awareness-raising as part of a suite of activities aimed at increasing Māori BSP participation. That is, it was not developed as an adjunct to the bowel screening test kit process. The DVD was intended to be viewed in a range of community settings. A famous Māori rugby player delivered key programme messages aimed at improving knowledge and reducing barriers, including the ease and cleanliness of the test, and key features of invitation and programme participation. The DVD also featured two well-known local Māori elders presenting a narrative description of their programme participation experience. This narrative emphasised the importance of the test, the ease of the test, the nature of return of results and the positive experience of the diagnostic follow-up test. Although the intended audience were Māori, it was felt that many Pacific people would also be able to identify with the experiences portrayed by the protagonists.

Study participants

Māori and Pacific ethnic group’s participation in the first year of the BSP was considerably lower than other ethnic groups (including Asian, NZ European and Other ethnicities) the DVD intervention was tested in both groups. In New Zealand ethnicity is self-identified and respondents may select multiple ethnicities. For both invitation and analysis prioritised ethnicity was used where, in accordance with Ministry of Health Standards, Māori overrides all other ethnicities, Pacific overrides all but Māori and Asian ethnicity is recorded in priority to European [9, 10].

For a 12-month period from 4 January 2013 to 31 December 2013, all Māori and Pacific people who were enrolled in the BSP but did not return a test kit within the usual programme interval of four weeks were classified as non-responders and included in the study.

Intervention and control groups

The intervention consisted of including the promotional DVD with the reminder letter sent out to people who had not responded (non-responders) to the initial screening invitation. The control group was comprised of non-responders who were only sent the usual reminder letter.

Group allocation

Following the pilot programme’s existing procedures, reminder letters were generated in batches 3–4 times a week for eligible individuals (Māori and Pacific) once the time from the date of being sent an invitation had exceeded 4 weeks without a test kit being returned to the Coordination Centre. All Māori and Pacific participants were identified for each batch of reminder letters manually. Participants with both Māori and Pacific ethnicity were assigned to the Māori group in accordance with the prioritised ethnicity procedures described above. Study participants were allocated to the DVD plus reminder letter or reminder letter only groups on alternate weeks. DVDs required a slightly larger envelope than the reminder letter alone.

Recruitment monitoring performed approximately halfway through the trial revealed an imbalance in group accrual with more individuals being assigned to the intervention than the control group. This was found to be mainly due to the timing of batches for the reminder letters. To correct the imbalance, the weeks assigned for intervention and control were reversed at week 35 in the study.

There was also some inconsistency in whether the participants were correctly sent DVDs or not according to their DVD/no DVD week allocation according to records kept of who was sent a DVD. This resulted in a slightly lower proportion being sent a DVD than should have been the case if the protocol had been followed correctly (45.3% versus 49.3%). This was mainly due to allocation of patients who became eligible during weekends. To test for any systematic bias a logistic regression model compared those who were misallocated (i.e. incorrectly sent DVDs in a ‘non-DVD week’ or not sent a DVD in a ‘DVD week’) with those who were correctly sent DVDs or not according to their allocated week. This found no significant differences between them in ethnicity, gender, age or deprivation quintile. On this basis it was concluded that the misallocation was completely at random and we conducted all analyses on a per protocol basis. The logistic regression results and a separate analysis on an ‘intention to randomise’ basis is provided as Additional file 1.

Outcomes

There were two pre-specified primary outcomes of interest for the study: the difference in participation and the difference in spoiled kit return associated with being sent the DVD. Participation in the BSP was defined by the return of a test kit regardless of the result (positive, negative or ‘spoiled kit’). Participation was censored at three months post reminder letter generation in accordance with BSP standard operating procedure. ‘Spoiled’ kits are samples which cannot be analysed for one reason or another (e.g. no date on the specimen). A return was considered spoiled if any of the kits returned by that individual were spoiled. Those returning spoiled kits were sent replacement kits with an explanation as to why the previous sample was not usable.

Two non-prespecified secondary outcomes were analysed: the ‘Good first kit’ return rate (first kit returned was not spoiled hence the number of spoiled kits was zero) and the ‘Any good kit’ return rate (a good kit was returned at some stage and hence a test result was obtained).

Analysis

The trial was powered to detect a 5% difference in participation between intervention and control groups in both primary outcomes. Self-identified ethnicity was sourced from the BSP Register (ethnicity as recorded on the National Health Index). Prioritised ethnicity was used in analyses. Area deprivation was determined using the New Zealand Index of Deprivation 2006 (NZDep06) and grouped into quintiles from most deprived (Quintile 5) to least deprived (Quintile 1) [11, 12]. Participants who withdrew from the BSP within 3 months of the DVD/reminder letter were excluded from the analysis.

Wilson 95% confidence limits were calculated for all proportions and differences between proportions by Newcombe’s method without continuity correction [13, 14]. Separate pre-specified analyses were performed for Māori and Pacific invitees since it was hypothesised that the DVD might have more impact on Māori than Pacific ethnicity recipients. The Fleiss test of homogeneity of risk differences was used to assess whether a pooled Māori and Pacific analysis would be valid [15]. A log-binomial regression model was fitted to assess whether the impact of the DVD on outcomes changed after controlling for potential confounding by age, sex and deprivation. A separate model was fitted for Māori and Pacific. Data was analysed in Excel®, SAS® 9.4 and Stata® 13.

Results

After 12 months 5271 people (2333 Māori and 2938 Pacific) were randomised (see Consort Diagram in Fig. 1). Of those 2388 were sent a DVD with the reminder letter and 2883 just the usual reminder letter, the difference being attributable to the initial imbalance in group allocation resulting from the pseudo-randomisation process as described above. Table 1 shows that the DVD group did not differ from the no DVD group in demographic characteristics that are known to influence screening participation overall (ethnicity, age, sex and deprivation).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Consort Diagram

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of those who were sent the DVD compared with those who were not

Table 2 presents results from bivariate kit return (participation) analysis. It shows that for Māori, bowel cancer screening participation was 25.9% in the No DVD group and 13.6% in the DVD group. This is a 12.3% absolute reduction in participation and a 48.4% relative reduction in screening participation for Māori who were sent the DVD compared with Māori who did not. For the Pacific ethnicity group participation was 18.4% for the No DVD group and 10.1% for the DVD group, a 8.3% absolute and 45.1% relative reduction in participation. The Fleiss test for homogeneity of the risk difference rejected the null hypothesis (p = 0.043) which demonstrates that there was a significantly stronger absolute reduction in participation associated with being sent a DVD in Māori (compared with Pacific) that was more than should be expected by chance. For this reason, pooled analysis of kit return rates for two ethnic groups is not presented in Table 2. In a non-prespecified analysis it heterogeneity was detected between Māori males and females in kit return rates (p = 0.035); this was of borderline significance for Pacific (p = 0.075), with a stronger negative effect on participation observed in males than females. The table shows that being sent a DVD had a larger negative effect on participation among males than females.

Table 2 Kit return in DVD and No DVD groups by ethnicity and sex

Analysis of the spoiled kit rate is presented in Table 3. The spoiled kit rate for Māori who returned a test kit was 12.4% in the DVD group compared with 33.1% in those who were not sent a DVD, an absolute difference of 20.7% and a relative reduction in kit spoilage associated with being sent a DVD of 62.5%. The corresponding figures for the Pacific group were 21.9% kit spoilage in the DVD group, and 42.1% in the No DVD group with a risk difference of 20.2% and a relative reduction associated with the DVD of 47.9%. In this case the Fleiss test for homogeneity of risk difference was not significant (p = 0.92), suggesting that although Pacific had higher spoiled kit rates, the effect of the DVD on (reducing) the spoiled kit rate was similar for Māori and Pacific, and hence a pooled analysis is presented in Table 2 for this outcome, where the combined risk difference was 20.1%.

Table 3 Comparison of spoiled kit rates between the DVD and the usual participant reminder letter groups by ethnicity

Given that return rates were higher without the DVD, but spoiled kit rates were lower with the DVD, a secondary analysis to test whether there was an offset in benefit was conducted. This secondary analysis compared the proportions in each group where a non-spoiled kit was received on the first return (Additional file 1: Table S2). In Māori these were 11.9% among those sent the DVD and 17.3% among those who were not sent it (difference 5.5%; 95% CI 2.6–8.3%) and in Pacific they were 7.9 and 10.6% respectively (difference 2.7%; 95% CI 0.1–4.8%). These differences are considerably smaller than those based on all kit returns, but remain significant, showing that the DVD was associated with a lower rate of non-spoiled kit return. If subsequent non-spoiled kits are included in the calculation, then the differences widen again as some of those who initially returned a spoiled kit subsequently returned a non-spoiled one. In this case the (any) non-spoiled kit return rates in Māori were 13.4% among those sent the DVD and 22.2% in those not sent it (difference 8.8, 95% CI 5.6–11.8%) and for Pacific 9.6% in the DVD group and 14.5% in the No DVD group (difference 4.9%, CI 2.5–7.2%).

The log-binomial regression analysis (Table 4) established that the significant association between being sent a DVD and lower bowel screening participation was not confounded by gender, age or deprivation for either Māori or Pacific ethnic groups. The reduction in participation associated with the DVD was very similar to that found in the bivariate analysis (46.0% vs 45.6 and 44.9% vs 41.5% respectively for Māori and Pacific). The lower Spoiled kit rate among those sent the DVD was also unaffected by controlling for gender, age and deprivation. In the log-binomial regression model the DVD was associated with 60.0% lower spoiled kit rate for Māori compared with 62.5% in bivariate analysis; for Pacific the corresponding figures were 49.7 and 47.9%.

Table 4 Log-binomial regression models of outcomes

Discussion

Small media interventions are often developed and delivered in a health setting assuming that they will have a positive impact (or at least do no harm) without any rigorous evaluation of their impact. They may be developed for population-wide application or tailored to a target audience. Tailored approaches relevant to improving ethnic-specific screening participation using small media include: use of culturally informed narratives; promotional messages delivered by group members; messages targeting particular health beliefs; use of culturally appropriate language and visual appeal; or targeted health literacy approaches [16,17,18]. There is some evidence that favours visual or video-based strategies both as a community awareness raising strategy (increased community demand) and a patient focused strategy [19,20,21,22]. The evidence base for tailored approaches suggests they work best as part of a multi-level approach in organised screening programmes, including phone follow-up or navigator-based approaches [23,24,25].

In the Waitematā DHB bowel cancer screening pilot, which used a mailed test kit, people chose to participate at home with little or no interaction with their healthcare provider. An invitee’s decision is likely to have been influenced by the written information accompanying the test.

The BSP Steering Group fully expected that the DVD would have a positive impact on screening participation in Māori and possibly Pacific despite being designed for community awareness raising. At worst it was thought the DVD might have no impact on participation. This view was consistent with the evidence base for the impact on participation of small media which supports a positive effect [4, 26], with studies showing either no difference or a positive effect of up to 14% specifically with video/DVD material [19, 20, 27]. The finding that the DVD led to lower participation was therefore disappointing and quite unexpected for the BSP Steering Group (although the result is not inconsistent with previously published research on small media interventions which has highlighted the risks of ‘information overload’). It was therefore a salient lesson regarding the importance of rigorous evaluation, even of apparently innocuous interventions, to avoid these becoming an established component of healthcare delivery in the absence of prior evidence for their efficacy.

In retrospect several possible causes for the reduced participation in the DVD group can be identified: potential participants may have found the DVD itself off-putting (with or without viewing it); some may have viewed the DVD and found the content problematic; and others possibly viewed the DVD and made an informed decision not to participate on the basis of their increased knowledge. There is a well-known relationship between the provision of more information and reduced participation in screening [28,29,30,31]. An example of this was the 16% reduction in participation seen in an Australian trial of a colorectal screening decision-aid intervention for socioeconomically disadvantaged populations [8]. However, most reports in the literature of reduced screening participation from small media interventions relate to lengthy educational booklets or intensive decision-aid techniques aimed at improving knowledge and informed consent which were not the focus of this DVD.

Though the content of the DVD may not have been ideal for use as a reminder adjunct, it is difficult to see how it would have led to the observed negative effect on screening participation. It incorporated both narrative and educational components to improve knowledge and reduce barriers to participation. The protagonist in the narrative was a woman, which may have discouraged some men, but the observed reduction in participation was significant for both sexes. This approach was advocated for, and endorsed by Māori health providers at the outset of the BSP [32].

Rather than being due to the content of the DVD, a more plausible reason for lower participation in those sent the DVD could be that some of these individuals felt that it was necessary to watch the DVD before performing the test. If they did not own a DVD player or did not have the time to watch it this would have represented a barrier to participation. No reason for providing the DVD was included with the reminder letter.

The lower rate of spoiled kits in those sent the DVD is difficult to interpret. Whereas it is tempting to attribute this to guidance provided within the DVD on how to perform the test, it is also possible that with a higher kit return rate, the additional participants in the No DVD group were individuals who had (unmeasured) characteristics that made them more likely to return a spoiled kit. Among those who returned kits, the No DVD group had a higher proportion of males than the DVD group (49% vs 39%), but the DVD attributable reduction in rate of spoiled kits was not significantly different between males and females (19.7% vs 19.8%). Other demographic factors did not vary greatly between the groups either. The spoiled kit rate in the BSP was very high initially (and at the time that this study was conducted). Further reductions in the spoiled kit rate were seen after a redesign of the test kit instructions and accompanying materials that was undertaken after this project finished.

Limitations

The pseudo-randomised trial design was a convenient, efficient and practical approach in the context of an ongoing real-world programme, but in retrospect randomisation at the level of the individual might have avoided some of the problems noted in the methods section such as the unequal accrual rate in the DVD and No DVD group (although we have shown that it is unlikely to have introduced any bias – see Additional file 1 for details).

In this study we were unable to examine the reasons for reduced participation or record which DVD recipients watched the video. The literature suggests that other DVD media have relatively low uptake in terms of audience viewing with reasons provided by patients including lack of time, fear of cancer and not having a DVD player [33]. DVD viewing was found to be higher and related to improved screening participation in physician-based opportunistic colorectal cancer screening settings which is unlikely to be relevant to the BSP [21].

Conclusion

It was anticipated that the targeted nature of the DVD in this study would have no effect or could produce a small marginal gain in screening participation for Māori, and potentially Pacific people in the context of the BSP. It was also hypothesised that it might lead to a reduction in spoiled kits, which at the outset of the BSP was a significant problem. Instead we found that the DVD intervention was associated with lower screening participation in both these priority groups, whose participation was already below the rest of the eligible population. Although the DVD was also associated with a reduced spoiled kit rate, this was not sufficient to offset the lower participation so that even the return rate for non-spoiled kits was lower in the intervention group. This study adds to the literature on the impact of specific types of small media intervention and it provides a reminder that the potential for harm in interventions can easily be overlooked if these are not subject to rigorous assessment prior to introduction as part of routine practice.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BSP:

Bowel Screening Pilot

DHB:

District Health Board

DVD:

Digital Video Disc

References

  1. Hulme-Moir M. The New Zealand bowel screening pilot. N Z Med J. 2012;125(1356):9–12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Baron RC, Rimer BK, Coates RJ, Kerner J, Kalra GP, Melillo S, Habarta N, Wilson KM, Chattopadhyay S, Leeks K. Client-directed interventions to increase community access to breast, cervical, and colorectal Cancer screening. Am J Prev Med. 35(1):S56–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Camilloni L, Ferroni E, Cendales B, Pezzarossi A, Furnari G, Borgia P, Guasticchi G, Rossi P. Group tMtipW: methods to increase participation in organised screening programs: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Senore C, Inadomi J, Segnan N, Bellisario C, Hassan C. Optimising colorectal cancer screening acceptance: a review. Gut. 2015;64(7):1158–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Flight IH, Wilson CL, Griffiths L, Myers RE. Interventions for improving uptake of population-based screening for colorectal cancer using fecal occult blood testing. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2008, 4).

  6. Sandiford P, Buckley A, Robinson T, Tozer G, Holdsworth D, Badkar J. A community laboratory drop-off option for bowel screening test kits increases participation rates: results from an interrupted time series analysis. J Public Health. 2018;40(2):e133–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Gimeno Garca AZ. Factors influencing colorectal cancer screening participation. Gastroenterol Res Pract. 2012.

  8. Smith SK, Trevena L, Simpson JM, Barratt A, Nutbeam D, McCaffery KJ. A decision aid to support informed choices about bowel cancer screening among adults with low education: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2010;341:c5370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Ministry of Health. HISO 10001:2017: Ethnicity Data Protocol. Wellington: Ministry of Health; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Ministry of Health. Ethnicity Data Protocols for the Health and Disability Sector. Wellington: Ministry of Health; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Salmond C, Crampton P, Atkinson J. NZDep2006 index of deprivation users manual. Wellington: University of Otago; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Salmond CE, Crampton P. Development of New Zealand's deprivation index (NZDep) and its uptake as a national policy tool. Can J Public Health. 2012;103(8 Suppl 2):S7–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Wilson EB. Probable inference, the law of succession, and statistical inference. J Am Stat Assoc. 1927;22(158):209–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Newcombe RG. Two-sided confidence intervals for the single proportion: comparison of seven methods. Stat Med. 1998;17(8):857–72.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Fleiss JL, Levin B, Paik MC. Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York: Wiley; 2013.

  16. Shahid S, Finn L, Bessarab D, Thompson SC. Understanding, beliefs and perspectives of aboriginal people in Western Australia about cancer and its impact on access to cancer services. BMC Health Serv Res. 2009;9(1):132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Siddiqui AA, Sifri R, Hyslop T, Andrel J, Rosenthal M, Vernon SW, Cocroft J, Myers RE. Race and response to colon cancer screening interventions. Prev Med. 2011;52(3–4):262–4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Robillard AG, Larkey LJ: Health disadvantages in colorectal Cancer screening among African Americans: considering the cultural context of narrative health promotion. J Health Care Poor Underserved 2009, 20(2A Supplement ER) 102-119.

  19. Gimeno-García AZ, Quintero E, Nicolás-Pérez D, Parra-Blanco A, Jiménez-Sosa A. Impact of an educational video-based strategy on the behavior process associated with colorectal cancer screening: a randomized controlled study. Cancer Epidemiol. 2009;33(3–4):216–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Zapka JG, Lemon SC, Puleo E, Estabrook B, Luckmann R, Erban S. Patient education for colon cancer screening: a randomized trial of a video mailed before a physical examination. Ann Intern Med. 2004;141(9):683–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Levy BT, Daly JM, Xu Y, Ely JW. Mailed fecal immunochemical tests plus educational materials to improve colon cancer screening rates in Iowa research network (IRENE) practices. J Am Board Fam Med. 2012;25(1):73–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Noar SM, Benac CN, Harris MS. Does tailoring matter? Meta-analytic review of tailored print health behaviour change interventions. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(4):673–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Braun KL, Fong M, Kaanoi ME, Kamaka ML, Gotay CC. Testing a culturally appropriate, theory-based intervention to improve colorectal cancer screening among native Hawaiians. Prev Med. 2005;40(6):619–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Percac-Lima S, Grant RW, Green AR, Ashburner JM, Gamba G, Oo S, Richter JM, Atlas SJ. A culturally tailored navigator program for colorectal cancer screening in a community health center: a randomized, controlled trial. J Gen Intern Med. 2009;24(2):211–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Power E, Miles A, von Wagner C, Robb K, Wardle J. Uptake of colorectal cancer screening: system, provider and individual factors and strategies to improve participation. Future Oncol. 2009;5(9):1371–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. United States Preventative Screening Task Force: Recommendations for client- and provider-directed interventions to increase breast, cervical, and colorectal Cancer screening. Am J Prev Med 2008, 35(1, Supplement):S21-S25.

  27. Pigone M, Harris R, Kingsinger L. Videotape-based decision aid for colon cancer screening. A randomised, controlled, trial. Ann Intern Med. 2000;133:761–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Raffle AE. Information about screening – is it to achieve high uptake or to ensure informed choice? Health Expect. 2001;4(2):92–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Han PK, Duarte CW, Daggett S, Siewers A, Killam B, Smith KA, Freedman AN. Effects of personalized colorectal cancer risk information on laypersons’ interest in colorectal cancer screening: the importance of individual differences. Patient Educ Couns. 2015;98(10):1280–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. van Agt HM, Korfage IJ, Essink-Bot M-L. Interventions to enhance informed choices among invitees of screening programmes—a systematic review. Eur J Public Health. 2014;24(5):789–801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Edwards AG, Naik G, Ahmed H, Elwyn GJ, Pickles T, Hood K, Playle R. Personalised risk communication for informed decision making about taking screening tests. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013(2):Cd001865.

  32. Pitama S, Cave T, Huria T, Lacey C, Cuddy J, Frizelle F. Exploring Maori health worker perspectives on colorectal cancer and screening. N Z Med J. 2012;125(1356):75–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lewis CL, Brenner AT, Griffith JM, Pignone MP. The uptake and effect of a mailed multi-modal colon cancer screening intervention: a pilot controlled trial. Implement Sci. 2008;3(1).

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the support of Gaye Tozer, the Director of the Bowel Screening Pilot and Debbie Holdsworth the Chair of the BSP Steering Committee for their encouragement and support of this research.

Funding

The study received no dedicated funding. Costs were covered within the NZ Bowel Screening Pilot which was funded by the NZ Ministry of Health with in-kind contributions from Waitematā District Health Board. The Ministry of Health played no other role in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KB managed the implementation of the trial, contributed to design, analysis and interpretation, and wrote first draft of the paper; LZ was responsible for statistical analysis and advised on key aspects of design and interpretation of the results; SC provided input into the design, conduct, analysis and interpretation of the trial with specialist knowledge in the area of Māori health; AB was responsible for data management and provided in-depth knowledge of the BSP; EB was responsible for the development of the DVD intervention; PS conceptualised the trial, provided oversight of all aspects and prepared the final draft of the manuscript; all authors reviewed and read, commented on and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Sandiford.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Separate informed consent for this study (additional to the BSP consent) was not sought from participants because the purpose of this health services research was to test uptake, consent would have been impractical and may itself have reduced participation in the screening. Ethical approval with waiver of consent was received from the NZ Health and Disability Ethics Committee (12/NTA/91).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Supplementary material. (DOCX 41 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bartholomew, K., Zhou, L., Crengle, S. et al. A targeted promotional DVD fails to improve Māori and Pacific participation rates in the New Zealand bowel screening pilot: results from a pseudo-randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health 19, 1245 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7582-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7582-7

Keywords