Skip to main content

A cross-sectional analysis of green space prevalence and mental wellbeing in England

Abstract

Background

With urbanisation increasing, it is important to understand how to design changing environments to promote mental wellbeing. Evidence suggests that local-area proportions of green space may be associated with happiness and life satisfaction; however, the available evidence on such associations with more broadly defined mental wellbeing in still very scarce. This study aimed to establish whether the amount of neighbourhood green space was associated with mental wellbeing.

Methods

Data were drawn from Understanding Society, a national survey of 30,900 individuals across 11,096 Census Lower-Layer Super Output Areas (LSOAs) in England, over the period 2009–2010. Measures included the multi-dimensional Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (SWEMWBS) and LSOA proportion of green space, which was derived from the General Land Use Database (GLUD), and were analysed using linear regression, while controlling for individual, household and area-level factors.

Results

Those living in areas with greater proportions of green space had significantly higher mental wellbeing scores in unadjusted analyses (an expected increase of 0.17 points (95% CI 0.11, 0.23) in the SWEMWBS score for a standard deviation increase of green space). However, after adjustment for confounding by respondent sociodemographic characteristics and urban/rural location, the association was attenuated to the null (regression coefficient B = − 0.01, 95% CI -0.08, 0.05, p = 0.712).

Conclusions

While the green space in an individual’s local area has been shown through other research to be related to aspects of mental health such as happiness and life satisfaction, the association with multidimensional mental wellbeing is much less clear from our results. While we did not find a statistically significant association between the amount of green space in residents’ local areas and mental wellbeing, further research is needed to understand whether other features of green space, such as accessibility, aesthetics or use, are important for mental wellbeing.

Peer Review reports

Background

Mass migration and population growth over the last century have led to more than half of the world’s population residing in cities, creating a challenge for urban planners to efficiently accommodate new residents in a health promoting environment [1,2,3]. It has been suggested that mental health may differ between urban and rural areas, with studies contrasting in the direction of their conclusions [4,5,6]. Positive mental health and wellbeing have been linked to increased longevity, productivity and societal prosperity, but have also grown in prominence both politically and economically [7,8,9,10,11,12]. For example, the EU-level Beyond GDP (Gross Domestic Product) initiative was developed to be more inclusive of such social and environmental aspects of progress, by quantifying climate change, poverty and mental wellbeing, as well as the economy [13]. In the UK, results from the 2015 Annual Population Survey showed that, while mental wellbeing had on average increased over recent years, the divide between those rating their personal wellbeing at the highest and lowest levels had also grown, indicating a wellbeing inequality which needs to be addressed [14].

Mental wellbeing comprises two main components: the hedonic dimension, which includes happiness, life satisfaction and pain avoidance; and the eudaimonic dimension, which focuses on self-realisation, purpose in life and psychological function [15, 16]. Rather than just the absence of mental illness, mental wellbeing therefore encompasses aspects of positive affect, relaxation, functioning, personal relationships, life satisfaction and general happiness [17,18,19].

Emerging evidence suggests that aspects of the physical environment, and exposure to nature in particular, are often associated with higher levels of happiness and life satisfaction [20,21,22]. While these are important aspects of mental wellbeing, the relationship between green space and this multi-dimensional view of mental wellbeing remains relatively unexplored [20,21,22,23,24].

In urban environments, green space is considered to be any area of grass, trees or other vegetation, which in towns and cities is deliberately reserved for recreational, aesthetic or environmental purposes; this term therefore covers a range of green urban features, including parks, sports pitches and streetscape greenery. While abundant in rural areas, green spaces are usually designed into urban landscapes, typically at the expense of buildings. To encourage this to happen, the UK government sets out green space recommendations to encourage Councils to build these into each neighbourhood; these recommendations have been developed from government survey-based research and consideration of accepted walking distances between homes and green spaces [25].

Studies have sought to understand why green spaces seem to be beneficial for health and wellbeing. The theory of biophilia suggests that people pursue connections to nature; humans evolved in a natural landscape, where green spaces would have offered shelter, potential sources of food, and hence survival, so we may still experience positive feelings in such environments [26, 27]. Exposure to nature might enhance wellbeing by providing mental escape and restoration from fatigue, which is the focus for two key theories. Attention Restoration Theory proposes that effortful, directed attention is required to undertake everyday tasks, while the involuntary fascination which nature attracts provides an opportunity to rest the brain and regain concentration [28,29,30]. By contrast, it is suggested that urban environments may be less restorative, because of excessive stimuli and a need for directed attention to process these high levels of information [29, 31]. An alternative, the Stress Recovery Theory, argues that views of nature are the most beneficial for restoration, by helping stressed individuals recover a relaxed emotional state [32, 33]; these theories have been validated by a number of studies [31, 34,35,36,37,38,39,40]. It perhaps follows that individuals are often attracted to scenic environments, in particular trees, vegetation and water [1, 32, 41, 42], and so exposure to such landscapes may be valuable for happiness [22, 33, 43,44,45]. As well as these restorative mechanisms, it is theorised that green spaces may contribute to better health by enabling activities known to promote mental wellbeing, such as social interaction [2, 19, 46, 47] and physical activity [21, 48].

Recent research has begun investigating the association between the proportion of green space in neighbourhoods and residents’ mental health and wellbeing [8, 21, 23, 49, 50]. One study found a positive association to a single life satisfaction measure, by analysing 10,000 individuals living in Lower-Layer Super Output Areas (LSOAs) in urban England [22]. Other work has demonstrated that socioeconomic inequalities in mental wellbeing (indexed by the WHO-5 positive wellbeing index) tend to be smaller among those who feel they have good access to recreational areas within their urban neighbourhood, although this study did not objectively quantify green space, or restrict recreational areas to those that were specifically green [8]. Several studies also report that people are more likely to have lower levels of mental distress, as measured by the General Health Questionnaire (a psychiatric screening tool), when residing in areas with relatively more green space [22, 23, 51]. One such longitudinal study reported that ward-level proportions of green space were negatively associated with psychiatric morbidity, although the strength of this association varied across life course and by gender [52]. While lower levels of psychiatric symptoms are generally associated with better wellbeing, as described, mental wellbeing is a positive measure which reflects much more than an absence of distress [53].

While studies in this area tend to examine aspects of positive mental health, such as relaxation, satisfaction and general happiness [1, 22, 32, 41,42,43,44,45, 54, 55], we are only aware of one other study implementing a multi-dimensional measure of mental wellbeing. The study was based on a small selective sample in deprived areas of Scotland, and investigated the association between local green space proportions and mental wellbeing, of which the results were mixed and inconclusive [56].

Previous studies have tended to consider either urban green space or the wider benefits of contact with nature; while urban-rural differences in health have been studied, it is not yet known whether the association between green space and mental wellbeing in particular differs in urban and rural areas [1, 31, 32, 57, 58]. Although urbanisation reduces opportunities for people to interact with natural environments, it remains unclear whether or how this might affect the mental wellbeing of those who live in cities [59, 60].

The primary aim of this research was to test two hypotheses: (1) that neighbourhood areas of England with greater proportions of local-area green space are associated with higher levels of mental wellbeing; and (2) that the association between the proportion of local area green space and mental wellbeing may be confounded and/or modified by urban versus rural location.

Methods

Design

Sample

Data were drawn from the first wave of the UK Longitudinal Household Panel Study (UKLHS), known as Understanding Society, which ran from 2009 to 2010 [61]. Only residents of England were included, because of the availability of land use data. The UKLHS is a biennial survey of people aged 16 and over in a sample of private households across England, Scotland, Wales and Northern Ireland. Households were selected via random sampling of individual addresses within specific postcode sectors, to optimise sampling efficiency [62]. The wave 1 sample contained 50,994 individuals, from 30,169 households. Each household is also given a local-area identifier, by special licence access, which can be used to link UKLHS to the geographical green space data. These Lower-Layer Super Output Areas (LSOAs) are standardised UK Census units ideal for examining spatial data. England is divided up into 32,844 LSOAs, each of which contains 400–1200 residences and, within this data set, covers an average area of 4.2km2 (sd 12.8km2).

Mental wellbeing

Mental wellbeing was measured using the Short Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (SWEMWBS), which is comprised of 7 positively-worded questions relating to both hedonic and eudaimonic aspects of positive mental health [18, 61]. The questionnaire, issued through the Understanding Society survey, asked respondents to rate how they have been feeling “over the last 2 weeks” on 7 domains: optimistic about the future, useful, relaxed, close to other people, dealing with problems well, thinking clearly, and able to make up one’s mind. Using a 5-point Likert scale, options are “none of the time” (score 1), “rarely”, “some of the time”, “often” and “all of the time” (score 5). This results in a final rating between 7 and 35, with a higher number indicating better mental wellbeing [18].

Individual and household-level confounders

Potential confounders of the association between green space and mental wellbeing were identified from the literature, as well as examination of the individual data available within Understanding Society [21,22,23, 49, 51, 56, 63]. These included ten-year age group, gender, marital status (single/unmarried, married/civil partnership, and separated/divorced/widowed), ethnicity (white British, white other, black, South Asian, other), and total number of serious on-going physical health conditions (continuous, including clinical diagnoses of, for example, epilepsy, heart disease, cancer). Socioeconomic status was assessed by means of employment status (unemployed, employed and economically inactive), household income (quintiles adjusted for household composition [64]), household space (bedrooms per person, categorised into <1, 1–3, > 3), living alone, living with children, and housing tenure. Data on commuting time to work was also included, in line with previous work [22, 23, 65]. Local-area deprivation, at the LSOA level, was controlled for using the English Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD), which provides a score based on aspects including local education, income and crime statistics [66].

Green space

Green space data were obtained from the 2005 General Land Use Database (GLUD) [67], which provides land cover information for each LSOA in England. Each LSOA is given a total land cover and then divided into 9 usage categories, derived from Ordnance Survey’s MasterMap using visual inspection and information from the land registry; these groupings are domestic buildings, non-domestic buildings, domestic gardens, green space, water, path, road, rail, and ‘other’ [67]. For the purposes of this research, domestic gardens were not included as green space, as the category provided in the dataset included all domestic outdoor space, and so it could not be guaranteed that this was green. The relative amount of green space for each locality was calculated by dividing the area of green space by the total area for each LSOA, giving a proportion between 0 and 1.

Rural-Urban classification

Also included within the Understanding Society data [61], this Rural-Urban Classification divides England’s LSOAs into categories according to their level of urbanicity, based on population [68]. At the broadest level, urban centres are defined as settlements with a residential population greater than 10,000; as such, any local area is classified as urban if over 74% if its resident population lives in such an urban settlement. Within this dataset, the number of residents in urban areas, n, total 25,547; the rest are considered rural (n = 5353). This widest classification was selected for broad comparison and to ensure adequate amounts of data within each group.

Analysis

Analysis began by describing the distributions of mental wellbeing and green space, along with the characteristics of the study sample. To test for potential confounding, and to avoid collinearity, associations were estimated between each individual variable and green space and mental wellbeing, in turn. Those that were associated with both variables to a statistically significant degree met the selection criteria and were therefore considered to be potential confounders. Included in the final dataset were: sex, age, marital status, ethnicity, health conditions, employment, income, household space, living alone, living with children, housing tenure and commuting time to work.

As exploratory analyses revealed the distribution of SWEMWBS to be moderately skewed, we investigated the variance of this output in order to determine the most appropriate modelling technique. Linear regression modelling, found to be the most suitable, was used to estimate the association between mental wellbeing (SWEMWBS score) and the proportion of green space in each LSOA. Survey commands in the R Survey package were used to control for the clustered sampling of participants within the primary sampling units (PSUs); these are a stratified sample of postcodes designed to be representative of the UK population, in both economic and ethnic terms. The use of survey commands in R allowed us to generate robust estimates of variance in the association between individual exposure to green space and mental wellbeing that took account of autocorrelation (and therefore higher-level variances) in the dataset.

In the unadjusted model, using SWEMWBS score as the dependent variable, the regression coefficient (B) for green space represents an estimate of the amount by which wellbeing score increases a standard deviation increase in green space. To adjust for potential confounders, multivariate models were then built, including individual, socio-economic, place and household variables. The adjusted regression model was then run using urban/rural location as an additional variable. Analyses were completed with R 3.1.2 [69] using the Survey package [70], and Stata [71].

Results

In total, 50,994 individuals were included in wave 1 of the study, from 30,169 different households, which equates to a 57.6% participation response from the initially selected households, followed by an 81.8% individual-level response rate to the questionnaires issued to these agreeing households [72]. Little direct information was available regarding the characteristics of non-responding individuals, although they may be compared in terms of local-area socioeconomic statistics. The data collectors (Understanding Society) observed slightly lower response rates in areas with higher proportions of single-person households (59.0% response in 1st quartile of single-person households, compared to 55.5% in the highest,4th, quartile) and people in full-time employment (59.7% response in 1st quartile, 56.6% in 4th). Similarly, at the individual level, response rates were somewhat higher in areas of lower deprivation, in terms of Council Tax band (86.2% response in the lowest band A, 79.5% response in the highest bands E-H), suggesting a modest association between socio-economic status and survey participation [72].

Of the responding individuals, 42,972 were residents of England. After removing those who had missing SWEMWBS (mental wellbeing) scores, the final sample contained 30,900 individuals, from 19,684 different households, which is 61.0% of the original sample from the UKLHS. The sample covers 11,096 LSOAs across England, which vary considerably in size between urban (mean 0.9km2, sd 2.3km2) and rural areas (mean 19.6km2, sd 25.1km2). Of those not completing the mental wellbeing questions, mean green space exposure was 0.36 (sd 0.28), which was lower than the final sample (mean 0.42, sd 0.30) (Significance of t-test, p < 0.001).

From a socioeconomic perspective, local-area deprivation was significantly greater among SWEMWBS non-completers (mean score 27.1, sd 17.2 versus, 22.2, sd 15.6)(p < 0.001), although average equivalised income was consistent (£5515/month, sd £5438 for responders versus £5511/month, sd £5970 for non-responders) (p = 0.831).

In the final sample, prevalence of local area green space, given as a proportion of each LSOA, had a mean value of 0.42 (sd 0.30), with values of 0.33 (sd 0.24) and 0.82 (sd 0.19) in urban and rural areas, respectively. SWEMWBS scores were slightly negatively skewed; the mean score for the sample as a whole was 25.2 (sd 4.5), with a modal value of 28.0, and was significantly lower in urban than rural areas (mean score 25.1 (sd 4.6) versus 25.6 (sd 4.3))(p < 0.001).

The characteristics of people living in urban (n = 25, 547) and rural (n = 5353) areas also differed. The mean age of respondents was higher in rural areas, which also had greater proportions of married individuals. Income was also higher in rural areas, where area-level deprivation was considerably lower, household space was greater and more people owned their own home. These findings are presented in Table 1; t-tests were used to estimate the significance of the difference between urban are rural variables.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for the UK Longitudinal Household Survey, Data Sample

The unadjusted regression coefficient, B, for the association between proportion of green space and mental wellbeing was 0.17 points (95% CI 0.11, 0.23) in the SWEMWBS score, per standard deviation increase in green space. After controlling for all individual and household-level confounding factors (apart from urban/rural location), this coefficient was reduced 0.01 points (−0.05, 0.07) (p = 0.774).

Finally, adjusting further for urban/rural location in the association between a standard deviation increase in green space and SWEMWBS score, the resultant B value was −0.01 points (−0.08, 0.5, p = 0.712). While green space and urbanity were significantly linearly associated (B = −0.23, p < 0.001), we only found slight, but statistically insignificant evidence of effect modification (B = −0.11, 95% CI -0.29, 0.11, p = 0.382) between these variables. Stratified univariate models showed that the association was slightly stronger in rural (B = 0.12 points, 95% CI -0.01, 0.21, p = 0.062) than urban areas (B = 0.07 points, 95% CI 0.01, 0.13, p = 0.027), for a standard deviation increase in green space, although only the urban result was statistically significant.

The results of the fully-adjusted model are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 Fully Adjusted Linear Regression Model

As a sensitivity analysis, we repeated these models using quasi-poisson and log-transformed regressions, to account for the skewed distribution of the SWEMWBS variable. These modelling techniques did not significantly change our findings.

Discussion

Main findings

Previous research has demonstrated local-area prevalence of green space to be positively related to life satisfaction, happiness and reduced risk of psychiatric morbidity [22, 23, 51, 52]. In particular, studies applying data from the British Household Panel Survey (the predecessor to Understanding Society, which collected similar data), have shown a significant association between proportion of local area green space and lower GHQ scores, which held across longitudinal analyses [22, 23, 52]. We failed to find such an association when using a multi-dimensional measure of mental wellbeing as the study outcome, after adjusting for a wide range of potential confounders. These differences may be methodological, as we controlled for local-area deprivation and urban/rural location, as well as modelling green space as a continuous proportion, while Astell-Burt et al. did not [73]. However, White et al. found significant associations between green space and GHQ in their urban-area studies, while controlling for similar potential confounders, which, compared to our results, suggests that mental wellbeing reflects more than an absence of mental distress [22].

Although we hypothesised that urban/rural location may modify any associations between green space and mental wellbeing, we did not find any evidence supporting such an effect modification.

It may be useful to speculate on the processes underlying the observed confounding of the association between green space and mental wellbeing. For example, it has been suggested that levels of community and social support may be lower in rural areas, where people may be more isolated (perhaps because of difficulties accessing transport, or through fewer opportunities to socialise in remoter rural areas) [4]. Similarly, services (health and otherwise) may be less accessible in rural areas. However, we also note that our estimates were limited by the smaller sample of those living in rural areas, where variance in the proportion of green space was smaller than that observed in urban areas. Our findings should therefore be interpreted with caution.

These findings may also reflect methodological limitations, such as only including LSOA-level green space prevalence, or conceal more nuanced associations between green space and mental wellbeing. Green space itself may take many forms, and it may be that the association with mental wellbeing depends on the type rather amount of green space [74, 75]. Similarly, previous studies have shown that the quality of green space, and its biodiversity, were positively associated with mental health, where quantity was found to be less significant [76]. Context is also likely to matter [57, 58] and studies show that places that look untended or are poorly lit may be perceived as unsafe [47, 77, 78].

Strengths and limitations

To the best of our knowledge this is the first study to test the association between green space and a multi-dimensional mental wellbeing measure that includes both eudaimonic and hedonic mental wellbeing items, in all parts of England. The UKLHS is the largest household survey in the UK to date [22, 23], and contains extremely detailed socio-economic data as well as spatial identifiers. The latter allowed us to link the survey data to land use data, and to compare the effects of urban/rural location on mental wellbeing and on the association between green space and mental wellbeing.

Despite the strengths of this work, the quantification of green space is relatively simplistic, and it is possible that associations with mental wellbeing were not detected as a result of grouping all types of green space into one variable.

It is also possible that the attribution of green space scores according to the value for LSOAs introduced an element of misclassification, since it takes no account of accessibility or interaction with this space. As the LSOAs are derived according to population, neighbourhoods in urban areas will naturally be much smaller geographically than those in sparser settings, thereby making adjacent areas in built-up environments more accessible to these residents. Future research which includes data on distances to the nearest green space (which may extend to that in adjacent LSOAs) might demonstrate larger associations with mental wellbeing. These data were limited to the green space in the LSOA of residence, and did not take account of where respondents worked or spent time, or areas traversed when commuting. At the individual level, there was evidence of greater response rates in less deprived areas, a possible source of selection bias. Finally, our cross-sectional study, by design, had a limited capacity to establish causality.

Conclusions

The proportion of green space in an individual’s local area was significantly and positively associated with mental wellbeing in univariate models, but became weaker and statistically non-significant after adjusting for socio-demographic variables and urban/rural location. While the green space in an individual’s local area has been shown to be related to aspects of mental health such as happiness and life satisfaction, the association to multi-dimensional mental wellbeing is much less clear. Further research is therefore needed to explore the relationship of other aspects of green spaces aside from size, such as accessibility, aesthetics and use, to mental wellbeing.

Abbreviations

GDP:

Gross domestic product

GLUD:

General land use database

LSOA:

Lower layer super output area

SWEMWBS:

Short warwick-edinburgh mental well-being scale

UKLHS:

UK Longitudinal household panel study

References

  1. De Vries S, et al. Natural environments—healthy environments? An exploratory analysis of the relationship between greenspace and health. Environ Plan. 2003;35(10):1717–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Lachowycz K, Jones AP. Towards a better understanding of the relationship between greenspace and health: development of a theoretical framework. Landsc Urban Plan. 2013;118:62–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. United Nations. World Urbanization Prospects 2014. New York: United Nations Publication; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Paykel E, et al. Urban–rural mental health differences in Great Britain: findings from the National Morbidity Survey. Psychol Med. 2000;30(2):269–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Sundquist K, Frank G, Sundquist J. Urbanisation and incidence of psychosis and depression. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;184(4):293–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Probst JC, et al. Rural-urban differences in depression prevalence: implications for family medicine. Fam Med Kansas City. 2006;38:9.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Mitchell R, Popham F. Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: an observational population study. Lancet. 2008;372(9650):1655–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Mitchell RJ, et al. Neighborhood environments and socioeconomic inequalities in mental well-being. Am J Prev Med. 2015;49(1):80–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lachowycz K, Jones AP. Does walking explain associations between access to greenspace and lower mortality? Soc Sci Med. 2014;107:9–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. De Neve JE, Oswald AJ. Estimating the influence of life satisfaction and positive affect on later income using sibling fixed effects. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109(49):19953–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Oswald AJ, Proto E, Sgroi D. Happiness and productivity. 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Ashkanasy NM. International happiness: A multilevel perspective. Acad Manag Perspect. 2011;25(1):23–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cassiers I. Beyond GDP, Measuring progress, true wealth, and the well-being of nations: Conference Proceedings. 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Evans J, Macrory I, Randall C. Measuring National Well-being: Life in the UK, 2015. Office for National Statistics. 2015. http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20160105160709/http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/dcp171766_398059.pdf. Accessed 12 May 2017.

  15. Ryan RM, Deci EL. On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52(1):141–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Henderson LW, Knight T. Integrating the hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives to more comprehensively understand wellbeing and pathways to wellbeing. Int J Wellbeing. 2012;2:3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. O'Donnell G, Oswald AJ. National well-being policy and a weighted approach to human feelings. Ecol Econ. 2015;120:59–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Tennant R, et al. The Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2007;5:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. World Health Organization, Promoting mental health: Concepts, emerging evidence, practice: Summary report. 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Bratman GN, Hamilton JP, Daily GC. The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1249(1):118–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Mitchell R. Is physical activity in natural environments better for mental health than physical activity in other environments? Soc Sci Med. 2013;91:130–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. White M, et al. Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(6):920–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Alcock I, et al. Longitudinal effects on mental health of moving to greener and less green urban areas. Environ Sci Technol. 2014;48(2):1247–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Seresinhe CI, Preis T, Moat HS. Quantifying the impact of scenic environments on health. Sci Rep. 2015;5

  25. England N. Nature nearby: accessible natural greenspace guidance. Natural England: Peterborough; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Wilson EO. Biophilia. Boston: Harvard University Press; 1984.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Wilson EO. Biophilia and the conservation ethic. Evol Perspect Environ Problems. 2007:49–257.

  28. Kaplan R. Impact of urban nature: A theoretical analysis. Urban Ecol. 1984;8(3):189–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Berman MG. Jonides, Kaplan S. The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol Sci. 2008;19(12):1207–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Kaplan S. Meditation, restoration, and the management of mental fatigue. Environ Behav. 2001;33(4):480–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Hartig T, et al. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J Environ Psychol. 2003;23(2):109–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ulrich RS. Human responses to vegetation and landscapes. Landsc Urban Plan. 1986;13:29–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Ulrich RS, et al. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. 1991;11(3):201–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Ulrich R. View through a window may influence recovery. Science. 1984;224(4647):224–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Jiang B, Chang CY, Sullivan WC. A dose of nature: Tree cover, stress reduction, and gender differences. Landsc Urban Plan. 2014;132:26–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Carrus G, et al. Go greener, feel better? The positive effects of biodiversity on the well-being of individuals visiting urban and peri-urban green areas. Landsc Urban Plan. 2015;134:221–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Staats H, Van Gemerden E, Hartig T. Preference for restorative situations: Interactive effects of attentional state, activity-in-environment, and social context. Leis Sci. 2010;32(5):401–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Van den Berg AE, et al. Green space as a buffer between stressful life events and health. Soc Sci Med. 2010;70(8):1203–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Nordh H, et al. Components of small urban parks that predict the possibility for restoration. Urban For Urban Green. 2009;8(4):225–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Nordh H, Østby K. Pocket parks for people–A study of park design and use. Urban For Urban Green. 2013;12(1):12–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Bell SL, et al. Seeking everyday wellbeing: The coast as a therapeutic landscape. Soc Sci Med. 2015;142:56–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Lee SW, et al. Relationship between landscape structure and neighborhood satisfaction in urbanized areas. Landsc Urban Plan. 2008;85(1):60–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Lo AY, Jim C. Citizen attitude and expectation towards greenspace provision in compact urban milieu. Land Use Policy. 2012;29(3):577–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Mullaney J, Lucke T, Trueman SJ. A review of benefits and challenges in growing street trees in paved urban environments. Landsc Urban Plan. 2015;134:157–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Petersen LK. The materiality of everyday practices in urban greenspace. J Environ Policy Plan. 2013;15(3):353–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Dempsey N, Brown C, Bramley G. The key to sustainable urban development in UK cities? The influence of density on social sustainability. Prog Plan. 2012;77(3):89–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Maas J, et al. Social contacts as a possible mechanism behind the relation between green space and health. Health Place. 2009;15(2):586–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Maas J, et al. Physical activity as a possible mechanism behind the relationship between green space and health: a multilevel analysis. BMC Public Health. 2008;8:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Kimpton A, Wickes R, Corcoran J. Greenspace and Place Attachment: Do Greener Suburbs Lead to Greater Residential Place Attachment? Urban Policy Res. 2014;32(4):477–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. White MP, et al. Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(6):920–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Weimann H, et al. Effects of changing exposure to neighbourhood greenness on general and mental health: A longitudinal study. Health Place. 2015;33:48–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Astell-Burt T, Mitchell R, Hartig T. The association between green space and mental health varies across the lifecourse. A longitudinal study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2014;68(6):578–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Seligman ME. Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. New York: Simon and Schuster; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Halpern D. Mental health and the built environment: more than bricks and mortar? Routledge; 2014.

  55. Li HN, et al. On the study of the effects of sea views, greenery views and personal characteristics on noise annoyance perception at homes. J Acoust Soc Am. 2012;131(3):2131–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Ward Thompson C, Aspinall P, Roe J. Access to green space in disadvantaged urban communities: evidence of salutogenic effects based on biomarker and self-report measures of wellbeing. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. 2014;153:10–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Verheij R, Maas J, Groenewegen P. Urban—rural health differences and the availability of green space. Eur Urban Reg Stud. 2008;15(4):307–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Maas J, et al. Green space, urbanity, and health: how strong is the relation? J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60(7):587–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Hartig T, et al. Nature and health. Annu Rev Public Health. 2014;35:207–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Galea S, Vlahov D. Urban health: evidence, challenges, and directions. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26:341–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. University of Essex. Institute for Social and Economic Research, Understanding Society: Waves 1-5. UK Data Service. 2009-2014:2015.

  62. Buck N, McFall S. Understanding Society: design overview. Longitudinal Life Course Stud2011;3:1:5-17.

  63. Mitchell R, Popham F. Greenspace, urbanity and health: relationships in England. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61(8):681–3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Figini P. Inequality measures, equivalence scales and adjustment for household size and composition. Maxwell School of Citizenship and Public Affairs, Syracuse University. 1998. http://www.lisdatacenter.org/wps/liswps/185.pdf. Accessed 12 May 2017.

  65. Ambrey C, Fleming C. Public greenspace and life satisfaction in urban Australia. Urban Stud. 2014;51(6):1290–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Department for Communities for Local Government. The English Indices of Deprivation 2010. p. 2010.

  67. Office of the Deputy Prime Minister. Generalised Land Use Database Statistics for England. London: Communities and Local Government; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Government Satistical Service. The 2011 Rural-Urban Classification for Local Authority Districts in England, F.a.R.A. Department for the Environment. Sheffield. 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  69. The R Foundation for Statistical Computing. R version 3.1.2. 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Lumley T. Analysis of complex survey samples. J Stat Softw. 2004;9(1):1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  71. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. Texas: StataCorp LP; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Lynn PJ, et al. An initial look at non-response and attrition in Understanding Society. 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Astell-Burt T, Feng X, Kolt GS. Mental health benefits of neighbourhood green space are stronger among physically active adults in middle-to-older age: evidence from 260,061 Australians. Prev Med. 2013;57(5):601–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Taylor L, Hochuli DF. Creating better cities: how biodiversity and ecosystem functioning enhance urban residents’ wellbeing. Urban Ecosyst. 2015;18(3):747–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Wheeler BW, et al. Beyond greenspace: an ecological study of population general health and indicators of natural environment type and quality. Int J Health Geogr. 2015;14:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Francis J, et al. Quality or quantity? Exploring the relationship between Public Open Space attributes and mental health in Perth, Western Australia. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(10):1570–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Van den Berg AE, Jorgensen A, Wilson ER. Evaluating restoration in urban green spaces: Does setting type make a difference? Landsc Urban Plan. 2014;127:173–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Gatersleben B, Andrews M. When walking in nature is not restorative—The role of prospect and refuge. Health Place. 2013;20:91–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Helen Parsons for her statistical guidance on this work.

Funding

Thank you to the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council for the financial input which supported this research, by providing an annual stipend to VH.

Availability of data and materials

Land use data is freely available from the UK Government Statistical Service, accessed via: https://data.gov.uk/dataset/land_use_statistics_generalised_land_use_database.

The UKLHS data that support the findings of this study are available from the UK Data Service but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and so are not publicly available. The data source may be accessed via:

https://discover.ukdataservice.ac.uk/catalogue/?sn=6676&type=Data%20catalogue

Authors’ contributions

VH conducted the literature review, collected the data, performed the statistical analysis and led the drafting of the article. SW participated in the study design, performed some analyses and aided with the preparation and editing of the manuscript. SJ participated in the design and preparation of the study. All authors have read and approved this manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

No formal ethical approval was required, as we performed secondary analysis of existing data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Victoria Houlden.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Houlden, V., Weich, S. & Jarvis, S. A cross-sectional analysis of green space prevalence and mental wellbeing in England. BMC Public Health 17, 460 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-017-4401-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-017-4401-x

Keywords