Skip to main content

Digital self-presentation and adolescent mental health: Cross-sectional and longitudinal insights from the “LifeOnSoMe”-study

Abstract

Background

The intensive use of social media among adolescents has caused concern about its impact on their mental health, but studies show that social media use is linked to both better and worse mental health. These seemingly contradictory findings may result from the diverse motivations, interactions, and experiences related to social media use, and studies investigating specific facets of social media use in relation to mental health and well-being, beyond general usage metrics, have been called for. Aspects of self-presentation on social media, such as feedback-seeking and upwards social comparison have been linked to worse mental health, however, there is a need for more studies exploring the relationship between self-presentation on social media and adolescent mental health over time.

Aim

The aim of this study was to explore the cross-sectional and longitudinal relationship between aspects of self-presentation and depression, anxiety, and well-being among adolescents.

Methods

This study utilised both cross-sectional and longitudinal datasets from the LifeOnSoMe-study, comprising 3,424 and 439 participants, respectively (OSF preregistration https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/BVPS8). Latent Class Analysis (LCA) was used to identify similar response patterns within the Self-Presentation and Upwards Social Comparison Inclination Scale (SPAUSCIS). Regression models and first differencing methods were applied to evaluate the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between focus on self-presentation and mental health and well-being among adolescents.

Results

A strong emphasis on self-presentation was linked to increased levels of depression and anxiety in both males and females, and reduced well-being in females when compared to those with lower or intermediate self-presentation focus. The effect sizes ranged from small to medium. Furthermore, an escalation in self-presentation focus over time was associated with a slight increase in symptoms of anxiety and depression; however, the association with well-being did not reach statistical significance.

Conclusion

The results of the present study suggest that a heightened focus on self-presentation, which includes behaviours such as seeking feedback, employing strategic self-presentation tactics, and engaging in upward social comparisons, is associated with an elevated risk of reduced mental health.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

The intensive use of social media among adolescents has caused concerns about its impact on their mental health and well-being, and a host of scientific papers have addressed this issue [1]. A recent umbrella review showed that the amount of time spent on social media use is weakly associated with both higher levels of mental health problems and with higher well-being among adolescents [2]. These seemingly contradictory findings may be attributed to heterogeneity of social media use and to person-specific effects [3], meaning that social media use can entail widely different motivations, interactions, experiences, and behaviours, and that any effects of social media use are likely to vary depending on how, why, and by whom they are used. Therefore, investigating how particular facets of social media use influence mental health and well-being, beyond general metrics of frequency and duration of use, has been called for [2]. In addition, research should focus on key attributes spanning a range of different social media platforms, in line with an affordance approach [4], to stay relevant in the ever evolving social media landscape. In the context of social media, affordances refer to “the perception of action possibilities users have when engaging with social media and its features” ([5], pp. 408–409).

One aspect of social media use that has been studied in relation to mental health and well-being is self-presentation [6, 7]. Social approval is seen as one of the main goals of self-presentation on social media [8], and some adolescents place great emphasis on their online personas [9,10,11]. In line with Goffman’s theory of self-presentation and social interaction [12], all social encounters entail some form of performance to manage how one is perceived by others (i.e., self-presentation). To present the best possible version of themselves, people downplay certain characteristics and enhance others; a process called impression management [12]. Compared to traditional face-to-face interactions, social media affordances facilitate impression management and idealized self-presentation by allowing users to manipulate their text and image based communication [13]. Furthermore, the number of likes and number or content of comments can easily be compared to others’ to quantify one’s social success [14]. Some people make great efforts to receive the desired feedback, referred to as feedback-seeking or digital status seeking [15]. Arguably, as self-presentation on social media is often idealized and is mainly positive, upward social comparison, i.e., comparing oneself to someone who is viewed as better than oneself [16], may be particularly likely [14, 17]. Social media use also increases the number of available comparison targets to include not only peers in one’s immediate surroundings, but also a wider network of acquaintances, ‘influencers’, and celebrities, thereby expanding the opportunities for engaging in upward social comparison.

Studies on adolescents have shown that different aspects of self-presentation, such as feedback-seeking, strategic self-presentation such as editing photos, and upward social comparison, are associated with worse mental health in terms of more symptoms of anxiety and depression, and reduced body satisfaction and well-being [11, 18,19,20,21]. These findings can be linked to the broader concept of ‘approval anxiety’, i.e., the degree of psychological arousal about others’ reactions to one’s messages and posts on social media, which has been proposed as one component of digital stress [22]. Digital stress, in turn, has been shown to increase the risk of negative mental health outcomes as a result of social media use. Self-presentation on social media may therefore be one aspect of social media use that can have negative consequences for adolescent mental health. Most previous studies are, however, based on cross-sectional data, and more longitudinal studies are needed to establish the relevance of aspects of self-presentation on social media to adolescent mental health. The few longitudinal studies that exist have shown that posting a lot of content on social media, being preoccupied with one’s physical attractiveness in social media photos, feedback-seeking, and upward social comparison are linked to symptoms of anxiety and depression, and reduced well-being [18, 23, 24].

Adolescence is a period when peer approval becomes increasingly relevant, and seeking approval alongside a heightened sensitivity to social rewards may be a an important motivator for using social media during this developmental phase [5]. Adolescents seem to vary a great deal in their preoccupation with self-presentation on social media. In a previous study, we investigated how adolescents differed in their preoccupation with likes, comments, and followers, in deleting posts with too few likes and manipulating images to look better, and in upward social comparison, collectively referred to as “focus on self-presentation” [25]. The results showed that females and adolescents with low emotional stability and high scores on extraversion, were more likely to be highly focused on self-presentation. Similarly, adolescent girls have been found to report higher levels of feedback-seeking and social comparison [11, 18], post more ‘selfies’, be more focused on their physical appearance, and be more concerned about peer feedback, compared to adolescent boys [26]. While some research has found that the associations between aspects of self-presentation on social media and mental health problems are similar for boys and girls [24], some findings indicate that the association is stronger for girls [11, 18].

The aim of the present study was to further explore the relationship between focus on self-presentation and depression, anxiety, and well-being. Firstly, a latent class analysis was used to map out response patterns on a seven-item scale assessing focus on self-presentation among adolescents. Secondly, the association between these response patterns and mental health was assessed separately for males and females using a cross-sectional dataset. Lastly, the longitudinal association between focus on self-presentation and mental health was assessed over two time-points.

Methods

The present study (OSF preregistration https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/BVPS8) was based on data from an online survey conducted in two rounds in 2020 and 2021, in Bergen, Norway, called the LifeOnSoMe-study. Bergen is the second-largest city in Norway and has a population of about 300,000. All senior high school pupils of 16 years or older were invited to participate in the survey via their teachers and information screens on their school. The pupils received a link to a website and logged in using their electronic ID. Before starting the survey, they received information about the study and provided their informed consent. In addition, those that had participated in the 2020 data collection received an email with a link to the survey. The participation rate was 53% in 2020 and 35% in 2021. The broader aim of the LifeOnSoMe-study was to explore the relationship between adolescents’ motivations, experiences, and behaviours related to social media use and sociodemographic variables, lifestyle and social factors, and mental health.

The present study was based on two separate datasets. The cross-sectional dataset comprised responses from the two rounds of the survey. For those who completed the survey both in 2020 and 2021 (n = 461), we only used their 2020 responses. The total number of participants in the cross-sectional dataset was 3,771. Of these, participants missing information about gender (n = 5) or age (n = 158) were excluded. Furthermore, only 40 participants ticked the option “non-binary” for gender. This number is too low to perform meaningful analyses, and these participants were excluded from the study. Those with 100% missing values on the independent variable (n = 144) were excluded from the analyses, resulting in a total sample size of n = 3,424. The longitudinal dataset was based on the responses of those who completed the survey both in 2020 and 2021 (N = 461, 59% females). Of these, 22 participants missing 100% of the items of the independent variable were excluded (n = 4 at T1 and n = 18 at T2), resulting in a total sample of n = 439.

Variables

Focus on self-presentation

To assess focus on self-presentation on social media, we used the Self-Presentation and Upward Social Comparison Inclination Scale (SPAUSCIS), which was developed based on qualitative focus group interviews with adolescents. The development of the scale is described in detail elsewhere [11, 25]. In a previous study, we showed that the SPAUSCIS had one latent factor and high internal consistency in a sample of adolescents [25]. The scale consists of 7 statements regarding focus on self-presentation on social media, covering feedback-seeking, strategic self-presentation, and upward social comparison (see supplementary Table S1). The participants were asked how much each statement pertained to them, and the response options were “not at all”, “very little”, “sometimes/partly true”, “a lot”, and “very much”, coded 1–5. The total score was computed by averaging the sum score on the total number of items, resulting in a total score ranging from 1–5. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87 in the cross-sectional sample and 0.86 in the longitudinal sample (at T1).

Social media use

The participants’ frequency of social media use was measured by the following question: “How often do you use social media?” The response alternatives were “almost never”, “several times a month, but less than once a week”, “1–2 times per week”, “3–4 times per week”, “5–6 times per week”, “every day”, “several times each day”, and “almost constantly”. In the present study, we created a tripartite variable which differentiated between “daily or less”, “many times each day”, and “almost constantly”. The participants’ duration of social media use was assessed by the following question: “On the days that you use social media, approximately how much time do you spend on social media?” The seven response options ranged from “less than 30 min” to “more than 5 h”. The response options were categorized into “less than 2 h”, “2–4 h”, “4–5 h”, and “more than 5 h”.

Symptoms of anxiety

Symptoms of anxiety were measured using the General Anxiety Disorder 7 (GAD-7; [27]). The GAD-7 consists of 7 questions related to symptoms of general anxiety. The response options ranges from 0 (not at all) to 3 (almost every day). The measure was used as a continuous variable with the total score ranging from 0 to 21. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.90 in the cross-sectional sample and 0.89 in the longitudinal sample (at T1).

Symptoms of depression

Symptoms of depression were measured using the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ; [28]). The SMFQ consists of 13 statements related to symptoms of depression. The response options are 0 (not true), 1 (sometimes true), and 2 (true). The scores on each item are summed to a total score ranging from 0 to 26. The measure was used as a continuous variable. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.91 in the cross-sectional sample and 0.88 in the longitudinal sample (at T1).

Well-being

The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS) was used to assess the participants’ level of mental well-being [29]. The WEMWBS focuses solely on positive aspects of mental health, covering positive affect, satisfying personal relationships, and positive functioning. The scale has 14 positively scored items and responses are given on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “none of the time” (1) to “all of the time” (5). The minimum score is 14 and the maximum score is 70, with a higher score indicating better mental well-being. The responses are based on the previous two weeks. The Norwegian version of the WEMWBS was used in the present study, which has shown good validity and reliability for Norwegian adolescents [30]. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.93 in the cross-sectional sample and 0.92 in the longitudinal sample (at T1).

Background variables

Participants provided their age, gender, and which year in senior high school (first, second, or third) and which program they attended college (preparatory or vocational education). Subjective socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed by the question “How well off do you consider you own family to be compared to others?” The response options ranged from 0 (“very poor”) to 10 (“very well off”). In the current study, SES was recoded into a tripartite variable of low SES (scores 0–4; 6.4%), medium SES (5–7, 52%), and high SES (8–10, 42%). Personality was measured using the Ten-Item Personality Inventory [31], consisting of ten items measuring two opposing traits of each personality dimension (Extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and openness to new experiences). The items are preceded by “I see myself as”, followed by trait adjectives. The response categories range from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The total score on each trait is calculated by taking the average of the two items after recoding the reverse-scored item, resulting in a total score ranging from 2 to 14.

Statistical analyses

All analyses were performed using R version 4.1.3 [32] and RStudio version 2023.06.1 + 524 [33]. To assess the structural validity of the SPAUSCIS, a confirmatory factor analysis was performed using the cross-sectional dataset. Internal validity was assessed with Cronbach’s alpha, using the ‘psych’ package [34] and the confirmatory factor analysis was performed using the ‘lavaan’ package [35] and DWLS estimator suitable for ordinal variables [36]. Groups with similar response patterns on the items of the SPAUSCIS were identified using latent class analysis (LCA), using the ‘poLCA’ package [37]. The most appropriate number of latent classes was chosen based on several statistical criteria: Aikake information criterion (AIC), Bayesian information criterion (BIC), relative entropy, and the Lo-Mendell-Rubin ad hoc adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR-LR), as well as interpretability of the model.

Cross-sectional associations

Linear regression was used to assess the associations between latent class membership and depressive symptoms, symptoms of anxiety, and well-being. The associations were estimated for the full sample and separately for males and females, and expressed as coefficients with corresponding standard errors, in addition to Cohen’s ds. As SES, frequency and duration of social media use, and the personality traits of extraversion and emotional stability has been linked to both focus on self-presentation [25] and to mental health outcomes in previous studies [2, 38, 39], all regressions were adjusted for these variables in multiple linear models. For the full sample, adjustments were also made for gender. Adjusted Cohen’s d values were calculated following the procedure included in the ESIZEREG module for Stata [40]. Likelihood ratio tests were used to examine a potential gender moderation in the associations between class membership and the dependent variables, comparing models with the interaction gender × class membership and models with gender included as a covariate. In all analyses, a p-value of < 0.05 indicated statistically significant associations. A post-hoc analysis assessing the correlation between the total score of the SPAUSCIS as a continuous variable with symptoms of depression and anxiety, and well-being using Spearman rank correlation.

Longitudinal associations

The ‘plm’ package [41] was used to estimate first difference models to assess the longitudinal associations between focus on self-presentation and mental health and well-being. First difference models difference out fixed effects such as gender, socioeconomic status and other variables that are assumed to be fixed over time [42]. Thus, the model avoids bias due to unobserved time-invariant variables. To ease interpretation of the results, we report both the “raw” coefficients and coefficients based on z-scored dependent variables. When using standardized dependent variables, the coefficients are interpreted as standard deviations: For every one-unit increase in the independent variable, the dependent variable increases by a given number of standard deviations.

Missing data

There were some missing data. After excluding those that were missing 100% of the SPAUSCIS items from the dataset, there were 0.8 to 3.9% missing on the items of the SPAUSCIS in the cross-sectional dataset, 0.2 to 1.1% missing in the longitudinal dataset at T1 and 1.1 to 4.3% at T2. The SPAUSCIS total score is calculated as the mean of the item scores and those missing one or more items received a mean based on the completed items.

The total scores of SMFQ, GAD-7 and WEMWBS were calculated by dividing the sum score of completed items on the number of completed items, multiplied by the total number of items of the relevant scale. Pairwise deletion was used throughout the analyses to retain as much information as possible.

Results

Table 1 shows descriptive information for the cross-sectional data. The mean age of the sample was 17.28 years (SD 1.01), and 56% were girls. There were significant differences between girls and boys in all variables except age, school year and birth country. Females had higher scores on the duration and frequency of social media use and on focus on self-presentation, as well as on symptoms of depression and anxiety, and lower scores on well-being.

Table 1 Descriptives for the cross-sectional data

The CFA of the items of the SPAUSCIS resulted in a Comparative Fit Index (CFI) of 0.999, a Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) of 0.998, a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of 0.051 (95%CI 0.043–0.060, p = .398), and a standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) of 0.021, all signalling god fit [43]. Items 2 and 3 and items 6 and 7 had highly correlated error terms, which were allowed for in the model.

The LCA yielded three classes corresponding to a low (class 1), intermediate (class 2), and high (class 3) focus on self-presentation, in line with the previous findings [25]. Predicted class membership was 44% in class 1, 33% in class 2, and 23% in class 3. Class 3 and 2 was dominated by females, while class 1 was dominated by males. Class 3 also had a lower proportion of adolescents with high SES, and a higher proportion of adolescents using social media ‘almost constantly’ compared to class 1 and 2. See supplementary figure S1 and table S2 for a more detailed description of the LCA results. See also supplementary table S3 for descriptives across class membership and S4 for an overview of SPAUSCIS scores across class membership.

Table 2 shows the results of the linear models. Being in class 3 was associated with higher symptoms of anxiety and depression compared to class 1 and 2 in both crude and fully adjusted cross-sectional analyses for the sample as a whole and for males and females when analysed separately (all p’s < .01). The effect sizes were small-to-medium in crude models (Cohen’s ds from 0.34–0.66 for anxiety and 0.43-0.74 for depression) and small in fully adjusted models (Cohen’s ds from 0.16–0.32 for anxiety and 0.25–0.33 for depression). For well-being, being in class 3 was associated with lower well-being compared to class 1 and 2 for males, females, and the sample as a whole in the crude models (all p’s < .05), with small effect sizes (Cohen’s d from -0.20- -0.46). In the fully adjusted models, the lower well-being associated with class 3 membership was no longer significant for males. Class 2 membership was not associated with any difference in symptoms of anxiety, depression or well-being compared to class 1 membership in adjusted models, but was associated with lower well-being for the sample as a whole in the crude model (Cohen’s d -0.13, p < .001). The likelihood ratio tests comparing models with and without the interaction term class membership × gender were not significant, meaning that the associations between class membership and anxiety, depression, and well-being were not significantly different for males and females (results provided in the Appendix, all p’s > .05).

Table 2 Linear models for GAD-7, SMFQ, and WEMWBS separate for males and females and for males and females combined

The post-hoc analysis showed that the correlation coefficient was 0.38 (p < .001) for the SPAUSCIS and symptoms of depression, 0.36 (p < .001) for SPAUSCIS and symptoms of anxiety, and -0.27 (p < .001) for well-being.

Longitudinal associations

First difference modelling was used to assess how changes in focus on self-presentation, measured by the SPAUSCIS, from T1 to T2 was related to changes in symptoms of anxiety and depression, and well-being. The first difference model yielded a coefficient of 0.85 (SD 0.36, p = .037) for symptoms of anxiety, 1.53 (SD 0.39, p < .001) for symptoms of depression, and a non-significant coefficient for well-being (-1.24, SD 0.68, p = .069). Thus, for each increase of 1 on the SPAUSCIS (total score ranging from 1 to 5) from T1 to T2, symptoms of anxiety increased by 0.85 and symptoms of depression increased by 1.53. The decrease in well-being for each increase of 1 on the SPAUSCIS scale did not reach statistical significance. Using standardized coefficients, each increase of 1 on the SPAUSCIS from T1 to T2 was associated with an increase of 0.17 standard deviations in symptoms of anxiety (SD 0.07, p < .05) and 0.25 standard deviations in symptoms of depression (SD 0.06, p < .001), both corresponding to small effect sizes [44], and a non-significant decrease of -0.13 (SD 0.07, p = .069) in well-being.

Discussion

In this study, we used both cross-sectional and longitudinal data to investigate the relationship between focus on self-presentation on social media and experiences of symptoms of depression, anxiety, and overall well-being among adolescents. The results of a latent class analysis revealed, in line with a previous study [25], that the participants’ response patterns could be best characterized by a three-class solution, representing varying degrees of focus on self-presentation: low, intermediate, and high. A high focus on self-presentation was associated with higher scores on symptoms of depression and anxiety for both males and females, and lower scores on well-being among females, compared to a low or intermediate focus on self-presentation. Effect sizes ranged from small to medium. Additionally, we found that an increasing emphasis on self-presentation over time was associated with an increase in symptoms of anxiety and depression, although these effects were relatively small. Conversely, the association between an increased focus on self-presentation and well-being did not reach statistical significance. Hence, our findings suggest that a heightened focus on self-presentation, which includes behaviours like seeking feedback, employing strategic self-presentation tactics, and engaging in upward social comparisons, is associated with a small increase in risk of negative mental health outcomes.

Our findings are in line with previous studies [11, 18,19,20,21], and they add to the literature by showing these relationships by also using a longitudinal approach. Although this study is unable to establish a causal link between focus on self-presentation and mental health, there are some candidate mechanisms that could explain such a link. Firstly, placing importance on likes and comments may reflect a sense of self-worth that relies on online validation, making the individual vulnerable to fluctuations in likes and comments. Secondly, focus on self-presentation can be related to what Steele et al. [22] termed ‘approval anxiety’, which can contribute to an overall stress reaction (‘digital stress’) and consequently lead to symptoms of anxiety and depression, and lower mental well-being. Thirdly, a high focus on self-presentation may reflect a higher level of self-objectifications, i.e., an internalization of the observers’ gaze and viewing oneself as an object [45], which is regarded a risk factor for mental health problems [46,47,48]. Conversely, it is also possible that mental health problems lead to a higher focus on self-presentation. Studies have shown that underlying risk factors for poor mental health, such as shyness, loneliness, and neuroticism, predict heavier social media use and problematic social media use [49, 50], and may also predict a higher focus on self-presentation. To disentangle the causal relationship between focus on self-presentation and mental health, large multi-wave longitudinal studies are needed. The current finding that focus on self-presentation and mental health problems change concurrently indicate a crucial avenue for further investigation.

The current results showed that the group with a high focus on self-presentation was dominated by girls, but that the associations between focus on self-presentation and depression, anxiety, and well-being were not statistically different for males and females, as indicated by the interaction analysis. Similarly, Maheux et al. [24] found that while girls reported a higher level of preoccupation with their physical attractiveness in social media photos, the longitudinal association with depressive symptoms were similar for boys and girls. In the fully adjusted models of the present study, however, well-being was only associated with class membership for the sample as a whole and for girls, but not for boys. This may be related to the overrepresentation of girls in the dataset or to unobserved variables that are affecting the association differently for each gender. In the present study, we had no information about the content of the participants’ self-presentation. Studies have shown that girls’ self-presentation differs from boys. For example, girls have been shown to post more selfies and be more invested in physical appearance [26], which may impact the association between focus on self-presentation and well-being. A study by Svensson, Johnson, & Olsson [51] also showed gender differences, finding that self-presentation was associated with internalizing symptoms for girls only. Future research should explore these gender differences in the interactions between aspects of social media use and well-being.

In our study, no longitudinal association was observed between focus on self-presentation and well-being. This suggests that while an increase in focus on self-presentation over time may be related to more symptoms of anxiety and depression, it does not appear to impact well-being. The small effect sizes found for symptoms of anxiety and depression support this notion. At the same time, our longitudinal sample size was relatively limited, which may have contributed to the nonsignificant association due to issues of statistical power. Furthermore, it is possible that focus on self-presentation does not change very much from one year to the next, and that a longer time span would yield larger differences between focus on self-presentation at baseline and follow-up and a clearer relationship between focus on self-presentation and mental health and well-being. It is also possible that the relationship between focus on self-presentation and mental health differs between younger and older adolescent, in line with a study showing that the strength of the relationship between social media use and life satisfaction changed depending on the adolescents’ age [52]. Specifically, higher social media use predicted decreases in life satisfaction one year later among girls at ages 11–13 and 19, and among boys at 13–15 and 19. Exploring how focus on self-presentation is related to well-being among younger adolescents than those included in our study (16 +), would be of interest.

Our findings are in line with the results of a recent study by Winstone and colleagues [23], who employed latent class analysis to identify different user types on social media among 13-year-olds and how these types were related to mental health outcomes. In their study, adolescents characterized by high levels of content sharing (‘Broadcasters’) had a higher risk of poor mental health one year later, compared to those with moderate content sharing. In the present study, we did not measure self-presentation activity such as frequency of posting content, but rather how preoccupied the participants were with the feedback they received, strategic self-presentation, and their degree of upward social comparison. It may be that adolescents who post a lot on social media are also highly preoccupied with their online self-presentation, and that it is their preoccupation with self-presentation that increases their risk of mental health problems and not posting per se. In fact, some research indicates that self-presenting on social media even can have some benefits. For example, studies have shown that people can experience an increase in self-esteem after viewing one’s own social media profile [53, 54], and social media can facilitate authentic self-presentation of aspects of the self that are perceived as unwanted in offline social setting [55]. Furthermore, positive self-presentation (i.e., showing positive sides of the self) has been shown to increase subjective well-being, perhaps by supporting self-affirmation [56] and a positive self-image [57]. Future studies should explore these dynamics of posting on social media, different aspects of focus on self-presentation, and mental health in order to inform interventions to reduce mental health problems among adolescents.

Implications

Only a high focus on self-presentation was associated with a higher risk of symptoms of depression and anxiety in fully adjusted analyses; an intermediate focus did not show this relationship. This finding aligns with other research showing that moderate use of social media is not linked to negative outcomes, and that only high use or high investment is [58,59,60]. For example, one study found that using visual social media such as Instagram and Snapchat for more than two hours each day positively predicted internalizing symptoms, while less than two hours of use each day did not [59]. This implies that the common notion that all social media use is negative for mental health is unwarranted and can lead to unnecessary worrying among adolescents about their social media use. However, the results of the present study suggest that a high focus on self-presentation may increase the risk of mental health problems, and helping adolescents balance their preoccupation with self-presentation, for example using school-based programs, should be a priority. School-based programs that encourages adolescents to reflect and think critically about their own and others’ use of social media, may facilitate higher levels of social media literacy and build their resilience and ability to leverage the potential positive effects of social media, while negating negative effects [61]. Furthermore, in a clinical context, dimensions of social media use such as feedback-seeking and strategic self-presentation are important topics to consider, as there is a possibility that they contribute to a worsening of mental health. However, as social media also give opportunities for social support and friendship formation, particularly for marginalized groups (e.g., [62]), an open-minded approach is warranted. Furthermore, tech producers could help minimize any negative effects of social media use by for instance limiting affordances that trigger upward social comparison and feedback-seeking, such as beauty filters and likes, thus redesigning their social media platforms to support, rather than harm, mental health.

Strengths and limitations

In line with an affordances approach [4], this study focused on key attributes of social media platforms rather than specific social media platforms. Thus, the findings can be applicable to a wide range of platforms now and in the future. Furthermore, the measure of focus on self-presentation on social media was developed based on qualitative interviews with adolescents and adapted based on adolescent feedback, thus increasing the likelihood that the measure covered aspects of social media use that are relevant for adolescents and moving beyond quantity or frequency of self-presentation.

The present study also had some important limitations. Firstly, the validity of the observed longitudinal association rests on the assumption that there were no unobserved time-variant factors impacting the measured variables across the study period. The first difference model accounts for time-invariant factors such as gender, personality, and socioeconomic status, but factors that change over time are not accounted for. For example, given that the data were collected during the COVID-19 pandemic, it is possible that periods of lockdowns have led both to an increase in focus on self-presentation and in symptoms of depression and anxiety, and residual confounding cannot be ruled out. Secondly, the study included only two time points and is therefore limited in terms of determining cause and effect. Thirdly, assessing the relationship between focus on self-presentation and mental health and well-being over a longer time span could possibly have yielded a stronger relationship. To fully disentangle these causal relationships, further research employing large, multi-wave longitudinal studies is warranted. Furthermore, it is possible that the relationship between focus on self-presentation and mental health differs across different developmental periods, and studies should include a wider age range.

Conclusion

This study employed both cross-sectional and longitudinal data to investigate the link between adolescents’ focus on self-presentation on social media and their symptoms of depression, anxiety, and overall well-being. Analysing the data using LCA, we identified three distinct groups characterized by varying degrees of self-presentation focus: low, intermediate, and high. A high focus on self-presentation was associated with more symptoms of anxiety and depression for boys and girls, and with lower well-being for girls in fully adjusted models. Furthermore, an increase in self-presentation focus over time was associated with small increases in depressive and anxiety symptoms, while the effect on well-being was not statistically significant. These findings suggest that a high focus on self-presentation, including behaviours like seeking feedback, strategic self-presentation, and upward social comparisons, is associated with an elevated risk of poor mental health. The observed covariance between that focus on self-presentation and mental health problems underscores a significant relationship warranting further investigation.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analysed during the current study are not publicly available, as they contain sensitive information, and the ethical approval of the study does not include this option. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to GJH, Gunnhildjohnsen.Hjetland@fhi.no.

Abbreviations

SPAUSCIS:

Self-Presentation and Upwards Social Comparison Inclination Scale

GAD-7:

General Anxiety Disorder 7

SMFQ:

Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire

WEMWBS:

Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale

SES:

Socioeconomic status

LCA:

Latent class analysis

CFA:

Confirmatory factor analysis

References

  1. Schønning V, Hjetland GJ, Aarø LE, Skogen JC. Social media use and mental health and well-being among adolescents–a scoping review. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1949.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Valkenburg PM, Meier A, Beyens I. Social media use and its impact on adolescent mental health: an umbrella review of the evidence. Curr Opin Psychol. 2022;44:58–68.

  3. Beyens I, Pouwels JL, van Driel II, Keijsers L, Valkenburg PM. The effect of social media on well-being differs from adolescent to adolescent. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Moreno MA, Uhls YT. Applying an affordances approach and a developmental lens to approach adolescent social media use. Digit Health. 2019;5:2055207619826678.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Orben A, Meier A, Dalgleish T, Blakemore SJ. Mechanisms linking social media use to adolescent mental health vulnerability. Nat Rev Psychol. 2024;3:407–23.

  6. Orben A. Teenagers, screens and social media: a narrative review of reviews and key studies. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2020;55:1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Kross E, Verduyn P, Sheppes G, Costello CK, Jonides J, Ybarra O. Social media and well-being: pitfalls, progress, and next steps. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020.

  8. Boyd D. Why youth (heart) social network sites: the role of networked publics in teenage social life. In: Buckingham D, editor. Youth, identity, and digital media the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation series on digital media and learning. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2008. p. 2007–16.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Hjetland GJ, Schønning V, Hella RT, Veseth M, Skogen JC. How do Norwegian adolescents experience the role of social media in relation to mental health and well-being: a qualitative study. BMC Psychol. 2021;9(1):1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Weinstein E. The social media see-saw: positive and negative influences on adolescents’ affective well-being. New Media Soc. 2018;20(10):3597–623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Skogen JC, Hjetland GJ, Bøe T, Hella RT, Knudsen AK. Through the looking glass of social media. Focus on self-presentation and association with mental health and quality of life. A cross-sectional survey-based study. Oslo: Norwegian Institute of Public Health; 2021. Contract No.: 6.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Goffman E. The presentation of self in everyday life: Harmondsworth London. 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Walther JB, Whitty MT. Language, psychology, and new new media: the hyperpersonal model of mediated communication at twenty-five years. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2021;40(1):120–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Nesi J, Choukas-Bradley S, Prinstein MJ. Transformation of adolescent peer relations in the social media context: part 2—application to peer group processes and future directions for research. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2018;21(3):295–319.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Nesi J, Prinstein MJ. In search of likes: longitudinal associations between adolescents’ digital status seeking and health-risk behaviors. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2018.

  16. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relat. 1954;7(2):117–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Chou HTG, Edge N. “They are happier and having better lives than I am”: the impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’ lives. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2012;15(2):117–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Nesi J, Prinstein MJ. Using social media for social comparison and feedback-seeking: gender and popularity moderate associations with depressive symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2015;43(8):1427–38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Moroney E, Lee SS, Ebbert AM, Luthar SS. Digital media use frequency, online behaviors, and risk for negative mental health in adolescents in high-achieving schools. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022:13591045221108834.

  20. Jarman HK, Marques MD, McLean SA, Slater A, Paxton SJ. Motivations for social media use: associations with social media engagement and body satisfaction and well-being among adolescents. J Youth Adolesc. 2021:1–15.

  21. Hawes T, Zimmer-Gembeck MJ, Campbell SM. Unique associations of social media use and online appearance preoccupation with depression, anxiety, and appearance rejection sensitivity. Body Image. 2020;33:66–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Steele RG, Hall JA, Christofferson JL. Conceptualizing digital stress in adolescents and young adults: toward the development of an empirically based model. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2020;23(1):15–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Winstone L, Mars B, Haworth CM, Heron J, Kidger J. Adolescent social media user types and their mental health and well-being: results from a longitudinal survey of 13–14-year-olds in the United Kingdom. JCPP Adv. 2022;2:e12071.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Maheux AJ, Roberts SR, Nesi J, Widman L, Choukas-Bradley S. Longitudinal associations between appearance-related social media consciousness and adolescents’ depressive symptoms. J Adolesc. 2022;94(2):264–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Hjetland GJ, Finserås TR, Sivertsen B, Colman I, Hella RT, Skogen JC. Focus on self-presentation on social media across sociodemographic variables, lifestyles, and personalities: a cross-sectional study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(17):11133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Nesi J, Choukas-Bradley S, Maheux AJ, Roberts SR, Sanzari CM, Widman L, et al. Selfie appearance investment and peer feedback concern: multimethod investigation of adolescent selfie practices and adjustment. Psychol Pop Media. 2021;10(4):488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(10):1092–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Turner N, Joinson C, Peters TJ, Wiles N, Lewis G. Validity of the short mood and feelings questionnaire in late adolescence. Psychol Assess. 2014;26(3):752.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R, Platt S, Joseph S, Weich S, et al. The Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2007;5(1):63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Ringdal R, Bradley Eilertsen M-E, Bjørnsen HN, Espnes GA, Moksnes UK. Validation of two versions of the Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale among Norwegian adolescents. Scand J Public Health. 2018;46(7):718–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB Jr. A very brief measure of the big-five personality domains. J Res Pers. 2003;37(6):504–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  33. RStudio Team. RStudio: integrated development for R. Boston: RStudio, PBC; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. R package version. 2018.

  35. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48:1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Li C-H. The performance of ML, DWLS, and ULS estimation with robust corrections in structural equation models with ordinal variables. Psychol Methods. 2016;21(3):369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Linzer DA, Lewis JB. poLCA: An R package for polytomous variable latent class analysis. J Stat Softw. 2011;42(1):1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Bøe T, Dearing E, Stormark KM, Zachrisson HD. Subjective economic status in adolescence: determinants and associations with mental health in the Norwegian Youth@ Hordaland Study. J Fam Econ Issues. 2018;39(2):323–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Kang W, Steffens F, Pineda S, Widuch K, Malvaso A. Personality traits and dimensions of mental health. Sci Rep. 2023;13(1):7091.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Linden A. ESIZEREG: stata module for computing the effect size based on a linear regression coefficient. 2024. https://EconPapers.repec.org/RePEc:boc:bocode:s458607.

  41. Croissant Y, Millo G. Panel data economics in R: the plm package. J Stat Softw. 2018;27(2):1–43.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Wooldridge JM. Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. London: MIT press; 2010.

  43. Brown TA, Moore MT. Confirmatory factor analysis. In: Handbook of structural equation modeling. 2012. p. 361–79.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Andrade C. Mean difference, standardized mean difference (SMD), and their use in meta-analysis: as simple as it gets. J Clin Psychiatry. 2020;81(5):11349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Fredrickson BL, Roberts TA. Objectification theory: toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychol Women Q. 1997;21(2):173–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Tiggemann M, Slater A. The role of self-objectification in the mental health of early adolescent girls: predictors and consequences. J Pediatr Psychol. 2015;40(7):704–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Tiggemann M. Mental health risks of self-objectification: a review of the empirical evidence for disordered eating, depressed mood, and sexual dysfunction. 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Jones BA, Griffiths KM. Self-objectification and depression: an integrative systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2015;171:22–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Bian M, Leung L. Linking loneliness, shyness, smartphone addiction symptoms, and patterns of smartphone use to social capital. Soc Sci Comput Rev. 2015;33(1):61–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. O’Day EB, Heimberg RG. Social media use, social anxiety, and loneliness: a systematic review. Comput Hum Behav Rep. 2021;3:100070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Svensson R, Johnson B, Olsson A. Does gender matter? The association between different digital media activities and adolescent well-being. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Orben A, Przybylski AK, Blakemore SJ, Kievit RA. Windows of developmental sensitivity to social media. Nat Commun. 2022;13(1):1649.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Gentile B, Twenge JM, Freeman EC, Campbell WK. The effect of social networking websites on positive self-views: an experimental investigation. Comput Hum Behav. 2012;28(5):1929–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Gonzales AL, Hancock JT. Mirror, mirror on my Facebook wall: effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2011;14(1–2):79–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Escobar-Viera C, Shensa A, Hamm M, Melcher EM, Rzewnicki DI, Egan JE, et al. “I don’t feel like the odd one”: utilizing content analysis to compare the effects of social media use on well-being among sexual minority and nonminority US young adults. Am J Health Promot. 2020;34(3):285–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Toma CL, Hancock JT. Self-affirmation underlies Facebook use. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2013;39(3):321–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Kim J, Lee JER. The Facebook paths to happiness: effects of the number of Facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2011;14(6):359–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Przybylski AK, Weinstein N. A large-scale test of the goldilocks hypothesis: quantifying the relations between digital-screen use and the mental well-being of adolescents. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(2):204–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Marengo D, Longobardi C, Fabris M, Settanni M. Highly-visual social media and internalizing symptoms in adolescence: the mediating role of body image concerns. Comput Hum Behav. 2018;82:63–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Przybylski AK, Orben A, Weinstein N. How much is too much? Examining the relationship between digital screen engagement and psychosocial functioning in a confirmatory cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019.

  61. Schreurs L, Vandenbosch L. Introducing the Social Media Literacy (SMILE) model with the case of the positivity bias on social media. J Child Media. 2021;15(3):320–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Berger MN, Taba M, Marino JL, Lim MS, Skinner SR. Social media use and health and well-being of lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and queer youth: systematic review. J Med Internet Res. 2022;24(9):e38449.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We extend our gratitude to the pupils who took part in the survey, and we appreciate the collaboration and support provided by Bergen municipality and Vestland County Council for this study. Special thanks go to the resource group for their valuable contributions and discussions pertaining to the development of focus group interviews and the questionnaire.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Norwegian Institute of Public Health (FHI) The work of GJH was supported by the Dam Foundation [grant number 2021/FO347287], while the work of TRF, AIOA and JCS was supported by The Research Council of Norway [grant number 319845]. The work of IC was partly supported by the Research Council of Norway through its Centers of Excellence funding scheme, project number 262700. The funding sources were not involved in the study design, in the collection, analysis, or interpretation of the data, or in the writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, GJH and JCS; methodology, GJH and JCS; formal analysis, GJH and JCS; investigation, GJH, RTH, and JCS; writing—original draft preparation, GJH and JCS; writing—review and editing, GJH, TRF, BS, IC, RTH, AIOA, and JCS; project administration, JCS. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gunnhild Johnsen Hjetland.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The data collection was approved by the Regional Ethics Committee (REK) in Norway (reference number REK #65611) and was conducted in compliance with the principles outlined in the Helsinki Declaration. All participants were provided with information about the study’s overall objectives, both digitally and through communication with their teacher, and they provided electronic informed consent when participating. It was also made clear that participants had the option to withdraw from the study at any time. Additionally, all individuals invited to participate were at least 16 years old, granting them the legal capacity to independently provide consent; however, parents or guardians were also informed about the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Dr. Colman has received consulting fees from Meta Inc.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hjetland, G.J., Finserås, T.R., Sivertsen, B. et al. Digital self-presentation and adolescent mental health: Cross-sectional and longitudinal insights from the “LifeOnSoMe”-study. BMC Public Health 24, 2635 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-20052-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-20052-4

Keywords