Skip to main content

The association between screen time exposure and myopia in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis

Abstract

Objective

This study aimed to systematically review epidemiological evidence on associations between screen time exposure and myopia in children and adolescents, and to quantitatively evaluate summary effect estimates from existing literature.

Method

There were three online databases including PubMed, Embase, and Web of Science, for epidemiological studies on screen time exposure and myopia published before June 1, 2023. The risk of bias was assessed by the Newcastle Ottawa Scale (NOS) checklist. Summary odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated to evaluate the correlation between screen time exposure and myopia using random or fixed-effect models by exposure type (categorical/continuous). We also performed subgroup analysis by screen device type, study quality, geographic region, and research period.

Results

We searched 7,571 records from three databases and identified 19 eligible studies, including 14 high-quality studies and 5 moderate-quality studies. Meta-analyses suggested that there was a statistically significant correlation between screen time (high vs. low) and myopia. The pooled ORs with 95%CIs were respectively 2.24 (1.47–3.42) for cross-sectional studies, and 2.39 (2.07–2.76) for cohort studies. We also found a significant association between continuous exposure to screen time (per 1 h/d increase) and myopia in cohort studies. The pooled ORs with 95%CIs were 1.07 (1.01–1.13). In subgroup analysis stratified by screen device type in cross-sectional studies, screen time exposures from computers (categorical: OR = 8.19, 95%CI: 4.78–14.04; continuous: OR = 1.22, 95%CI: 1.10–1.35) and televisions (categorical: OR = 1.46, 95%CI: 1.02–2.10) were associated with myopia, while smartphones were not. Although publication bias was detected, the pooled results did not show significant changes after adjustment using the trim and fill method.

Conclusion

Our findings support that screen time exposure was significantly associated with myopia in children and adolescents. Notably, screen time exposure from computers may have the most significant impact on myopia.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Myopia, also known as near-sightedness or short-sightedness, is one of the refractive errors [1]. The feature of myopia is the excessive elongation of the ocular globe [2]. The risk of pathological eye changes like cataracts, glaucoma, retinal detachment, and macular degeneration can be increased by high myopia, all of which can lead to irreversible vision loss. Consequently, myopia brings further vision challenges [3,4,5,6]. It is evident that environmental factors, such as education, near-work activities, and outdoor activities exert a significant influence on the prevalence of myopia [7,8,9]. In order to effectively prevent and control myopia, it is necessary to explore more potential factors that may affect myopia.

Screen time includes computers, televisions, video games, and other mobile digital devices (e.g., smartphones, laptops, and tablets) [10,11,12]. Screen time exposure has become a ubiquitous part of children’s and adolescents’ day-to-day lives [13]. In recent years, with the popularity of screen devices, children have more chances to be exposed to screen every day, while the age of first exposure to screen time has been reduced [14]. At the same time, the available screen devices and their range of use have rapidly expanded. Television used to be the primary mode of viewing screens in homes, but modern screen devices that children can use include mobile digital devices, such as computers, smartphones, and tablets [15]. Through e-books, games, and custom applications, children can gain more opportunities for cognitive engagement. When combined with the interactive nature of these activities with high-quality educational content, children may derive benefits, including improved language and motor skills, and strengthened social connections [16,17,18]. However, there is sufficient evidence to demonstrate the relationship between excessive screen time and negative health outcomes in children and adolescents, such as hypertension [19], insulin resistance [20], overweight/obesity [21], sleep disorder [10] and depression [22]. In 2018, the Chinese Ministry of Education imposed controls on the use of electronic products, stating that “the single use of electronic products for non-learning purposes should not exceed fifteen minutes, and the cumulative daily use should not exceed one hour” [23]. The World Health Organization (WHO) has proposed a recommendation to limit screen time to no more than two hours per day in 2020 [24]. In this field, the extent to which modern screen time exposure is beneficial or harmful to the health and development of children and adolescents remains an active research topic.

In recent years, an increasing number of epidemiological studies have estimated screen time exposure through self-reported questionnaires. Several studies have shown a positive correlation between screen time exposure and myopia [25,26,27], while others have reported null results [28,29,30]. Given that screen time exposure is almost ubiquitous in modern social life, researchers further elucidate that the relationship between screen time exposure and myopia is a major public health issue. As far as we know, previous systematic reviews have reported that the results of screen time and myopia were mixed [9, 31]. In general, the findings of published observational studies in relevant fields are contradictory, and the literature reviewed and included in relevant reviews need to be further supplemented and improved.

To fill these gaps, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis based observational studies that assessed the relationship between screen time exposure and myopia in children and adolescents. Screen time exposure types include categorical exposure and continuous exposure, and screen device types include computers, televisions, smartphones, laptops and tablets, etc. When data were available, we performed quantitative synthesis to calculate pooled effect estimates. We assessed the risk of bias in individual studies and rated the quality of evidence across studies.

Materials and methods

When conducting this systematic review and meta-analysis, two authors (ZZ and YZ) conducted literature screening, data extraction, risk of bias assessment, evidence synthesis and analysis. A third author (SX) was necessary if there was any disagreement between the two authors. The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) 2020 statement was followed to perform this meta-analysis (Table S1) [32].

Eligibility criteria

According to the PECOS statement, the eligibility criteria for Population, Exposure, Comparator, Outcomes, and Study Design are as follows: (P) studies of children and adolescents; (E) studies on categorical or continuous screen time exposure; (C) studies presenting comparative effect estimates (i.e., compared with children exposed to different levels of screen time) or continuous effect estimates (i.e., the impact on myopia by one hour of screen time increase per day); (O) Studies reporting the incidence or prevalence of myopia in children and adolescents. Adjusted odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for myopia must be used; (S) Study design–published observational studies using cross-sectional, cohort, or case-control study designs. If more than one publication for the same research population was found, the most recent one with the most comprehensive and informative was selected [33].

Search strategy and study selection

We searched PubMed, Embase, and Web of Science for related literature from inception to 1st June, 2023.

The search terms are as follows: (screen OR “digital screen” OR television OR computer OR smartphone OR tablet OR “electronic device”) AND (myopia OR “refractive error” OR “near sightless”) AND (children OR teenagers OR adolescents) (see Table S2 for details). The database search was restricted to studies published in English. Additionally, references and related reviews of identified articles were manually scanned, and a follow-up search was conducted prior to manuscript submission to identify qualified published data.

Studies were excluded from this study if their characteristics were as follows: (I) reviews, letters, or commentaries; (II) occupational exposure studies; (III) non-human studies; (IV) ecological studies; (V) studies without available effect estimates for this meta-analysis. After removing duplicated studies, two authors (ZZ and YZ) screened titles, abstracts, and full text for eligibility using EndNote X9. If there was any discrepancy between the two authors, a third author (SX) was required.

Data extraction and quality assessment

From each study, two investigators (ZZ and YZ) independently extracted the following information: first author and publication year, country and geographic region, study design, sample size and age, data source, screen device type, definition and examination of myopia, main results (ORs and 95%CIs), and adjustment for confounders. In order to gather unpublished data, a direct contact was made with authors when it was considered appropriate. The most fully adjusted effect estimate was used to perform the meta-analysis, which represents the greatest control over potential confounders. Data on screen time exposure were collected through questionnaires in the included studies.

Newcastle Ottawa Scale (NOS) evaluations, based on nine-star scoring system (one star represents one score), were conducted for cohort, panel, and case-crossover studies. And an adapted form of NOS for cross-sectional studies was also applied in this study [34]. The NOS consists of three categories: outcomes, comparability, and selection. An NOS score of 0 to 10 was used to evaluate studies, with a score greater than or equal to 7 suggesting high quality, a score between 5 and 6 indicating moderate quality, and a score less than or equal to 4 indicating low quality [35]. More detailed criteria for risk of bias assessment can be found in Table S3-S4.

Statistical analysis

In this meta-analysis of observational studies, R 4.3.1 software, including “meta” and “metafor” packages was used for statistical analysis. In this study, we applied the high versus low category method for studies with categorical exposures after a preliminary assessment of included studies [36]. For continuous exposure, we also extracted and standardized effect estimates for each 1 h/d increase in screen time exposure. We calculated log OR and standard error (SE) using OR and 95% CI that had been reported for the relationship between screen time exposure and the myopia in children and teenagers. Then, heterogeneity analysis was performed using Cochrane Q’ test and inconsistency index (I2) [37]. The fixed-effect model for analysis is used if P ≥ 0.05 and I2 ≤ 50%, while I2 ≤ 25% indicates low heterogeneity and 25–50% indicates moderate heterogeneity among included studies; the random-effect model for analysis is used if P < 0.05 and I2 > 50%, which indicate high heterogeneity among studies [38]. We performed subgroup analysis stratified by screen device type, study quality, geographic region, and research period. We divided the research period into before and after 2008, because the widespread use of smart devices can be traced back to after 2008, especially with the development of smartphones [39]. In our study, sensitivity analysis using the leave-one-out method was applied for the purpose of testing the meta-analysis results’ robustness. We applied funnel plots, and Egger’s test, to evaluate the publication bias of the outcome [40]. The presence of publication bias is suggested if the funnel plot is significantly asymmetric by visual inspection, as well as the P value of Egger’s test is greater than 0.05. If publication bias is observed, the trim and fill method will be applied to correct the pooled results [41].

Results

Characteristics of included studies

The flow chart of the literature search was presented in Fig. 1. A total of 6,493 articles were identified from PubMed, Embase, and Web of Science. 1,159 duplicated studies were excluded. After deleting 5,295 unrelated studies during the screening by the titles and abstracts, 39 articles were assessed for eligibility. After scanning the full text, 20 articles were excluded due to univariate analysis, no available data, no myopia prevalence, etc. (see Table S5 for details of exclusion). Finally, we included 19 eligible studies.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The flowchart of literature search

In total, 102,360 participants were involved in all included studies, 91,282 in cross-sectional studies (N = 15) and 11,078 in cohort studies (N = 4). Meanwhile, 13 (68%) studies used cycloplegic refraction, three studies (16%) used self-reported myopia [28, 51, 52], and three studies (16%) performed optometry with a noncycloplegic state [27, 53, 54]. The included studies were from nine countries, two studies (10%) were conducted in North America [27, 54], seven (38%) in Europe [25, 28,29,30, 45, 46, 52], six (32%) in East Asia [43, 44, 47, 48, 50, 53], two (10%) in South Asia [26, 49], and two (10%) in Southeast Asia [42, 51] (Table 1).

Table 1 Characteristics of included studies

Assessment of risk of bias

As presented in Table S6, according to the NOS checklist, 14 studies (74%) with a score ≥ 7 stars were considered high quality [26, 29, 30, 42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51, 53], while the remaining five studies (26%) with 5 or 6 starts were considered moderate quality [25, 27, 28, 52, 54]. The results of bias risk assessment using the NOS checklist revealed the following possible sources of bias: the sample size included in six (32%) studies was relatively small [25, 29, 30, 47, 52, 53]; five studies (26%) had insufficient strategies to deal with confounding factors (e.g. gender or age) [25, 27, 28, 46, 54], while five studies (26%) lacked adjustment for key confounding factors (e.g. outdoor activities) [28, 30, 42, 44, 52]; five studies (26%) did not use cycloplegic refraction to confirm myopia cases [28, 51,52,53,54]; four studies (21%) did not explicitly demonstrate in the main text that there were no results of interest at the beginning of the study [42, 45, 50, 54].

Screen time exposure and myopia

A total of 11 studies involving 90,415 participants were related to the relationship between categorical exposure to screen time (high vs. low) and myopia in children and adolescents [25,26,27,28, 43,44,45,46, 49, 50, 53]. We found a significantly higher odds ratio of myopia in the highest category of screen time exposure in cross-sectional studies (OR = 2.24, 95%CI: 1.47–3.42), compared to the lowest category (Fig. 2A). A significant relationship between categorical exposure to screen time and myopia was also observed in cohort studies (OR = 2.39, 95%CI: 2.07–2.79) (Fig. 2B). In the subgroup analysis stratified by screen device type, we found screen time exposure (high vs. low) from computers (OR = 8.19, 95%CI: 4.78–14.04) and televisions (OR = 1.46, 95%CI: 1.02–2.10) were significantly related to myopia in cross-sectional studies, while not from smartphones (OR = 1.94, 95%CI: 0.70–5.39) (Figure S1). According to the subgroup analysis stratified by quality assessment, in cross-sectional studies, we observed significant associations in the high quality group (OR = 2.94, 95%CI: 1.50–5.76), while the moderate quality group not (OR = 1.59, 95%CI: 0.85–2.97) (Figure S2). In the subgroup analysis stratified by geographic region, we found significant associations in East Asia (OR = 1.62, 95%CI: 1.07–2.45), and South Asia (OR = 8.19, 95%CI: 4.78–14.04), while not in Europe and America (OR = 1.59, 95%CI: 0.85–2.97) (Figure S3). In addition, we performed a subgroup analysis stratified by research period, and observed significant associations in the research period after 2008 group (OR = 2.99, 95%CI: 1.67–5.35), while the research period before 2008 group not (OR = 1.08, 95%CI: 0.91–1.29). Due to the limited number of studies included, we did not perform subgroup analysis in cohort studies.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot for the association between screen time exposure and risk of myopia in children and adolescents. (A) screen time (high vs. low) and myopia in cross-sectional studies; (B) screen time (high vs. low) and myopia in cohort studies; (C) screen time (per 1 h/d increase) and myopia in cross-sectional studies; (D) screen time (per 1 h/d increase) and myopia in cohort studies

A total of 8 studies involving 11,925 participants were included for the association between continuous exposure (per 1 h/d increase) to screen time and myopia in children and adolescents [29, 30, 47, 48, 51, 52, 54, 55]. We found no association between screen time exposure with 1-hour increase per day and myopia in cross-sectional studies (OR = 1.15, 95%CI: 0.97–1.37) (Fig. 2C), however, a significant association was observed in cohort studies (OR = 1.07, 95%CI: 1.01–1.13) (Fig. 2D). According to the subgroup analysis stratified by screen device type, we observed significant associations between screen time exposure (per 1 h/d increase) from computers (OR = 1.22, 95%CI: 1.10–1.35) was associated with myopia in cross-sectional studies, while not from smartphones (OR = 1.78, 95%CI: 0.40–7.97) (Figure S5). We did not perform subgroup analysis stratified by quality assessment and research period for continuous exposure to screen time and myopia, as all relevant studies were considered high quality and their research period was after 2008. Furthermore, subgroup analysis stratified by geographic region showed significant associations in East Asia (OR = 1.22, 95%CI: 1.10–1.35), while not in Europe and America (OR = 1.15, 95%CI: 0.75–4.37) (Figure S6).

Publication bias and sensitivity analysis

Funnel plots and Egger’s test were performed to estimate publication bias in cross-sectional studies. Due to the limited number of studies included, we were unable to perform publication bias detection and sensitivity analysis for cohort studies. In cross-sectional studies, publication bias was detected in the screen time (high vs. low)-myopia group (Fig. 3A). The P value of Egger’s test in this group was 0.012, indicating the existence of publication bias. After trim and fill analysis, the pooled OR (95%CI) was 1.34 (1.18–1.52) in this exposure-outcome group. A similar situation was also found in screen time (per 1 h/d increase) and myopia (Fig. 3B). The P value of Egger’s test in this group was 0.028, indicating the existence of publication bias. After trim and fill analysis, the pooled OR (95%CI) was 1.02 (0.86–1.20). Our sensitivity analysis using the leave-one-out method presented that the combined results are generally robust in the screen time (high vs. low)-myopia group (Fig. 4A), while not in the screen time (per 1 h/d increase)-myopia group (Fig. 4B).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Funnel plots show the risk of publication bias in the meta-analysis. (A) screen time (high vs. low) and myopia in cross-sectional studies; (B) screen time (per 1 h/d increase) and myopia in cross-sectional studies

Fig. 4
figure 4

Sensitivity analysis using leave-one-out method for the association between screen time exposure and myopia. (A) screen time (high vs. low) and myopia in cross-sectional studies; (B) screen time (per 1 h/d increase) and myopia in cross-sectional studies

Discussion

In recent years, the influence of screen time exposure on children’s vision has attracted worldwide attention. Our study provided significant evidence for the correlation between screen time exposure and myopia. To our knowledge, this study is the largest and most comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis of screen time exposure and myopia in children and adolescents. Meanwhile, this study provided evidence for the association between screen time exposure from different devices and myopia. Specifically, we acknowledge that the degree of association between the use of certain screen devices and myopia is relatively small. However, it should be pointed out that considering the widespread exposure to screen time, the combined effects at the population level may translate into a noticeable rise in the number of myopic individuals.

In this study, we conducted a systematic review to examine the association between screen time exposure and myopia in children and adolescents. The findings of this meta-analysis showed that categorical exposure and continuous exposure to screen time were both related to myopia. What’s more, we discovered that screen time exposures from computers and televisions were significantly associated with myopia, while smartphones were not. This discovery may indicate that computer screens were more capable of generating myopic signals than other screen devices. Of course, the heterogeneity contained in these analytical models deserve a careful explanation of the results. According to the view from Swiatczak and Schaeffel, smartphones may not be likely to cause myopia because their letter sizes minimize center-surround effects, which may be important in linking reading to myopia development [56]. Currently, due to the digitalization of education, controlling computer screen time may be more challenging than controlling the use of digital smart devices, which are often used more for leisure [31].

Our findings differ from previous systematic reviews in several aspects. For example, a meta-analysis carried out by Foreman et al. (2021) included 11 observational studies (including five prospective cohort studies), and found that the use of smart devices may be related to myopia prevalence [31]. However, Lanca et al. (2020) reviewed five observational studies (including two prospective cohort studies), and reported that there was no statistical significance between screen time exposure and myopia (pooled OR = 1.02, 95 CI% 0.96–1.08) [57]. Foreman et al. believed that the reason for this result was that some insignificant ORs in their models came from significant OR transformations in the source article, which may have led to the absence of associations observed in Lanca’s meta-analysis [31]. At the same time, both meta-analyses combined continuous and categorical data of screen time exposure together without separating them, which may lead to deviations in statistical results. In addition, Foreman et al. and Lanca et al. did not include television viewing as part of screen time, and key studies on the relationship between television screen time and myopia included in our study were excluded from their meta-analysis. Of course, it is permissible for these systematic reviews to make inconsistent conclusions considering the differences in test methods for screen time and the confounder of near work. Furthermore, it should be highlighted that the majority of the research included in this analysis was published within the last five years, making this study timely, comprehensive, and significant for public health.

Most of the studies included in this study confirmed myopia cases through cycloplegic refraction. However, several studies have obtained relevant data in the form of self-reported myopia through questionnaires [28, 51, 52, 55]. Self-reported myopia assessments may be used for two different aspects. Firstly, self-reported measurements enable quick surveys and extensive population coverage. For example, this is particularly critical during the COVID-19 epidemic, as face-to-face expert ophthalmic examinations are not feasible considering safe distance measures and the closure of optometry clinics [55]. Secondly, a previous study by Cumberland et al. showed no statistically significant difference between detailed ophthalmic assessment and self-reported myopia, and suggested using self-reported outcomes as a reasonable and accurate alternative method to confirm refractive status in a large sample size population-based questionnaire [58].

It is necessary to incorporate the analysis of the relationship between screen usage and myopia in historical and social contexts. Our subgroup analysis revealed that screen time exposure was associated with myopia in East Asia and South Asia, while in Europe and America not. Meanwhile, we also observed that screen time was associated with myopia after 2008. The epidemic of myopia was well-established in most of those Asia regions, such as Taiwan, Hongkong, Singapore, and Chinese mainland, before screen devices were in large-scale use [59,60,61,62]. It is important to recognize that the Internet did not become publicly accessible until 1993, and digital smart devices did not emerge until 2008 [39]. This has significant implications for myopia prevention strategies. If traditional book reading could lead to a widespread increase in myopia, then a prevention approach focused on reducing screen time in favor of book reading might not be effective. Global education authorities tend to focus on screen time as an issue, probably because it means they can avoid solving the more complex problems around total near-work and outdoor time. Similar to reading and studying (e.g., doing homework, and writing), using computers, playing video games, watching TV, and using smartphones/tablets are also considered near-work activities [7, 31]. More time spent on near-work activities was associated with higher odds of myopia [7]. This implies that a more crucial objective should be to limit the duration and frequency of near-work activities, regardless of the medium used. According to Morgan et al.‘s opinion, if future studies confirm a definite causal relationship between screen time and myopia, there is a chance that restricting screen device usage alone could result in children returning to previous habits that contributed to myopia, such as long-time near work associated with reading books and writing [39]. Without supplementary measures, limiting digital device usage may not significantly affect myopia rates. Furthermore, screen usage could also be harmful if it diverts children from spending time outdoors [25]. Simply limiting screen time may have little effect on preventing myopia in children and adolescents. Active promotion of outdoor activity during daylight may help delay the onset and slow myopia progression in childhood and adolescence [63, 64]. Previous epidemiological studies have provided longitudinal evidence and cross-sectional evidence of a protective effect of outdoor time [65, 66]. Rose et al. highlighted the potential for preventive measures, suggesting a well-defined biological pathway that could mediate a causal effect. Specifically, they proposed that exposure to brighter outdoor light could enhance dopamine release, subsequently inhibiting axial elongation of the eyeball [66]. This proposition is supported by compelling evidence from animal studies, which demonstrate that increased ambient light exposure can trigger these beneficial neurochemical and anatomical changes [67, 68]. The increased time outdoors has become the leading approach to the prevention of the onset of myopia. If the established pattern of spending more time indoors persists, it is necessary to take active steps to encourage children to go outdoors.

Myopia is rare among children with limited or no access to schooling. However, as education systems advance and more children receive formal education, the incidence of myopia after 12 years of schooling rises to approximately 20% [39]. In the school environment, there’s a tendency for children with longer durations of education to exhibit higher rates of myopia. Moreover, among students at the same educational level, those who excel academically and pursue more rigorous academic paths tend to have a higher prevalence of myopia [59]. Mendelian randomization analyses have indicated the influence of educational attainment on myopia [69,70,71]. The increase in myopia prevalence throughout school education is mainly due to the increase in environmental risk factors encountered during the school year, rather than simply from advancing age [72]. The epidemiological link between additional tutoring and the development of myopia has also been studied [59]. Especially in South Korea, it is now estimated that more than 75% of schoolchildren receive some private tutoring, for nearly 7 h a week [73]. In 2006, the cost of additional tutoring for parents in South Korea amounted to 2.57% of the country’s gross domestic product (GDP), surpassing the government’s education budget and representing nearly 20% of household income [74]. By 2016, South Korea boasted over 70,000 registered private tutoring institutions, with a majority located in Seoul [75]. This could potentially elucidate the 96.5% prevalence of myopia among young males in Seoul [76]. With the development of technology and GDP, educational pressure may lead students to use screen devices such as smartphones and computers for long periods of time to learn online courses, receive additional tutoring or practice, and increase the incidence of myopia.

The main advantage of this study is that it provides the latest and most comprehensive evidence on the association between screen time exposure and myopia in children and adolescents. Second, we pooled effect estimates from categorical and continuous exposure to screen time separately. Previous meta-analyses in related fields did not consider to separate processing of categorical data and continuous data, and this would compromise the accuracy of pooled results [31, 57]. Third, this meta-analysis extracted data from multivariate-adjusted models, which to some extent reduced the influence of confounding factors. Further, compared to previous meta-analyses, we conducted an in-depth analysis for screen device type, study quality geographic region, and research period to reveal potential patterns and influencing factors, thereby providing more details and insights for understanding this field.

Despite these advantages, some limitations should also be mentioned. First, the majority of the studies included in this study are cross-sectional studies, and the associations reported in these studies cannot prove causal relationships. Second, we found high heterogeneity in included cross-sectional studies, which may be due to differences in research design, population characteristics, exposure levels, and outcome evaluation in included literature, or perhaps heterogeneity comes from trying to add activities that really are not functioning in the same way, making a cumulative estimate inappropriate; due to the small number of included cohort studies and over 90% of the weight coming from one study, this may result in statistical analysis not being able to have accurate and reliable pooled effect estimates and heterogeneity. Third, all included studies are divergent in their measurement methods and geographic regions, the results of self-reported myopia or lack of ciliary muscle paralysis may not be as accurate as those of studies performing ciliary muscle paralysis; research in developing countries provides different myopia prevalence compared to developed countries, however, a meta-analysis works best when studies are expected to give broadly similar results. Unavoidable differences in the included studies can affect the effectiveness of meta-analysis in this study. Fourth, the studies included in this study were all based on data obtained from self-reported questionnaires for screen time. Considering that participants tend to underestimate their screen time, the study may be influenced by recall bias [77]. Fifth, adequate outdoor time has been proven to be a protective factor for myopia. However, some of the literature we included in the meta-analysis did not adjust for important confounding factors such as outdoor activities or outdoor time, which may interfere with the final results [8]. Finally, because most of the literature included did not provide effect values for the association between screen distance and myopia, we are unable to further explore the impact of usage distance from screen devices such as computers, televisions, and smartphones on myopia.

The results of this study support the hypothesis that screen time exposure may be related to myopia in children and adolescents, but the direction and degree of these associations may vary depending on the type of screen device and individual screen time. More epidemiological research, particularly prospective cohort studies, is needed to confirm these potential associations, nevertheless. Here are some ideas for resolving the remaining open issues in this area:

  • In addition to screen time exposure from computers and televisions, the independent impact of smartphones and tablets on myopia has not been fully explored, and there is a lack of research in related fields. And compared to other screen devices, smartphones and tablets have closer viewing distances, so further independent evaluation should be conducted in future research.

  • At present, almost all screen time data is obtained through self-reported questionnaires. In order to reduce the impact of recall bias, a feasible method is to install an application on the screen device that can track real-time usage, allowing accurate investigation of the dose-dependent impact of screen time on myopia in longitudinal studies [31].

  • In included studies, cross-sectional methods are still widely used to describe screen time exposure and myopia, and further cohort studies are needed to clarify its potential causal relationship.

  • More systematic investigations on categorical exposure and continuous exposure to screen time are needed.

Data availability

Upon reasonable request, the corresponding author (Shaojun Xu) will provide the dataset generated and/or analyzed in the current study (Email: xushaojun@ahmu.edu.cn).

References

  1. Morgan IG, Ohno-Matsui K, Saw SM. Myopia. Lancet. 2012;379(9827):1739–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Liu Y, Wang L, Xu Y, Pang Z, Mu G. The influence of the choroid on the onset and development of myopia: from perspectives of choroidal thickness and blood flow. Acta Ophthalmol. 2021;99(7):730–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Yang J, Ouyang X, Fu H, Hou X, Liu Y, Xie Y, Yu H, Wang G. Advances in biomedical study of the myopia-related signaling pathways and mechanisms. Biomed Pharmacother. 2022;145:112472.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ha A, Kim CY, Shim SR, Chang IB, Kim YK. Degree of myopia and glaucoma risk: a dose-response meta-analysis. Am J Ophthalmol. 2022;236:107–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bullimore MA, Ritchey ER, Shah S, Leveziel N, Bourne RRA, Flitcroft DI. The risks and benefits of myopia control. Ophthalmology. 2021;128(11):1561–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Clark R, Lee SS, Du R, Wang Y, Kneepkens SCM, Charng J, Huang Y, Hunter ML, Jiang C, Tideman JWL, et al. A new polygenic score for refractive error improves detection of children at risk of high myopia but not the prediction of those at risk of myopic macular degeneration. EBioMedicine. 2023;91:104551.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Huang HM, Chang DS, Wu PC. The association between near work activities and myopia in children-a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(10):e0140419.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Xiong S, Sankaridurg P, Naduvilath T, Zang J, Zou H, Zhu J, Lv M, He X, Xu X. Time spent in outdoor activities in relation to myopia prevention and control: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Acta Ophthalmol. 2017;95(6):551–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Baird PN, Saw SM, Lanca C, Guggenheim JA, Smith Iii EL, Zhou X, Matsui KO, Wu PC, Sankaridurg P, Chia A et al. Myopia. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2020;6(1):99.

  10. Hale L, Guan S. Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Sleep Med Rev. 2015;21:50–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Horowitz-Kraus T, Hutton JS. Brain connectivity in children is increased by the time they spend reading books and decreased by the length of exposure to screen-based media. Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway: 1992). 2018;107(4):685–693.

  12. Eirich R, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, McGuinness C, Christakis DA, Madigan S. Association of screen time with internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in children 12 years or younger: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2022;79(5):393–405.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Byrne R, Terranova CO, Trost SG. Measurement of screen time among young children aged 0–6 years: a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2021;22(8):e13260.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Dumuid D. Screen time in early childhood. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2020;4(3):169–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kilic AO, Sari E, Yucel H, Oguz MM, Polat E, Acoglu EA, Senel S. Exposure to and use of mobile devices in children aged 1–60 months. Eur J Pediatr. 2019;178(2):221–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Trost SG, Brookes DSK. Effectiveness of a novel digital application to promote fundamental movement skills in 3- to 6-year-old children: a randomized controlled trial. J Sports Sci. 2021;39(4):453–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Council On C, Media: media and young minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5).

  19. Wyszynska J, Podgorska-Bednarz J, Deren K, Mazur A. The relationship between physical activity and screen time with the risk of hypertension in children and adolescents with intellectual disability. Biomed Res Int. 2017;2017:1940602.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Nightingale CM, Rudnicka AR, Donin AS, Sattar N, Cook DG, Whincup PH, Owen CG. Screen time is associated with adiposity and insulin resistance in children. Arch Dis Child. 2017;102(7):612–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Fang K, Mu M, Liu K, He Y. Screen time and childhood overweight/obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Care Health Dev. 2019;45(5):744–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Li L, Zhang Q, Zhu L, Zeng G, Huang H, Zhuge J, Kuang X, Yang S, Yang D, Chen Z, et al. Screen time and depression risk: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:1058572.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Geiger SD, Xiao J, Shankar A. Positive association between perfluoroalkyl chemicals and hyperuricemia in children. Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(11):1255–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. WHO. WHO guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour [EB /OL].(2020-02-09) [2021-12-30]. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/336656

  25. Harrington S, O’Dwyer V. The association between time spent on screens and reading with myopia, premyopia and ocular biometric and anthropometric measures in 6- to 7-year-old schoolchildren in Ireland. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2023;43(3):505–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Saxena R, Vashist P, Tandon R, Pandey RM, Bhardawaj A, Menon V, Mani K. Prevalence of myopia and its risk factors in urban school children in Delhi: the North India myopia study (NIM study). PLoS ONE. 2015;10(2):e0117349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Chiang SY, Weng TH, Lin CM, Lin SM. Ethnic disparity in prevalence and associated risk factors of myopia in adolescents. J Formos Med Assoc. 2020;119(1 Pt 1):134–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Schuster AK, Krause L, Kuchenbacker C, Prutz F, Elflein HM, Pfeiffer N, Urschitz MS. Prevalence and time trends in myopia among children and adolescents. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2020;117(50):855–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Berticat C, Mamouni S, Ciais A, Villain M, Raymond M, Daien V. Probability of myopia in children with high refined carbohydrates consumption in France. BMC Ophthalmol. 2020;20(1):337.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Hagen LA, Gjelle JVB, Arnegard S, Pedersen HR, Gilson SJ, Baraas RC. Prevalence and possible factors of myopia in Norwegian adolescents. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):13479.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Foreman J, Salim AT, Praveen A, Fonseka D, Ting DSW, Guang He M, Bourne RRA, Crowston J, Wong TY, Dirani M. Association between digital smart device use and myopia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Digit Health. 2021;3(12):e806–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, Shamseer L, Tetzlaff JM, Akl EA, Brennan SE, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. J Clin Epidemiol. 2021;134:178–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Gui S-Y, Qiao J-C, Xu K-X, Li Z-L, Chen Y-N, Wu K-J, Jiang Z-X, Hu C-Y. Association between per- and polyfluoroalkyl substances exposure and risk of diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol. 2022;33(1):40–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Herzog R, Alvarez-Pasquin MJ, Diaz C, Del Barrio JL, Estrada JM, Gil A. Are healthcare workers’ intentions to vaccinate related to their knowledge, beliefs and attitudes? A systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Sahebjada S, Al-Mahrouqi HH, Moshegov S, Panchatcharam SM, Chan E, Daniell M, Baird PN. Eye rubbing in the aetiology of keratoconus: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2021;259(8):2057–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Wu Y, Gui SY, Fang Y, Zhang M, Hu CY. Exposure to outdoor light at night and risk of breast cancer: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Environ Pollut. 2021;269:116114.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Azami M, Salamati M, Ranjbar R, Sahebkar A. The association between metabolic syndrome and erosive esophagitis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. EXCLI J. 2021;20:1532–43.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1539–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Morgan IG, Jan CL. China turns to school reform to control the myopia epidemic: a narrative review. Asia Pac J Ophthalmol (Phila). 2022;11(1):27–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Chua SY, Ikram MK, Tan CS, Lee YS, Ni Y, Shirong C, Gluckman PD, Chong YS, Yap F, Wong TY, et al. Relative contribution of risk factors for early-onset myopia in young Asian children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(13):8101–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Guan H, Yu NN, Wang H, Boswell M, Shi Y, Rozelle S, Congdon N. Impact of various types of near work and time spent outdoors at different times of day on visual acuity and refractive error among Chinese school-going children. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(4):e0215827.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Guo L, Yang J, Mai J, Du X, Guo Y, Li P, Yue Y, Tang D, Lu C, Zhang WH. Prevalence and associated factors of myopia among primary and middle school-aged students: a school-based study in Guangzhou. Eye (Lond). 2016;30(6):796–804.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hansen MH, Laigaard PP, Olsen EM, Skovgaard AM, Larsen M, Kessel L, Munch IC. Low physical activity and higher use of screen devices are associated with myopia at the age of 16–17 years in the CCC2000 eye study. Acta Ophthalmol. 2020;98(3):315–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Harrington SC, Stack J, O’Dwyer V. Risk factors associated with myopia in schoolchildren in Ireland. Br J Ophthalmol. 2019;103(12):1803–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Liu S, Ye S, Xi W, Zhang X. Electronic devices and myopic refraction among children aged 6–14 years in urban areas of Tianjin, China. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2019;39(4):282–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Qian DJ, Zhong H, Li J, Niu Z, Yuan Y, Pan CW. Myopia among school students in rural China (Yunnan). Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2016;36(4):381–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Singh NK, James RM, Yadav A, Kumar R, Asthana S, Labani S. Prevalence of myopia and associated risk factors in schoolchildren in North India. Optom Vis Sci. 2019;96(3):200–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Tsai TH, Liu YL, Ma IH, Su CC, Lin CW, Lin LL, Hsiao CK, Wang IJ. Evolution of the prevalence of myopia among Taiwanese schoolchildren: a review of survey data from 1983 through 2017. Ophthalmology. 2021;128(2):290–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Toh SH, Coenen P, Howie EK, Mukherjee S, Mackey DA, Straker LM. Mobile touch screen device use and associations with musculoskeletal symptoms and visual health in a nationally representative sample of Singaporean adolescents. Ergonomics. 2019;62(6):778–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. McCrann S, Loughman J, Butler JS, Paudel N, Flitcroft DI. Smartphone use as a possible risk factor for myopia. Clin Exp Optom. 2021;104(1):35–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Xie Z, Long Y, Wang J, Li Q, Zhang Q. Prevalence of myopia and associated risk factors among primary students in Chongqing: multilevel modeling. BMC Ophthalmol. 2020;20(1):146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Deng L, Gwiazda J, Thorn F. Children’s refractions and visual activities in the school year and summer. Optom Vis Sci. 2010;87(6):406–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Liu J, Li B, Sun Y, Chen Q, Dang J. Adolescent vision health during the outbreak of COVID-19: Association between digital screen use and myopia progression. Front Pediatr. 2021;9:662984.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Swiatczak B, Schaeffel F. Transient eye shortening during reading text with inverted contrast: effects of refractive error and letter size. Transl Vis Sci Technol. 2022;11(4):17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Lanca C, Saw SM. The association between digital screen time and myopia: a systematic review. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2020;40(2):216–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Cumberland PM, Chianca A, Rahi JS, Eye UKB, Vision C. Accuracy and utility of self-report of refractive error. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2016;134(7):794–801.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Morgan IG, French AN, Ashby RS, Guo X, Ding X, He M, Rose KA. The epidemics of myopia: aetiology and prevention. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2018;62:134–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Holden BA, Fricke TR, Wilson DA, Jong M, Naidoo KS, Sankaridurg P, Wong TY, Naduvilath TJ, Resnikoff S. Global prevalence of myopia and high myopia and temporal trends from 2000 through 2050. Ophthalmology. 2016;123(5):1036–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Dolgin E. The myopia boom. Nature. 2015;519(7543):276–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Yusufu M, Bukhari J, Yu X, Lin TPH, Lam DSC, Wang N. Challenges in eye care in the Asia-Pacific Region. Asia Pac J Ophthalmol (Phila). 2021;10(5):423–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Zadnik K, Mutti DO. Outdoor activity protects against childhood myopia-let the sun shine in. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(5):415–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Jin JX, Hua WJ, Jiang X, Wu XY, Yang JW, Gao GP, Fang Y, Pei CL, Wang S, Zhang JZ, et al. Effect of outdoor activity on myopia onset and progression in school-aged children in northeast China: the Sujiatun eye care study. BMC Ophthalmol. 2015;15:73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Jones LA, Sinnott LT, Mutti DO, Mitchell GL, Moeschberger ML, Zadnik K. Parental history of myopia, sports and outdoor activities, and future myopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48(8):3524–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Rose KA, Morgan IG, Ip J, Kifley A, Huynh S, Smith W, Mitchell P. Outdoor activity reduces the prevalence of myopia in children. Ophthalmology. 2008;115(8):1279–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Landis EG, Park HN, Chrenek M, He L, Sidhu C, Chakraborty R, Strickland R, Iuvone PM, Pardue MT. Ambient light regulates retinal dopamine signaling and myopia susceptibility. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2021;62(1):28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Stone RA, Cohen Y, McGlinn AM, Davison S, Casavant S, Shaffer J, Khurana TS, Pardue MT, Iuvone PM. Development of experimental myopia in chicks in a natural environment. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57(11):4779–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Cuellar-Partida G, Lu Y, Kho PF, Hewitt AW, Wichmann HE, Yazar S, Stambolian D, Bailey-Wilson JE, Wojciechowski R, Wang JJ, et al. Assessing the genetic predisposition of education on myopia: a Mendelian randomization study. Genet Epidemiol. 2016;40(1):66–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Mountjoy E, Davies NM, Plotnikov D, Smith GD, Rodriguez S, Williams CE, Guggenheim JA, Atan D. Education and myopia: assessing the direction of causality by Mendelian randomisation. BMJ. 2018;361k2022.

  71. Plotnikov D, Williams C, Atan D, Davies NM, Ghorbani Mojarrad N, Guggenheim JA, Eye UKB, Vision C. Effect of education on myopia: evidence from the United Kingdom ROSLA 1972 reform. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2020;61(11):7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Ding X, Morgan IG, Hu Y, Yuan Z, He M. Exposure to the life of a school child rather than age determines myopic shifts in refraction in school children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2022;63(3):15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Three-Fourths of South Korean Students Get Private Tutoring. Survey. https://en.yna.co.kr/view/AEN20200429008900315. Accessed November 6, 2021.

  74. Private Tutoring Thrives During Debate Over Efficacy of Public Education. https://en.yna.co.kr/view/AEN20070507003200315. Accessed November 6, 2021.

  75. F. K: China Clamps Down on Cram Schools, but South Korea is a Different Story. https://www.dw.com/en/china-clamps-downon-cram-schools-but-south-korea-is-a-different-story/a-58760942. Accessed November 6, 2021.

  76. Jung SK, Lee JH, Kakizaki H, Jee D. Prevalence of myopia and its association with body stature and educational level in 19-year-old male conscripts in Seoul, South Korea. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(9):5579–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Lee H, Ahn H, Nguyen TG, Choi SW, Kim DJ. Comparing the self-report and measured smartphone usage of college students: a pilot study. Psychiatry Investig. 2017;14(2):198–204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the Educational Commission of Anhui Province of China (Grant No. KJ2021ZD0039), Research Fund of Anhui Institute of Translational Medicine (Grant No. 2023zhyx-B13), and National Nature Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 2021YFC2702102, 2021YFC2702105).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ZZ and SX conceived the idea, performed the statistical analysis, and drafted this meta-analysis. ZZ and YZ selected and searched the relevant papers. ZZ and YT assessed each study. ZZ and SX are the guarantors of the overall content. JQ provided the code for analysis. SX supervised the entire study process and contributed to the critical revision of the manuscript. All authors revised and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shaojun Xu.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Authorship contributions

ZZ and SX conceived the idea, performed the statistical analysis, and drafted this meta-analysis. ZZ, and YZ selected and searched the relevant papers. ZZ and YT assessed each study. ZZ and SX are the guarantors of the overall content. JQ provided the code for analysis. SX supervised the entire study process and contributed to the critical revision of the manuscript. All authors revised and approved the final manuscript.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zong, Z., Zhang, Y., Qiao, J. et al. The association between screen time exposure and myopia in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. BMC Public Health 24, 1625 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-19113-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-19113-5

Keywords