Skip to main content

Visual memory and alcohol use in a middle-aged birth cohort

This article has been updated

Abstract

Light and moderate alcohol use has been reported to be associated with both impaired and enhanced cognition. The purpose of this study was to explore whether there was a linear relationship between visual memory and alcohol consumption in males and females in a large middle-aged birth cohort population in cross-sectional and longitudinal settings. Data were collected from 5585 participants completing 31-year (1997–1998) and 46-year (2012–2014) follow-ups including Paired Associate Learning (PAL) test at 46-years follow-up. The participants were originally from 12,231 study population of the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1966 (NFBC1966). The PAL test was conducted to assess visual memory. Reported alcohol use was measured as total daily use of alcohol, beer, wine, and spirits converted into grams and as frequency and amount of use of beer, wine, and spirits. The total daily alcohol use was not associated with reduced visual memory. The frequency of use of beer and wine in males was associated with better visual memory in cross-sectional and longitudinal settings. Using six or more servings of spirits was associated with worse visual memory in males in cross-sectional and longitudinal settings. The study suggested a lack of a linear association between drinking and visual memory in the middle-aged population.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

In general population studies, both alcohol use disorder (AUD) and heavy drinking have been found to be associated with cognitive impairment, including visual memory deficits [1,2,3,4,5]. Alcohol use disorder (AUD) is characterized by clinically significant psychosocial and behavioural problems arising from repeated and continuous use of alcohol and episodic heavy drinking is referred to as having 4 or more drinks for females and 5 or more drinks for males in a single day in the past year [6, 7]. Visual memory is the capacity to remember what has previously been seen in the form of visual images [8]. Heavy episodic drinking has been found to be associated with impaired visual memory in both male and female adolescents [9]. Light and moderate drinking, on the other hand, have not been found to be associated with cognitive impairment. On the contrary, recent studies have found that light and moderate drinking are associated with cognitive enhancement [10,11,12]. Current light (mild) drinking is referred to as having at least 12 drinks in the past year but 3 drinks or fewer per week, on average over the past year. Current moderate drinking is referred to as having more than 3 drinks but no more than 7 drinks per week for women and more than 3 drinks but no more than 14 drinks per week for men, on average over the past year. Current heavier drinking is referred to as having more than 7 drinks per week for women: more than 14 drinks per week for men, on average over the past year. The weekly safe drinking limit is 14 units/ week for male and 7units/ week for female in the U.S.A. and 14 units/ week for both male and female in the U.K. [7, 13]. Daily ethanol consumption of ≥ 40 g for male or ≥ 20 g for females has been the modified criteria for hazardous (or risky) drinking [14]. Hazardous drinking is a pattern of alcohol consumption that increases the risk of harmful consequences for the user or other [15].

Cross-sectional studies mostly suggest that moderate to heavy drinking is associated with cognitive decline [16,17,18,19] and mild to moderate drinking is either associated with cognitive enhancement [19,20,21,22,23] or is not associated with cognitive performance [24, 25]. A dose-response positive association between amount of alcohol use and cognition has been found in females [26].

Longitudinal studies among the general population mostly suggest a positive correlation between light to moderate drinking and cognitive function [2, 27,28,29,30]. Some longitudinal studies, however, suggest no association, whether positive or negative [31,32,33,34]. Mild to moderate alcohol use, compared to former alcohol use and lifelong abstinence, has been found to be associated with better cognition [29].

There are not many longitudinal studies investigating the cognitive impact of alcohol use and any change in alcohol use in middle-aged people. So, it is interesting to know whether alcohol use is good or bad for cognition in middle-aged population both in cross-sectional and in longitudinal settings. From the clinician’s point of view, it is also important to know how different patterns and types of alcohol use affect cognition. In the current study, we focused on the potential linear relationship between visual memory and alcohol consumption (total daily use of alcohol converted into grams) in both cross-sectional and longitudinal settings in the Finnish middle-aged birth cohort population. The volume of alcohol can be converted into grams of ethanol using conversion calculator [14].

We also explored the association of different patterns (frequency and amount) and types of alcohol (beer, wine, spirits) with visual memory in the same population and in the same settings.

The main aim of this study was to explore the association of alcohol use with visual memory and new learning.

The specific research aims were to study the following:

  1. 1.

    The association of daily total alcohol use, converted into gram of ethanol, at 46 years of age with visual memory at 46 years of age (Cross-sectional study).

  2. 2.

    The association of grams per day alcohol use, converted into gram of ethanol, at 31 years with visual memory at 46 years of age (Longitudinal study 1).

  3. 3.

    The association of changes in daily total alcohol use, converted into gram of ethanol, from 31 to 46 years of age with visual memory at 46 years of age (Longitudinal study 2).

  4. 4.

    The association of the frequencies and amount of the use of beer, wine, and spirits with visual memory.

Materials and methods

Study population

The study population of the present study was selected from the participants of the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1966 (NFBC1966) [35]. A total of 12,055 mothers with expected delivery dates between 1 January and 31 December 1966 from Oulu and Lapland (the two northernmost provinces of Finland) had been recruited for the study. Originally 12,231 males and females whose expected year of birth was 1966 were included in this study. Among them, 11,979 were born in 1966, 189 were born at the end of 1965 and 63 were born in early 1967. The follow-ups were conducted at 1, 14, 31, and 46 years of age [36]. Data from 5585 participants completing the 31-year (1997–1998) and 46-year (2012–2014) follow-ups and completing the Paired Associate Learning (PAL) test at 46-years follow-up were included in the present study (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart showing selection of study population

Information about alcohol consumption, background factors, and lifestyle were gathered using a postal questionnaire in 1997 when the participants were 31 years old. The survey was repeated, and the Paired Associative Learning (PAL) test was conducted, at follow-up in 2012–2014 when the participants were 46 years old.

From the original study population, 5,585 participants conducted the PAL test at the age of 46 years (Fig. 1).

The cross-sectional study exploring the association between alcohol use at the age of 46 years and cognition at the age of 46 years included 4,137 participants, after excluding those with missing information on alcohol consumption (N = 1,040) and covariates (N = 408) at the age of 46 years (Fig. 1).

Longitudinal study 1 exploring the association between alcohol use at the age of 31 years and cognition at the age of 46 years included 4,791 participants after excluding those with missing information on alcohol consumption (N = 692) and covariates (N = 102) at the age of 31 years (Fig. 1).

Longitudinal study 2 exploring the association between change in alcohol use between the ages of 31 years and 46 years and cognition at the age of 46 years included 3,934 participants after excluding those with missing information on alcohol consumption (N = 1,581) at the age 31 and 46, and covariates (N = 70) at the age of 31 years.

Alcohol drinking measures

Alcohol use was measured by questionnaire at the ages of 31 years and 46 years. The items included questions on the frequency and amount of use of different types of alcohol (Beer and other light drinks, wine, and spirits).

One drink (alcohol dose) was also explained along with the questionnaire as follow:

  • a bottle (33 cl) of medium beer or cider, or,

  • a glass (12 cl) of mild wine or a small glass (8 cl) of fortified wine, or,

  • a restaurant serving (4 cl) of spirits.

Calculation of gram per day alcohol use

Daily intake of total alcohol, as converted into grams of 100 % alcohol ethanol per day, daily intake of beer and other light drinks, in grams per day as converted into grams of 100 % ethanol, daily in-take of wine as converted into grams of 100 % ethanol, and intake of spirits, as converted into grams of 100 % ethanol in grams per day. We assumed one bottle of mild drink to contain one standard unit of alcohol (= 12 g of ethanol), wine to contain 13 % alcohol, and spirits to contain 38 % alcohol. From the questionnaire answers, daily consumption of ethanol was calculated by multiplying the typical frequency of use by quantity at both 31 years and 46 years.

Frequency of use of beer and other light drinks, wine, spirits

The frequency of alcohol use was measured on a 10-point scale (1 drinking never and 10 drinking daily). For the frequency of drinking light drinks, wine, or spirits, the cut-off was weekly drinking (drinking less frequently than once a week vs. drinking weekly or more frequently). Drinking less frequently than once a week covered drinking less than monthly and monthly. Drinking weekly or more frequently covered drinking weekly, daily or almost daily. We adopted this cut-off point from the frequency of binging question (Never, less than monthly, monthly, weekly, daily or almost daily) of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test for Consumption (AUDIT-C) questionnaire. The AUDIT-C alcohol use questionnaire has been validated in Finland in previous studies [37, 38].

Amount of use of beer and other light drinks, wine, spirits

Quantities consumed on a typical occasion were asked about separately for all three beverage types: mild drinks in the number of 0.33 l bottles; wine in the number of glasses (= 12 cl) or 0.75 l bottles; and spirits in the number of 4 cl shots or 0.5 l bottles. The mild drink quantity was measured on a 9-point scale (1 drinking none and 9 drinking 15 bottles or more), and for wines and spirits, the typical quantity was measured similarly. The cut-off point for the amount was used when drinking for light drinks was 6 bottles (under 6 bottles vs. 6 bottles or more), and for wines, one bottle (under one bottle of wine vs. a bottle of wine or more), and for spirits six drinks (under six drinks vs. six drinks or more). We adopted this cut-off point from the binging question (How often do you have six or more drinks on one occasion) of the AUDIT-C questionnaire.

Visual memory

The Paired Associative Learning (PAL) task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) was used to assess visual memory. In the PAL test random patterns are shown in six boxes on the screen. The patterns are then displayed in the middle of the screen, one at a time, and the participant must select the box in which the pattern was originally located. PAL has been found to differentiate between Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment [39]. PAL has been successfully used in different cultures and educations in both longitudinal and cross-sectional studies [40]. This study used the PAL test because it can reliably assess the type and degree of functional loss and the specificity of age-related cognitive decline [41].

In the PAL test, we assessed visual memory using the primary outcome variables, ‘total errors adjusted score’ and ‘first trial memory score’. Total Errors (Adjusted) (TEA) reflects how quickly the participant learns when they have multiple attempts at each problem, while First Trial Memory Score (FTMS) reflects how many patterns the participant correctly places on the first attempt at each problem.

Both PAL TEA and PAL FTMS were used as continuous variables.

Covariates

For our cross-sectional analyses at the age of 46, we controlled for covariates those were collected at the same visit (at the age of 46), but for our longitudinal analyses, we controlled for baseline covariates collected at the earlier visit (at the age of 31) (Fig. 1). We used educational level, marital status, diet, physical activity, smoking and cardiometabolic diseases as covariates.

Educational level is associated with cognitive functioning [42]. The question addressing the education of the NFBC1966 participants was: ‘What is your basic education?’ (Less than 9 years of basic school, 9 years of basic school, matriculation examination). In the analysis, we combined ‘less than 9 years of basic school’ and ‘9 years of basic school’ as ‘No matriculation examination’.

The question addressing the marital status of the NFBC1966 participants was: ‘What is your relationship status?’ (Married since, cohabiting since, single, legal separation or divorced since, widowed since). In the analysis, we combined ‘married’ and ‘cohabiting’ as ‘In a relationship’, and ‘single’, ‘legal separation or divorced’ and ‘widowed’ as ‘Not in a relationship’.

Diet might be associated with cognitive functioning [43,44,45]. Diet was categorised as: ‘Healthy (Consuming vegetables, roots, and salad 3 times per week or more)’ and ‘Unhealthy’ (Consuming vegetables, roots, and salad 2 times per week or less)’.

Physical activity might [46,47,48,49] or might not [50, 51] be associated with cognition. Physical activity was categorised as: ‘Active (1 h or more brisk physical activity at a time causing at least some breathlessness and sweating)’ and ‘Inactive (Less than 1 h of brisk physical activity at a time causing at least some breathlessness and sweating)’.

Smoking has been found to be associated with reduced psychomotor speed in the middle age [52]. Heavy drinking with smoking has been associated with worse cognition than only heavy drinking [53, 54]. Heavy drinking with smoking has been associated with worse cognition than only heavy drinking [55]. Smoking has also been reported to exacerbate the cognitive decline associated with cardiometabolic multimorbidity [56]. Smoking was categorised as “Non-smoking” and “Smoking”.

Cardiometabolic diseases have long been recognised to be risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia [57,58,59,60]. Cognitive decline was reported to be accelerated in older adults with co-morbid cardiometabolic diseases like diabetes, hypertension, heart disease, and stroke [56]. Data on cardiometabolic diseases were collected from questionnaires used in 31-year and 46-year follow-ups. Cardiometabolic diseases were categorised as “Not having any cardiometabolic diseases” and “Having any cardiometabolic diseases”.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using R version R 4.2.3. We evaluated the association between cognition and alcohol use using two different cognition variables: PAL FTMS and PAL TEA. The association between the PAL tests was analysed using linear regression, and a β with 95 % CI was reported. We considered < 0.01 p-value as statistically significant, as there were so many comparisons.

All continuous variables were normalised using z-scores. We assessed crude models and models adjusted with education, relationship status, diet, and physical activity. All analyses were conducted separately in males and females, as alcohol consumption differs between the two sexes [61] and performance in visual memory tests, including the PAL test, has also been reported to be different in males and females [62, 63]. Additionally, we performed multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) on the drinking data, incorporating information on drinking habits and other covariates from both visits [64]. We also conducted sensitivity analyses using the imputed drinking and co-variate values.

Attrition analysis

To evaluate the representativeness of our study population, an attrition analysis was performed while studying the associations with alcohol use in the longitudinal study 1 dataset.

Results

Background factors

About two-fifth of the participants in all three datasets were female. One-third of the males and more than half of the females had basic education above matriculation (12 years or more). About four-fifths of the participants were married or cohabitating. Half of the males and two-thirds of the females ate a healthy diet in both the cross-section at age 31 dataset and the change in alcohol use dataset. In the cross-section at age 46 dataset, three-fifths of males and four-fifths of females ate a healthy diet. One-third of males and one-fifths of females took active exercise in the age 31 and the change in alcohol use dataset. One-third of males and females took active exercise at the age of 46. Three-fourth of males and females were non-smokers in the age 31 and the change in alcohol use dataset. Three-fifth of males and females were non-smokers at the age of 46. Four-fifths of the participants did not have any cardiometabolic diseases (Table 1).

Table 1 Background factors for male and female participants in the cross-sectional, longitudinal 1, and longitudinal 2 datasets

Alcohol consumption and cognitive measures

In males, mean total alcohol consumption was 18 (SD 25) grams per day in the cross-sectional dataset and 13 (SD 18) in the longitudinal 1 dataset. In females, mean total alcohol consumption was 7 (SD 11) grams per day in the cross-sectional dataset and 5 (SD 10) in the longitudinal 1 dataset (Table 2).

Table 2 Alcohol consumption and visual memory for male and female participants in the cross-sectional, longitudinal 1, and longitudinal 2 datasets

The mean PAL TEA score was 14 in males and 12 in females in all datasets. The mean PAL FTMS score was 19 in males and 20 in females in all datasets (Table 2).

Association of grams per day total alcohol use with visual memory

No statistically significant associations were found in males or females, between gram per day total alcohol use and visual memory in the cross-sectional, longitudinal 1 or longitudinal 2 settings (Table 3).

Table 3 Association of alcohol use in grams of alcohol per day with visual memory for male and female participants in the cross-sectional, longitudinal 1, and longitudinal 2 datasets

Association of gram per day beer and other light drinks, wine, and spirits use with visual memory

No statistically significant associations were found in males or females, between gram per day beer and other light drinks, wine, and spirits use and visual memory in the cross-sectional or longitudinal 1 setting (Supplementary Tables 13).

Association of frequency of beer and other light drinks, wine, and spirit use with visual memory in the cross-sectional and longitudinal 1 datasets

The association between frequency of use of beer and other light drinks, wine, spirits and visual memory in males and females in the cross-sectional and longitudinal 1 datasets are shown in Supplementary Tables 46.

Males using beer and other light drinks and other light drinks once a week or more at 46 years of age made less errors in visual memory task at the same age (cross-sectional) compared to males using beer and other light drinks less than once a week, both before and after adjustment. They also made higher scores in PAL test before adjustment. Males using beer and other light drinks once a week or more at 31 years of age also made less errors in visual memory task and higher scores in PAL test at the age of 46 years (longitudinal-1) compared to males using beer and other light drinks less than once a week, both before and after adjustment. However, none of these findings were statistically significant (Supplementary Table 4).

Males who used to drink wine once a week or more at the age of 46 years (cross-sectional) and those who used to drink wine once a week or more at the age of 31 years (longitudinal-1) both made less errors in visual memory task and higher scores in PAL test at the age of 46 years compared to their corresponding counterparts, before adjustment, with statistically significant difference in cross-sectional setting (Supplementary Table 5).

Females who used to drink spirits once a week or more at the age of 46 years made more errors and lower scores in PAL test at the same age (46 years, cross-sectional) compared to those who did not. But the differences were not statistically significant (Supplementary Table 6).

Association of amount of beer and other light drinks, wine, and spirit use with visual memory in the cross-sectional and longitudinal 1 datasets

The association between amount of beer and other light drinks, wine, spirits and visual memory in males and females in the cross-sectional and longitudinal 1 datasets are shown in Supplementary Tables 79.

There appear to be no significant findings about the amount of use of beer and other light drinks and wine with visual memory (Supplementary Tables 7 and 8).

Males using six or more servings of spirits at 31 years made more errors before adjustment and lower scores before and after adjustment, in PAL test at 46 years (longitudinal 1) compared to males using fewer than six servings, and the differences were not statistically significant (Supplementary Table 9).

Attrition analysis

For longitudinal study-1, alcohol use data at age 31 years were available for 2,744 male and 3,369 female participants. At the age of 46 years, 25 % of males and 18.7 % of females had dropped out. The dropped-out males used significantly more alcohol and beer and other light drinks than those who participated at the age of 46 years (Supplementary Table 10).

For cross-sectional study at the age of 45 years, alcohol use data were available for 2,747 male and 3,021 female participants. About 32 % of males and 25 % of females had dropped out. The dropped-out males used more alcohol and beer and other light drinks than those who didn’t (Supplementary Table 10).

Sensitivity analyses in cross-sectional dataset

For our cross-sectional study, we have done sensitivity analyses in the cross-sectional dataset among male and female participants who were present at both visits (At the ages of 31 and 46 years). The results were like those of our original analyses (Supplementary Table 11).

Sensitivity analyses using multiple imputation

We performed multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) on the drinking data, incorporating information on different drinking patterns and covariates models included education, marital status, diet, physical activity, smoking and cardiometabolic diseases from both 31-year and 46-year follow-ups. Then we performed sensitivity analyses using the imputed drinking data and found no differences (Supplementary Table 12).

Summary of results

In summary, we did not find any association of cognitive decline with daily use of total alcohol, beer and other light drinks, wine and spirits, converted into grams of ethanol, which was not as per our expectations. Females drinking spirits once a week or more cross-sectionally, performed significantly worse on PAL TEA and made less scores on PAL FTMS compared to those using less frequently. Males using six or more servings of spirits made less first trial memory scores longitudinally compared to those using less than six. Males using beer and other light drinks and wine once a week or more made less errors and higher scores in PAL test longitudinally compared to those using less frequently.

Discussion

Summary of main findings

Most participants reported light consumption of alcohol. Because there were not many differences, our study did not report many statistically significant findings. The main finding of our birth-cohort-based longitudinal population study revealed that in mid-life when controlled for education, marital status, diet, physical activity, smoking and cardiometabolic diseases, the daily use of total alcohol, beer and other light drinks, wine and spirits, converted into grams of ethanol, was not associated with decreased visual memory in both cross-sectional and longitudinal settings. Both wine and beer and other light drinks results for men are on same direction: Those who used to drink once a week or more at 31 years made less errors and higher scores in PAL test at 46 years (longitudinal 1) compared to the those who used to drink ‘Less than once a week’. Women using spirits once a week or more at the age 46 years made more errors and lower scores in PAL test at 46 years (cross-sectional) compared to those who did not. Men using six or more servings of spirits at 31 years made less first trial memory scores compared to those using less than six servings of spirits. Almost all these results were not statistically significant.

Comparison with other studies

In general population studies, AUD has been found to be associated with visual memory deficits [1, 3]. Heavy drinking has also been found to be associated with impaired visual memory [2] and reaction time [4]. Heavy episodic drinking has also been found to be associated with poor visual memory in adolescents [9]. A positive association between moderate drinking and cognition has also been found [65]. In the present study, we found that use of six or more servings of spirits was associated with impaired visual memory in males, but that was the only finding that showed a negative association between drinking and cognition.

There are not many longitudinal studies investigating the cognitive impact of alcohol use and change in alcohol use in middle-aged people. In a study by Yan and colleagues no significant association of mild to moderate alcohol use with cognition was found in both the baseline and follow-up after 1 year [66]. A prospective cohort study of the middle-aged to elderly population with 9 years of mean follow-up revealed that mild to moderate alcohol consumption was associated with better cognitive functioning [67]. In another longitudinal study, an increase in alcohol use from the baseline to follow-up after 11 years was found to be associated with better cognition in females [30]. A fourth study by Arntzen et al. reported that light to moderate wine use at the baseline was associated with better cognition at follow-up after 7 years [68]. Our current study did not find any association between the baseline use of alcohol and cognition at the follow-up provided the fact that we did not measure cognition at all at the baseline.

As in our study, some other longitudinal studies, however, suggest no association, whether positive or negative [31,32,33,34]. One recent study also did not find any significant association of mild to moderate alcohol use with cognition compared to nonalcohol users both at the baseline and at follow-up after 1 year where age, education, smoking, tea drinking, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, diabetes and traumatic brain injury were used as covariates [66].

Earlier cross-sectional studies suggested that mild to moderate drinking was either associated with cognitive enhancement [19,20,21,22,23] or was not associated with cognition at all [24, 25]. The study findings in our cross-sectional dataset at age 46 were like those studies.

Hop-Derived Iso-α-Acid, a chemical component found in dark beer and other light drinks, has been associated with better visual memory in animal model studies [69]. Our finding on beer and other light drinks use did not comply with this finding, as we found a negative association between beer and other light drinks use frequency and visual memory. Resveratrol, a polyphenol compound found in red wine, has been associated with better cognitive performance in a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study in humans [70]. This might explain our findings suggesting an association of frequency of wine consumption with better visual memory in males. However, white wine has little of this component, and there is no record of whether the wine consumed was, red, rose or white. The association between increased amount of spirits and impaired visual memory in males in our study can be explained by the activation of pro-inflammatory cytokines, free-radical damage, and thiamine deficiency caused by ethanol-induced neurotoxicity [71].

The number of drinks per week has been found to be associated with cognitive impairment [72]. However, one cross-sectional cohort study revealed that moderate, regular drinking was associated with better cognition than non-drinking and less-frequent drinking [20]. Another cohort study revealed that irregular drinking and abstinence were associated with cognitive decline compared to regular drinking [73]. In the cross-sectional part of our cohort study, more frequent use of wine was found to be associated with better cognition in males.

A dose-response positive association between alcohol use and cognition has been found among female current drinkers compared to former heavy drinkers and abstainers [26]. An increase in alcohol use from the baseline to follow-up after 11 years has been found to be associated with better cognition in females [30]. In our study, changes in the amount of alcohol use from baseline to follow up were not associated with cognitive function at follow up. We, however, did not measure cognition at the baseline. Light to moderate wine use at the baseline has been found to be associated with better cognition at follow-up after 7 years [68]. In our study, more frequent use of wine in males were associated with better cognition both cross-sectionally and longitudinally.

Strengths

Different patterns of alcohol use in both cross-sectional and longitudinal settings of a large cohort population are the novel aspects of the current study. The age group of the participants was middle aged, which has not been studied thoroughly previously. We were able to use multiple covariates like education, marital status, diet, physical activity, smoking and cardiometabolic diseases in this study. Gram per day use of total alcohol, beers, wine, and spirits and frequencies and amounts of beer and other light drinks, wine and spirits were used. We were able to study the association between visual memory and reported alcohol use in both cross-sectional and longitudinal settings. The longitudinal setting included using both alcohol use at age 31 years and change in alcohol use between ages 31 and 46 years as exposures.

Limitations

We did not categorise alcohol use into mild, moderate, and heavy; neither did we exclude abstinence. Most of the alcohol use measures were self-reported which might introduce biases. It is also important to note that our results did not represent cognition in general, but only one very limited aspect (visual memory) of this wide phenomenon. As only one cognitive measure was applied, we could not draw conclusions on general cognitive performance. No measure of cognition or visual memory was assessed at the baseline. Without such a baseline control, it is difficult to interpret the findings and the direction of the effect. Only educational attainment was used to assess socioeconomic status (SES), not household income although educational attainment largely correlates with earning [74, 75]. Higher SES participants are likely to have both better visual memory and better incomes to purchase wine. Education level was assessed only via a binary variable (no matriculation exam vs. matriculation exam). Depression is associated with impairment of memory [76] and executive functioning [77]. In the current study, however, we did not use depressive symptoms as a possible confounder because a recent study found no association between depressive symptoms and visual memory in the same population [78]. Possible changes in marital status, diet, and exercise over time might also make it difficult to predict the true effects of covariants in this longitudinal study. We did not present interaction analyses by sex to show whether any findings were indeed different for men and women, or whether the differences in the observed sex-specific associations between alcohol use and cognition stemmed from differences in statistical power. The high attrition level was another limitation of our study.

Clinical and public health implications

It is important to study cognition and its relationship with alcohol, if alcohol use affects cognition and if cognition affects the use of alcohol. Our results suggest that light to moderate alcohol use might not be harmful for cognition in the general population. This might have an impact on public health, and the statutory bodies might need to reform their existing alcohol policies. However, the findings of this study should be interpreted cautiously, and no clinical conclusions can be drawn until the results of this study are replicated in other studies.

Conclusions

It is important to study cognition and its relationship with alcohol, if alcohol use affects cognition and if cognition affects the use of alcohol. Our study suggests a lack of a linear association between reported alcohol use and visual memory. The findings of the current study concerning the association of reported alcohol use with cognitive enhancement in male middle-aged birth-cohort population both cross-sectionally and longitudinally are novel. Based on our study, recommendations on the safety limits of alcohol use in adults might need to be reconsidered. The observed gender-specific findings in our study are interesting and require further investigation. These might encourage future researchers to explore more in this field.

Availability of data and materials

NFBC data is available from the University of Oulu, Infrastructure for Population Studies. Permission to use the data can be applied for research purposes via an electronic material request portal. In the use of data, we follow the EU general data protection regulation (679/2016) and Finnish Data Protection Act. The use of personal data is based on cohort participant’s written informed consent at his/her latest follow-up study, which may cause limitations to its use. Please, contact NFBC project center (NFBCprojectcenter@oulu.fi) and visit the cohort website for more information.

Change history

  • 21 March 2024

    The article has been updated to correct a typo in the abstract.

References

  1. Ros-Cucurull E, et al. Alcohol use disorder and cognitive impairment in old age patients: a 6 months follow-up study in an outpatient unit in Barcelona. Psychiatry Res. 2018;261:361–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.069.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Woods AJ, et al. Current heavy alcohol consumption is associated with greater cognitive impairment in older adults. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2016;40(11):2435–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13211.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Bernardin F, Maheut-Bosser A, Paille F. Cognitive impairments in alcohol-dependent subjects. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:78. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00078.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Piumatti G, Moore S, Berridge D, Sarkar C, Gallacher J. The relationship between alcohol use and long-term cognitive decline in middle and late life: a longitudinal analysis using UK Biobank. J Public Health (Oxf). 2018;40(2):313–4. https://doi.org/10.1093/pubmed/fdy032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Sabia S, et al. High alcohol consumption in middle-aged adults is associated with poorer cognitive performance only in the low socio-economic group. Results from the GAZEL cohort study. Addiction (Abingdon, England). 2011;106(1):93–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03106.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. World Health Organization. International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems. 11th ed. 2020. https://icd.who.int/.

  7. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. National health interview survey. Glossary- alcohol 2018. 2018. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis/alcohol/alcohol_glossary.htm#print. Accessed 24 Oct 2023.

  8. APA dictionary definition of visual-memory. 2022. https://dictionary.apa.org/visual-memory.

  9. Vinader-Caerols C, Monleón S. Binge drinking, alone or with cannabis, during adolescence triggers different effects on immediate visual memory in men and women. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:797221. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.797221.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Brennan SE, et al. Long-term effects of alcohol consumption on cognitive function: a systematic review and dose-response analysis of evidence published between 2007 and 2018. Syst Rev. 2020;9(1):33. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-019-1220-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Rehm J, Hasan O, Black SE, Shield KD, Schwarzinger M. Alcohol use and dementia: a systematic scoping review. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-018-0453-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Neafsey EJ, Collins MA. Moderate alcohol consumption and cognitive risk. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2011;7:465–84. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S23159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Public Health England. Alcohol use screening tests 2017. Available at: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/alcohol-use-screening-tests. Accessed 24 Oct 2023.

  14. Bohn MJ, Babor TF, Kranzler HR. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): validation of a screening instrument for use in medical settings. J Stud Alcohol. 1995;56(4):423–32. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1995.56.423.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fujii H, Nishimoto N, Yamaguchi S, Kurai O, Miyano M, Ueda W, Oba H, Aoki T, Kawada N, Okawa K. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test for Consumption (AUDIT-C) is more useful than pre-existing laboratory tests for predicting hazardous drinking: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:379. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3053-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Bartholow BD, et al. Alcohol effects on response inhibition: variability across tasks and individuals. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;26(3):251–67. https://doi.org/10.1037/pha0000190.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Mayhugh RE, et al. Moderate-heavy alcohol consumption lifestyle in older adults is associated with altered central executive network community structure during cognitive task. PLoS One. 2016;11(8):e0160214. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160214.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Carrilho PEM, Santos MBMD, Piasecki L, Jorge AC. Doença de Marchiafava-Bignami: uma rara entidade com prognóstico sombrio. Rev Bras Ter Intensiva. 2013;25:68–72. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0103-507X2013000100013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Kim JW, et al. Alcohol and cognition in the elderly: a review. Psychiatry Investig. 2012;9(1):8–16. https://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2012.9.1.8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Reas ET, Laughlin GA, Kritz-Silverstein D, Barrett-Connor E, McEvoy LK. Moderate, regular alcohol consumption is associated with higher cognitive function in older community-dwelling adults. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2016;3(2):105–13. https://doi.org/10.14283/jpad.2016.89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Panza F, et al. Alcohol consumption in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: harmful or neuroprotective? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;27(12):1218–38. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.3772.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Bond GE, Burr R, McCurry SM, Graves AB, Larson EB. Alcohol, aging, and cognitive performance in a cohort of Japanese Americans aged 65 and older: the Kame project. Int Psychogeriatr. 2001;13(2):207–23. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610201007591.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Spencer RL, Hutchison KE. Alcohol, aging, and the stress response. Alcohol Res Health. 1999;23(4):272–83.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Moussa MN, et al. Long-term moderate alcohol consumption does not exacerbate age-related cognitive decline in healthy, community-dwelling older adults. Front Aging. 2015;6:341. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00341neuroscience.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Sabia S, et al. Alcohol consumption and cognitive decline in early old age. Neurology. 2014;82(4):332–9. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000000063.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Davis BJ, et al. The alcohol paradox: light-to-moderate alcohol consumption, cognitive function, and brain volume. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(12):1528–35. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glu092.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Zhang Y, Li Y, Zheng X, Zhu L, Xu B. Association between alcohol consumption in midlife and cognitive function in old age: findings from the China health and Nutrition Survey. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis. 2021;31(11):3044–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.numecd.2021.07.023.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Koch M, et al. Alcohol consumption and risk of dementia and cognitive decline among older adults with or without mild cognitive impairment. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(9):e1910319. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.10319.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Ganguli M, Vander Bilt J, Saxton JA, Shen C, Dodge HH. Alcohol consumption and cognitive function in late life: a longitudinal community study. Neurology. 2005;65(8):1210–7. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000180520.35181.24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Britton A, Singh-Manoux A, Marmot M. Alcohol consumption and cognitive function in the Whitehall II Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;160(3):240–7. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwh206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Chosy EJ, Edland S, Launer L, White LR. Midlife alcohol consumption and later life cognitive impairment: light drinking is not protective and APOE genotype does not change this relationship. PLoS One. 2022;17(3):e0264575. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264575.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Sun L, et al. Alcohol consumption and subclinical findings on cognitive function, biochemical indexes, and cortical anatomy in cognitively normal aging Han Chinese population. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:182. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00182.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Topiwala A, et al. Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and cognitive decline: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ. 2017;357. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j2353.

  34. Lobo E, et al. Is there an association between low-to-moderate alcohol consumption and risk of cognitive decline? Am J Epidemiol. 2010;172(6):708–16. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwq187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Rantakallio P. The longitudinal study of the northern Finland birth cohort of 1966. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 1988;2(1):59–88. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-3016.1988.tb00180.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Nordström T, et al. Cohort profile: 46 years of follow-up of the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1966 (NFBC1966). Int J Epidemiol. 2021. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyab109.

  37. Liskola J, Haravuori H, Lindberg N, Kiviruusu O, Niemelä S, Karlsson L, Marttunen M. The predictive capacity of AUDIT and AUDIT-C among adolescents in a one-year follow-up study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2021;218:108424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Kaarne T, Aalto M, Kuokkanen M, Seppä K. AUDIT-C, AUDIT-3 and AUDIT-QF in screening risky drinking among Finnish occupational health-care patients. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2010;29(5):563–7. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2010.00172.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Swainson R, et al. Early detection and differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease and depression with neuropsychological tasks. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2001;12(4):265–80. https://doi.org/10.1159/000051269.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Barnett JH, Blackwell AD, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. The Paired Associates Learning (PAL) test: 30 years of CANTAB translational neuroscience from laboratory to bedside in dementia research. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2016;28:449–74. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2015_5001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. de Rover M, et al. Hippocampal dysfunction in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a functional neuroimaging study of a visuospatial paired associates learning task. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(7):2060–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Gross AL, et al. Effects of education and race on cognitive decline: an integrative study of generalizability versus study-specific results. Psychol Aging. 2015;30(4):863–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000032.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Więckowska-Gacek A, Mietelska-Porowska A, Wydrych M, Wojda U. Western diet as a trigger of Alzheimer’s disease: from metabolic syndrome and systemic inflammation to neuroinflammation and neurodegeneration. Ageing Res Rev. 2021;70:101397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2021.101397.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Davidson TL, Sample CH, Kanoski SE. Western diet and cognitive impairment. In: Martin CR, Preedy VR, editors. Diet and nutrition in dementia and cognitive decline. Academic; 2015. p. 295–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407824-6.00027-6.

  45. Kanoski SE, Davidson TL. Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol Behav. 2011;103(1):59–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Luo L, et al. Sex-specific longitudinal association between baseline physical activity level and cognitive decline in Chinese over 45 years old: evidence from the China health and retirement longitudinal study. Aging Ment Health. 2021:1–9. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2021.1935456.

  47. Krakovska O, et al. Healthy memory aging – the benefits of regular daily activities increase with age. Aging. 2021;13(24):25643–52. https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.203753.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Gaertner B, et al. Physical exercise and cognitive function across the life span: results of a nationwide population-based study. J Sci Med Sport. 2018;21(5):489–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2017.08.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Lo AH, Woodman RJ, Pachana NA, Byrne GJ, Sachdev PS. Associations between lifestyle and cognitive function over time in women aged 40–79 years. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;39(2):371–83. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-130971.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Wu Z, et al. High-intensity physical activity is not associated with better cognition in the elder: evidence from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13(1):182. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-021-00923-3.

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Elovainio M, et al. Physical and cognitive function in midlife: reciprocal effects? a 5-year follow-up of the Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009;63(6):468–73. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2008.081505.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kalmijn S, van Boxtel MP, Verschuren MW, Jolles J, Launer LJ. Cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance in middle age. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156(10):936–44. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwf135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Hagger-Johnson G, Sabia S, Brunner E, Shipley M, Bobak M, Marmot M, et al. Combined impact of smoking and heavy alcohol use on cognitive decline in early old age: Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(2):120–5. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.122960.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Sabia S, et al. Impact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: the Whitehall II cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(6):627–35. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2016.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Wu J, Dong W, Pan XF, Feng L, Yuan JM, Pan A, Koh WP. Relation of cigarette smoking and alcohol drinking in midlife with risk of cognitive impairment in late life: the Singapore Chinese Health Study. Age Ageing. 2019;48(1):101–7. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afy166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Jin Y, Liang J, Hong C, Liang R, Luo Y. Cardiometabolic multimorbidity, lifestyle behaviours, and cognitive function: a multicohort study. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2023;4(6):e265–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2666-7568(23)00054-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Brayne C, Burns A, Cohen-Mansfield J, Cooper C, Costafreda SG, Dias A, Fox N, Gitlin LN, Howard R, Kales HC, Kivimäki M, Larson EB, Ogunniyi A, Orgeta V, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet (London, England). 2020;396(10248):413–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Wang Z, Marseglia A, Shang Y, Dintica C, Patrone C, Xu W. Leisure activity and social integration mitigate the risk of dementia related to cardiometabolic diseases: a population-based longitudinal study. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(2):316–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2019.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Dove A, Guo J, Marseglia A, Fastbom J, Vetrano DL, Fratiglioni L, Pedersen NL, Xu W. Cardiometabolic multimorbidity and incident dementia: the Swedish twin registry. Eur Heart J. 2023;44(7):573–82. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurheartj/ehac744.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Tai XY, Veldsman M, Lyall DM, Littlejohns TJ, Langa KM, Husain M, Ranson J, Llewellyn DJ. Cardiometabolic multimorbidity, genetic risk, and dementia: a prospective cohort study. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2022;3(6):e428–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2666-7568(22)00117-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Slade T, et al. Birth cohort trends in the global epidemiology of alcohol use and alcohol-related harms in men and women: systematic review and metaregression. BMJ Open. 2016;6(10):e011827. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011827.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Asperholm M, Högman N, Rafi J, Herlitz A. What did you do yesterday? a meta-analysis of sex differences in episodic memory. Psychol Bull. 2019;145(8):785–821. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Trahan DE, Quintana JW. Analysis of gender effects upon verbal and visual memory performance in adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1990;5(4):325–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Zhang Z. Multiple imputation with multivariate imputation by chained equation (MICE) package. Ann Transl Med. 2016;4(2):30. https://doi.org/10.3978/j.issn.2305-5839.2015.12.63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Richard EL, et al. Alcohol intake and cognitively healthy longevity in community-dwelling adults: the Rancho Bernardo Study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59(3):803–14. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-161153.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Yan Z, et al. The effects of light-to-moderate alcohol consumption on the cognitive function of community nondemented male elderly: a cohort Study. Behav Neurol. 2021;2021:5681913. https://doi.org/10.1155/2021/5681913.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Zhang R, et al. Association of low to moderate alcohol drinking with cognitive functions from middle to older age among US adults. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(6):e207922. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.7922.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Arntzen KA, Schirmer H, Wilsgaard T, Mathiesen EB. Moderate wine consumption is associated with better cognitive test results: a 7 year follow up of 5033 subjects in the Tromsø Study. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl. 2010;190:23–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.2010.01371.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Ayabe T, Ohya R, Ano Y. Hop-derived Iso-α-acids in beer improve visual discrimination and reversal learning in mice as assessed by a touch panel operant system. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;13:67. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00067.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Kennedy DO, et al. Effects of resveratrol on cerebral blood flow variables and cognitive performance in humans: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91(6):1590–7. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2009.28641.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Chopra K, Tiwari V. Alcoholic neuropathy: possible mechanisms and future treatment possibilities. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2012;73(3):348–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2125.2011.04111.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Ge S, et al. Alcohol use and cognitive functioning among middle-aged and older adults in China: findings of the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study Baseline Survey. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2018;42(10):2054–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Almeida OP, Hankey GJ, Yeap BB, Golledge J, Flicker L. Alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment in older men: a mendelian randomization study. Neurology. 2014;82(12):1038–44. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000000255.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. World Bank Blog. Strong link between education and earnings 2018. Available at: https://blogs.worldbank.org/education/strong-link-between-education-and-earnings#:~:text=Typically%2C%20returns%20to%20education%20are%20estimated%20using%20the,level%20of%20education%20and%20amount%20of%20work%20experience. Accessed 24 Oct 2023.

  75. Blaug M. The correlation between education and earnings: what does it signify? High Educ. 1972;1:53–76. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01956881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Burt DB, Zembar MJ, Niederehe G. Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol Bull. 1995;117(2):285. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.285.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell A. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44(10):2029–40. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002535.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Taivalantti M, et al. Depressive symptoms as predictors of visual memory deficits in middle-age. J Affect Disord. 2020;264:29–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Staff and participants of the NFBC1966 study.

Funding

Open Access funding provided by University of Oulu (including Oulu University Hospital). NFBC 1966 received financial support from University of Oulu Grant no. 24000692, Oulu University Hospital Grant no. 24301140, ERDF European Regional Development Fund Grant no. 539/2010 A31592. The principal author was supported by the H2020 Marie Skłodowska-Curie Actions co-funding of regional, national, and international programmes (COFUND) (grant agreement no. 713606) for his doctoral programme (MSC-COFUND, I4FUTURE). The principal author was also supported by the Iso-Mällinen Foundation (Iso-Mällisen Säätiö) through a medical grant of EUR 4000 for the year 2019 (grant no. 0400 584622).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Atiqul Haq Mazumder prepared the main manuscript. Jennifer H Barnett has given valuable feedback in different phase of manuscript writing. Anu-Helmi Halt has substantially contributed at different phases of manuscript writing. Marjo Taivalantti has substantially contributed at different phases of manuscript writing. Martta Kerkelä has substantially contributed at different phases of manuscript writing. She has additionally helped in data analysis. Marjo-Riitta Järvelin has substantially contributed at different phases of manuscript writing. Juha Veijola has substantially contributed at different phases of manuscript writing. He also supervised the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Atiqul Haq Mazumder.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval: The study protocol of the NFBC 1966 was approved by The Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Oulu. Data protection of the NFBC 1966 was approved by the Finnish Privacy Protection Agency and the Ministry of Social Affairs and Health. Each follow-up study of the NFBC1966 has been evaluated by the regional ethical committee of the Northern Ostrobothnia Hospital District (EETTMK 94/11, 17.09.2012) [31]. All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent to participate: In the NFBC1966 written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study subjects were also informed that they could withdraw their consent at any time, at which point any samples or data stored from them would be destroyed.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mazumder, A.H., Barnett, J.H., Halt, AH. et al. Visual memory and alcohol use in a middle-aged birth cohort. BMC Public Health 24, 788 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18153-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18153-1

Keywords