Skip to main content

PTSD, FOMO and fake news beliefs: a cross-sectional study of Wenchuan earthquake survivors

Abstract

Background

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) sufferers show problematic patterns of Internet use such as fear of missing out (FOMO) and sharing misinformation and fake news. This study aimed to investigate these associations in survivors of the 2008 earthquake in Wenchuan, China.

Methods

A self-reported survey was completed by 356 survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake. A mediated structural equation model was constructed to test a proposed pattern of associations with FOMO as a mediator of the relationship between PTSD symptoms and belief in fake news, as well as moderators of this pathway.

Results

PTSD was directly associated with believing fake news (β = 0.444, p < .001) and with FOMO (β = 0.347, p < .001). FOMO mediated the association between PTSD and fake news belief (β = 0.373, p < .001). Age moderated the direct (β = 0.148, t = 3.097, p = .002) and indirect (β = 0.145, t = 3.122, p = .002) pathways, with effects more pronounced with increasing age. Gender was also a moderator, with the indirect effect present in females but not in males (β = 0.281, t = 6.737, p < .001).

Conclusion

Those with higher PTSD symptoms are more likely to believe fake news and this is partly explained by FOMO. This effect is present in females and not males and is stronger in older people. Findings extend knowledge of the role of psychological variables in problematic Internet use among those with PTSD.

Peer Review reports

On May 12, 2008, a magnitude 8.0 earthquake struck Wenchuan County, Sichuan Province, China [1], causing hundreds of thousands of fatalities and $150 billion in damages [1, 2]. The Wenchuan earthquake was a catastrophic event that prompted a great deal of research. According to Fan et al. [2], approximately 7,000 peer-reviewed articles about the earthquake had been published by the end of 2018. Notably, however, few of these articles documented the PTSD experienced by earthquake survivors.

PTSD is proved that it could significantly contribute to online information engagement, which indicates the increased exposure to Internet disinformation, as investigated the relationship between PTSD and disinformation engagement among adolescents who have experienced a major earthquake [3, 4]. The effects of PTSD are pervasive and far-reaching. PTSD is linked to problematic smart device use, which some academics have termed ‘iDisorder’ [3]. A recent study of adolescent earthquake survivors found that PTSD may also contribute to online disinformation exposure and engagement [4]. Several studies have been conducted to explore the reasons for these links, most with a focus on younger PTSD sufferers [4,5,6].

Researchers have explored relationships between PTSD and other aspects of maladaptive technology use [5]. One such aspect is the fear of missing out, or FOMO, which is characterized by a persistent need to stay connected to the experiences of others, typically through using social media [7,8,9]. FOMO is a widespread phenomenon that can be detrimental to physical and mental health [10]. It is not necessarily bad, as it can enhance the strength of social media connections and improve happiness in some [11]. In most people, however, it seems to be associated with a maladaptive ruminative style of thinking that can cause negative outcomes such as increased anxiety. Rumination refers to the vicious cycle that occurs when individuals repeatedly recall their feelings after a stressful life event. Studies have found that women and single people show higher levels of FOMO and ruminative thinking than men and married people [12, 13].

FOMO is also linked with the intensity of problematic smartphone use to assess their mediating role in PTSD [3, 14,15,16], and the results indicated that problematic smartphone use was most strongly associated with negative emotions and arousal in individuals with PTSD [14]. FOMO is a mental health-related construct that may drive some negative emotion, but PTSD remains the dominant concept because it can drive both excessive Internet usage and misinformation.

And with social media tools providing an ever-increasing amount of social information, there is a close proximity to real-time media activity on various social networks. This digital activity-driven flood of updates has given rise to a relatively new phenomenon known as FOMO [9, 11, 17]. Excessive social media use has been shown to cause significant fatigue, which is followed by high anxiety and depression, and FOMO plays an indirect role to predict this [18]. Przybylski attributes this phenomenon to the fact that social platforms offer benefits to the general public, the internet is likely a particularly effective tool for combating FOMO. In fact, media news provide a ‘efficient and low friction path’ [9], particularly for those who follow the news constantly. FOMO has also been studied in relation to news information, business information engagement and fake news [12]. The spread of nonexistent fake news on social media poses a serious threat to the vast majority of social platforms. In 2016, for instance, the sports brand New Balance faced widespread backlash from anti-Trump groups after rumors circulated that its products had been misbranded as being associated with the then-Trump presidential campaign. This resulted in a decline in the company’s stock price. In the same year, there was a massive Pepsi boycott after senior Pepsi employees were arrested for showing government hostility. Companies, governments, and even individuals can generate and disseminate information (or news) in order to quickly deliver their own agendas to large audiences via social media, making the threat of fake news imminent [17]. Few studies, however, have examined the relationship between fake news and FOMO, and fake news videos have begun to emerge, which, due to their novelty and potent visual impact, increase the risk of anxiety, and FOMO was found to be a direct contributor to this situation [19]. In contrast, based on responses from China and the United States, Jo et al. [20] investigated the mediating relationship between FOMO and misinformation trust across multiple age and cultural contexts. The findings show that older individuals are especially susceptible to misinformation because they are less likely to check suspicious content and have less motivation to share information online, so the age is also an important moderator variable in the study of the relationship between FOMO and misinformation. However, FOMO is not always a bad thing, as it has a positive effect on happiness and indirectly affects the strength of social media connections [11], despite the fact that other studies of FOMO’s relationship to happiness have primarily negative attitudes. For instance, Hattingh et al. [21] examines the association between FOMO and misinformation on social media, which is mediated by information and communication overload, online subjective well-being, whereas Leung et al. [22] examined the mechanisms of disinformation spread and depressive symptoms through three studies with the aim of highlighting the mediating role of FOMO.

Therefore, it may be essential to comprehend how FOMO and negative emotions (such as depression, anxiety, stress disorders, etc.) contribute to the varying levels of misinformation belief among the general public. By building up a theoretical framework for PTSD, FOMO, and fake news beliefs, this study will fill a gap in the literature by examining Wenchuan earthquake survivors to determine if their PTSD symptoms may contribute to their belief in fake news via the mediating effect of FOMO.

Review and hypothesis

The association between PTSD and fake news belief

Though research into fake news has been conducted in many academic fields, we focus here on belief in fake news and the psychological variables associated with such belief, such as negative emotions, anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms. As yet, few studies have investigated the link between negative emotions and engagement with so-called ‘dark phenomena’ on social media [21]. Delmastro and Paciello [23] suppose that the relationship between PTSD and trust in disinformation is a complex one, while Yang et al. [4] found a significant link between PTSD and exposure to Internet misinformation. The authors also highlight the role of news recommendation algorithms in social platforms, as algorithms can lead to echo chambers and confirmation bias for users, which is a major issue in believing misinformation and fake news [23]. They also discovered that people living alone were significantly more likely to believe and retweet fake news, and physical isolation in the real world (such as natural disasters, epidemics, etc.) may contribute to the belief in fake news.

Studies have revealed that psychological problems linked to infectious disease outbreaks may increase trust and retweeting of fake news on social platforms and that PTSD may be linked to dependence on social media platforms [4]. Other individual factors linked to trust in fake news include level of education, with better-educated individuals more able to discern the accuracy of information, and physical isolation, with individuals who live alone being more likely to believe and retweet fake news [24].

The mediating effect of FOMO

Numerous scholars have examined cognitive-psychological factors including FOMO as correlates of belief in fake news [11, 17,18,19, 22, 25, 26]. Talwar et al. [25], for example, found that FOMO, along with online self-disclosure and social media fatigue, were positively associated with sharing fake news. FOMO is skewed towards negative outcomes, which also explains depressive symptoms and PTSD respectively [17]. Another study [11] explains that FOMO is positively related to the intensity of online disinformation exposure and negatively related to social connections.

In some cases of social isolation, people may experience frequent FOMO when they are away from their social environment because they are not seeing their friends, partners, or family members [13]. It is reasonable to assume that the destruction of social networks caused by the earthquake is also a cause of FOMO. Other studies on the relationship between PTSD and FOMO typically include mediator variables, such as problematic mobile phone use [3, 8, 16, 27], anxiety or depression [5, 6, 10, 15, 16, 28], compensatory internet use [15], etc., and also consider the role of certain moderator variables, such as family environment [6], adulthood or not [4, 6]. Compensatory Internet use theory posits that FOMO and sharing false information may be markers of problematic Internet use that is engaged in to escape from real-world problems or reduce dysphoric moods [12].

In addition, FOMO usually plays as a mediator variable to expand PTSD and the domain of psychopathology-related constructs. Elhai et al. [7] was one of the first to examine the relationship between FOMO and PTSD severity in Asian subjects, and they discovered that FOMO may be an important variable in explaining certain types of psychopathology. In another study by Elhai et al. [8], he expressed concern that few studies had examined mental health variables associated with smartphone use and misinformation sharing.

Through a literature review, Tandon et al. [17] identified the limitation of all FOMO articles, namely that previous studies have focused on settings where the FOMO variable is associated with social media related concepts. The authors suggest continuing to validate the relationship between FOMO and fake news beliefs in settings that are disconnected from real-world scenarios. Social media news use and FOMO were positively correlated with intentional deepfake sharing, suggesting that individuals with lower cognitive abilities presented higher levels of FOMO and more sharing behaviour [19], which demonstrated that the indirect effect of social media news sharing on the progression of FOMO is greater for low cognitive individuals, and reveals for the first time that even those with a higher analytical mindset are susceptible to fake news, particularly deepfakes. In accordance with the above literature review, we therefore propose the following research hypothesis:

The moderating effect of age and gender

There is some evidence that any relationship that exists between FOMO and susceptibility to misinformation is dependent on demographic and cultural variables including gender [5, 29], family environment [6], and age [4, 6]. In a study of participants from China and the United States, Jo et al. [20] found older individuals to be especially vulnerable to misinformation because they are less likely to fact-check suspicious content. Elhai et al. [7] demonstrated associations between FOMO and demographic characteristics including age, gender, ethnicity, and relationship status. In another study, Elhai [29] and colleagues studied the relationship between FOMO and sharing fake news sharing on social platforms, finding that FOMO increased sharing of fake news and that age moderated this relationship, with the link stronger in older respondents than younger ones. Alt [26] examined FOMO and motivation for sharing fake news in students, finding that millennials of ethnic minorities were subject to pressure from their ethnic group which increased the likelihood of them spreading fake news. In another study, cognitive ability moderated a correlation between FOMO and intentional deepfake sharing, suggesting that individuals with lower cognitive ability had higher FOMO and were more likely to share deepfakes [19].

Some studies have confirmed gender differences in the prevalence of FOMO, such that it is higher among females [30]. In one study [5], the authors interpreted this by linking females’ higher incidence of depression with use of social networking sites, while in another, FOMO was linked to depression in females [16]. Another consideration is that generally more women than men participate in misinformation studies [4, 24], meaning that moderation effects in these studies could be an artefact of unbalanced gender distribution.

Aims and hypotheses

The main purpose of this study is to establish the structure of associations between PTSD, FOMO, and misinformation belief in survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake. We are also interested in exploring moderators of these associations. Based on the above review of relevant literature, we propose the following seven hypotheses for the current study:

H1

PTSD caused by the Wenchuan earthquake is positively associated with FOMO.

H2

FOMO is positively associated with believing fake news.

H3

PTSD is positively associated with believing fake news.

H4

FOMO mediates the association between PTSD and believing fake news.

H5

Age moderates the PTSD–fake news pathway.

H6

Age moderates the FOMO–fake news pathway.

H7

Gender moderates the FOMO–fake news pathway.

Figure 1 presents a model visualising these hypothesized relationships.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Research model

Method

Participants and procedure

From January to March 2023, we mailed and visited earthquake survivors in Beichuan County, a particularly hard-hit area of the Wenchuan earthquake. Inclusion criteria were that participants had normal hearing and vision, no concussion in the week before the test, and did not use psychostimulants or other substances that could affect the central nervous system. We distributed 800 copies of our survey. Once returned, we eliminated those with missing data and irregular responses, leaving a total of 356 valid copies and an effective response rate of 44.5%. Our final sample included 180 men and 176 women aged between 20 and 47 (30.42 ± 5.41).

Ethics

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Mianyang College of Administration. All participants gave written informed consent and were compensated for their time.

Measures

PTSD Scale

The majority of published research on PTSD has been conducted in a Western context. As such, there is relatively limited knowledge of PTSD in the Chinese population. As Leung et al. (2021) note, however, cultural context is an important factor to consider. We therefore referred to previous peer-reviewed studies that have used relevant measures translated for use with Chinese participants [20, 31]. One commonly used measure is the adult version of the PTSD Checklist - Civilian Version [32], and its subsequent modified versions [31], which show good validity and reliability in Chinese samples. The PTSD scale used here consists of 17 items, each with a five-point Likert response scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. An example item is “Even if nothing reminds you of it, you remember or have mental images of the traumatic event”. Higher total scores indicate more severe PTSD. Reliability analysis showed a Cronbach’s α of 0.952. Specific Cronbach’s α values of each PTSD scale item and other indicators are available at https://osf.io/3v9t8/.

FOMO Scale

Based on previous studies with Chinese participants [7, 20, 22, 31], we used Przybylski’s [9] scale to form a Chinese FOMO scale. Responses were given on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = not at all true of me to 5 = extremely true of me. An example item is “I fear others have more rewarding experiences than me”. Higher total scores indicate more severe experience of FOMO. Cronbach’s α was 0.943 in this sample.

Fake news belief Scale

We developed a five-item scale based on previous studies of social media use [4, 19, 33, 34] Responses were given on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = not at all true of me to 5 = extremely true of me. An example item is “Even if the news is not published by official media, you focus more on the news’s content than on its publisher”. Higher total scores indicate stronger belief of fake news. Cronbach’s α was 0.864.

Results

Factors analysis

Reliability analysis revealed that Cronbach’s α coefficients ranged from 0.864 to 0.952. As values greater than 0.7 are considered to be good [35], the scales used here had excellent reliability.

To account for possible common method bias from using only self-reported questionnaires [36], we conducted a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) using AMOS 24 software. In CFA, factor loadings of individual items must be above 0.6 and model fit must meet appropriate criteria. Structural equation model fit metrics are used for cardinality, degrees of freedom, cardinality/degrees of freedom, RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation), GFI (Goodness of Fit Index), AGFI (Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index), and RFI (Relative Fit Index), NFI (Normed Fit Index), IFI (Incremental Fit Index), TLI (Tucker-Lewis index) and CFI (Comparative Fit Index). We refer to the format of the Table 2 result reported by Li et al. [37] and form the analysis in Table 1. As shown in Table 1, all model fit indicators fell within a reasonable range, and the model fit the data well indicating our data are suitable for further analysis.

We also followed recommendations from Hair [38] regarding standardized factor loadings, which state that convergent validity can be assumed if composite reliability (CR) is over 0.7 and average variance extracted (AVE) is over 0.5. Analysis showed standardised factor loadings for each item ranged from 0.699 to 0.804, with all exceeding 0.6 and the significance threshold. CR values ranged from 0.864 to 0.952 and AVE values ranged from 0.540 to 0.563. Therefore, the data showed good convergent validity and passed the validation factor analysis.

Table 1 Confirmatory factor analysis

Descriptive statistics and correlations

Descriptive statistics for the principal scales and Bivariate Pearson correlations between the scale are shown in Table 2. PTSD prevalence was 38.1%, which is consistent with Wang et al. [39] and Ying et al. [40], with 28.4% and 42.5% respectively. Given the fact that this study is not a PTSD population survey, and the amount of people surveyed was not as large as in the two Wenchuan earthquake related PTSD studies of Wang et al. [39] and Ying et al. [40], and the area surveyed was not expansive as them, so the rate presented in this study needs to be further investigated. And the results show that PTSD was significantly correlated with FOMO (r = .347, p < .01; H2), and Fake News Belief (r = .444, p < .01; H3), with effect sizes ranging from moderate to large. And FOMO is also correlated with Fake News Belief (r = .482, p < .01; H1).

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations

Regression and mediation analyses

To test for mediation, we first employed a regression analysis using the three-step method [38] followed by an additional 5,000 simulations using bootstrapping to calculate confidence intervals for indirect effect bias correction. If the confidence interval does not contain zero, the indirect effect holds.

Analysis showed that PTSD had a significant positive effect on believing fake news (β = 0.444, p < .001), PTSD had a statistically significant positive effect on FOMO (β = 0.347, p < .001), and that PTSD and FOMO together had a significant positive effect on believing fake news (β = 0.373, p < .001). Our mediation model was therefore initially validated. Table 3 presents the results of this analysis.

Table 3 Regression analysis

Bootstrapped simulations of the mediated model were run 5,000 times using Model 4 of the PROCESS plugin [41]. As shown in Table 4, results demonstrated that PTSD had a significant indirect effect on believing fake news via FOMO (effect = 0.029, boot SE = 0.006, boot 95% CI = 0.019, 0.041), supporting the validity of the mediation model. PTSD also had a significant direct effect on believing fake news (effect = 0.071, boot SE = 0.011, boot 95% CI = 0.005, 0.093).

Table 4 Bootstrap test with mediator model

Moderator analysis

We tested age as a possible moderator of the PTSD → Fake News Belief and FOMO → believing fake news pathways. All variables were transformed for normalisation and analysed using Model 15 of PROCESS. Results are shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Analysis of the moderating effect of age

Analysis revealed a significant positive moderating effect of age on the PTSD → believing fake news pathway (β = 0.148, t = 3.097, p = .002). The positive effect of PTSD on believing fake news was greater when age was higher (+ 1SD) (β = 0.486, t = 6.796, p < .001) and smaller when age was lower (-1SD) (β = 0.191, t = 3.164, p = .002). Age also had a significant positive moderating effect on the FOMO → believing fake news pathway (β = 0.145, t = 3.122, p = .002). The effect of PTSD on FOMO was greater when age was higher (+ 1SD) (β = 0.486, t = 7.870, p < .001) than when and smaller when age was lower (-1SD) (β = 0.197, t = 2.892, p = .004). In addition, bootstrapped results of moderated mediation analysis revealed that the indirect effect was stronger when age was higher.

We next tested whether gender moderated the FOMO → believing fake news pathway using the same method as above. Results are shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Analysis of the moderating effect of gender

Gender had a significant positive moderating effect on the FOMO → believing fake news pathway (β = 0.281, t = 6.737, p < .001). Results of a simple slope analysis demonstrated that FOMO had a substantial positive effect on believing fake news in women (β = 0.640, t = 10.754, p < .001) but no such effect in males (β = 0.079, t = 1.279, p = .202). Additional bootstrapped results of the moderated mediation analysis showed that the mediation model was not valid for male participants (effect = 0.028, boot SE = 0.024, boot 95% CI = − 0.018, 0.077). For females, the model was significant (effect = 0.222, boot SE = 0.036, boot 95% CI = 0.155, 0.296).

Figure 4 shows the final evaluation of the mediated model with moderating effects.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mediation model with standardized path coefficients

Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to examine associations between PTSD, FOMO, and belief of fake news in a sample of earthquake survivors in Wenchuan, China. Results indicated that FOMO was positively associated with PTSD and believing fake news, supporting our hypotheses H1 and H2. This is also consistent with Elhai et al. [7, 8, 27, 28]’s research, while he suggests that FOMO may be an important variable in explaining certain types of psychopathology. We found that the PTSD of Wenchuan earthquake survivors is indeed related to their belief in fake news, supporting H3. This is consistent with findings from Yang et al. [4] who studied a sample of adolescents that had experienced a major earthquake. Next, we found that FOMO mediated the association between PTSD and believing fake news, supporting our hypothesis H4.

Considering the moderating effect of age, we found that it contributed to both direct and indirect effects in the model, with effects being stronger as age increased. This supports our hypotheses H5 and H6 and follows findings from Sela et al. [6], who found moderate associations between depression, FOMO, and problematic Internet use in adolescents. The finding may be explained by younger people typically accessing news and information through a range of friendship groups and social platforms [22], whereas older people tend to access more limited sources of information and may be less able to identify misinformation. Enhancing social skills [6], improving social support networks [4], and using positive thinking therapy [16] may help to ameliorate the negative effects of trust in fake news in those who experience FOMO, particularly older individuals.

We explored age and gender as potential moderating variables of our mediational model following Yang’s [4] approach of treating gender and age as covariates to avoid multicollinearity. Supporting our hypothesis H7, we found that gender moderated the FOMO → believing fake news pathway, such that the effect only existed in females. This is in line with findings from previous studies [16, 30]. It has been reported that women spend more time on social media, perhaps because women use smart devices more for communication whereas men use them more for entertainment such as playing games, a possible reason why women are more likely to experience FOMO [30]. Women are also more likely than men to imagine negative scenarios resulting from natural disasters and public safety emergencies, perhaps due to higher levels of ruminative thinking [13] or higher proneness to stress and anxiety [33].

Limitations

Our study had several limitations. The sample was limited to a single country, meaning findings may not translate to other locations or cultural contexts. We therefore encourage other researchers to employ a similar research design in other populations to validate our findings. The study relied on self-reported measures to assess the variables of interest. While this follows similar studies [4, 8, 15, 16], we note that more objective measures such as structured diagnostic interviews might strengthen the validity of the findings [15].

Implications

Despite these limitations, our findings have various theoretical and practical implications. Regarding the former, this is possibly the first study to explore links between PTSD, FOMO, and belief in fake news, and so it makes a significant academic contribution to the evidence base. It also does this in the context of a developing region. The study and its findings are timely due to the substantial increase in the dissemination of fake news during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Regarding practical implications, findings may be useful for policymakers and scholars. Given the observation that females and older individuals are more susceptible to fake news, it may be worth considering the implementation of safeguarding policies for specific demographic subgroups. One potential approach might involve monitoring the use duration of specific applications and issuing tailored security notifications to certain user groups. We also highly recommend the implementation of campaigns to raise awareness of fake news and promote media literacy and fact-checking. The media should assume a more proactive role in fostering awareness about fake news and its consequences. An example that could be followed is goviralgame.com, a UK platform in which players learn to resist manipulation techniques used to spread misinformation about COVID-19 [42]. Relatedly, Chinese media, educational institutions, and government should act responsibly in content creation and management. and engage in multisectoral action to treat those affected by traumatic events.

To mitigate the negative effects of fake news on those suffering from PTSD, future research could explore applicable psychological therapies such as positive thinking therapy [16]. Researchers could also further examine correlations between individual differences such as personality traits and specific platform characteristics to better understand who is most vulnerable to fake news [42].

Conclusion

This study has shown that PTSD symptoms in Wenchuan earthquake survivors are associated with the FOMO and believing fake news. Additionally, FOMO was found to mediate the association between PTSD and believing fake news. Moderating effects of age on both direct and indirect pathways were observed, and we found that the mediating effect was present among females but not males. Results provide an important reference for those wishing to analyse psychological variables in studies of Internet and social media use in those with PTSD.

Data Availability

The raw data can be found in https://osf.io/3v9t8/. More details of this study are available from the corresponding author(cgong22@m.fudan.edu.cn), upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Wang Z. A preliminary report on the Great Wenchuan Earthquake. Earthq Eng Eng Vib. 2008;7(2):225–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11803-008-0856-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Fan X, Juang CH, Wasowski J, Huang R, Xu Q, Scaringi G, van Westen CJ, Havenith H-B. What we have learned from the 2008 Wenchuan Earthquake and its aftermath: a decade of research and challenges. Eng Geol. 2018;241:25–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enggeo.2018.05.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Contractor AA, Frankfurt SB, Weiss NH, Elhai JD. Latent-level relations between DSM-5 PTSD symptom clusters and problematic smartphone use. Comput Hum Behav. 2017;72:170–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Yang X, Wu X, Qi J, Zhou X. Posttraumatic stress symptoms, adversity belief, and internet addiction in adolescents who experienced a major Earthquake. Curr Psychol. 2022;41(5):3013–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00816-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Oberst U, Wegmann E, Stodt B, Brand M, Chamarro A. Negative consequences from heavy social networking in adolescents: the mediating role of fear of missing out. J Adolesc. 2017;55(1):51–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Sela Y, Zach M, Amichay-Hamburger Y, Mishali M, Omer H. Family environment and problematic internet use among adolescents: the mediating roles of depression and fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav. 2020;106:106226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Elhai JD, Levine JC, Alghraibeh AM, Alafnan AA, Aldraiweesh AA, Hall BJ. Fear of missing out: testing relationships with negative affectivity, online social engagement, and problematic smartphone use. Comput Hum Behav. 2018;89:289–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Elhai JD, Gallinari EF, Rozgonjuk D, Yang H. Depression, anxiety and fear of missing out as correlates of social, non-social and problematic smartphone use. Addict Behav. 2020;105:106335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Przybylski AK, Murayama K, DeHaan CR, Gladwell V. Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav. 2013;29(4):1841–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Baker ZG, Krieger H, LeRoy AS. Fear of missing out: relationships with depression, mindfulness, and physical symptoms. Translational Issues in Psychological Science. 2016;2(3):275–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Roberts JA, David ME. The Social Media Party: fear of missing out (FoMO), Social Media Intensity, connection, and well-being. Int J Human–Computer Interact. 2020;36(4):386–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2019.1646517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Alt D, Boniel-Nissim M. Using multidimensional scaling and PLS-SEM to assess the relationships between personality traits, problematic internet use, and fear of missing out. Behav Inform Technol. 2018;37(12):1264–76. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2018.1502353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Düzce University (Turkey), Bayın, Ü, Makas S, Sakarya University (Turkey), Çelik, E., Sakarya University (Turkey), Biçener, E., & University S, Turkey). (. (2021). Examination of Individuals’ Level of Fear of COVID-19, Fear of Missing Out (FoMO), and Ruminative Thought Style. Education Quarterly Reviews, 4(2). https://doi.org/10.31014/aior.1993.04.02.215.

  14. Contractor AA, Weiss NH, Tull MT, Elhai JD. PTSD’s relation with problematic smartphone use: mediating role of impulsivity. Comput Hum Behav. 2017;75:177–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.05.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Wolniewicz CA, Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD. Boredom proneness and fear of missing out mediate relations between depression and anxiety with problematic smartphone use. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. 2020;2(1):61–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Yuan G, Elhai JD, Hall BJ. The influence of depressive symptoms and fear of missing out on severity of problematic smartphone use and internet gaming disorder among Chinese young adults: a three-wave mediation model. Addict Behav. 2021;112:106648. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Tandon A, Dhir A, Almugren I, AlNemer GN, Mäntymäki M. Fear of missing out (FoMO) among social media users: a systematic literature review, synthesis and framework for future research. Internet Res. 2021;31(3):782–821. https://doi.org/10.1108/INTR-11-2019-0455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Dhir A, Yossatorn Y, Kaur P, Chen S. Online social media fatigue and psychological wellbeing—A study of compulsive use, fear of missing out, fatigue, anxiety and depression. Int J Inf Manag. 2018;40:141–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ahmed S. Disinformation sharing thrives with fear of missing out among low Cognitive News users: a cross-national examination of intentional sharing of deep fakes. J Broadcast Electron Media. 2022;66(1):89–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838151.2022.2034826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Jo H, Yang F, Yan Q. Spreaders vs victims: the nuanced relationship between age and misinformation via FoMO and digital literacy in different cultures. New Media & Society. 2022;146144482211304. https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448221130476.

  21. Hattingh M, Dhir A, Ractham P, Ferraris A, Yahiaoui D. Factors mediating social media-induced fear of missing out (FoMO) and social media fatigue: a comparative study among Instagram and Snapchat users. Technol Forecast Soc Chang. 2022;185:122099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techfore.2022.122099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Leung ANM, Law W, Liang YY, Au ACL, Li C, Ng HKS. What explains the Association between Usage of Social Networking Sites (SNS) and depression symptoms? The mediating roles of self-esteem and fear of missing out. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(8):3916. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18083916.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Delmastro M, Paciello M. Depression, reduced education, and bias perceptions as risk factors of beliefs in misinformation. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):16408.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Yu S-C, Chen H-R, Liu A-C, Lee H-Y. Toward COVID-19 information: infodemic or fear of missing out? Healthcare. 2020;8(4):550. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare8040550.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Talwar S, Dhir A, Kaur P, Zafar N, Alrasheedy M. Why do people share fake news? Associations between the dark side of social media use and fake news sharing behavior. J Retailing Consumer Serv. 2019;51:72–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2019.05.026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Alt D. College students’ academic motivation, media engagement and fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav. 2015;49:111–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Elhai JD, McKay D, Yang H, Minaya C, Montag C, Asmundson GJG. Health anxiety related to problematic smartphone use and gaming disorder severity during COVID -19: fear of missing out as a mediator. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. 2021;3(1):137–46. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Elhai JD, Yang H, Fang J, Bai X, Hall BJ. Depression and anxiety symptoms are related to problematic smartphone use severity in Chinese young adults: fear of missing out as a mediator. Addict Behav. 2020;101:105962. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.04.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Elhai JD, Levine JC, Dvorak RD, Hall BJ. Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Comput Hum Behav. 2016;63:509–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Sultan Ibrahim SA, Dahlan A, Nur Wahida Mahmud Pauzi, Vetrayan J. (2022). Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) and its relation with Depression and Anxiety among University Students. Environment-Behaviour Proceedings Journal, 7(20), 233–238. https://doi.org/10.21834/ebpj.v7i20.3358.

  31. Xie X, Wang Y, Wang P, Zhao F, Lei L. Basic psychological needs satisfaction and fear of missing out: friend support moderated the mediating effect of individual relative deprivation. Psychiatry Res. 2018;268:223–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.07.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Weathers FW, Litz B, Herman D, Juska J, Keane T. PTSD Checklist—Civilian Version [dataset]. American Psychological Association; 2012. https://doi.org/10.1037/t02622-000.

  33. Hou F, Bi F, Jiao R, Luo D, Song K. Gender differences of depression and anxiety among social media users during the COVID-19 outbreak in China:a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):1648. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-09738-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Hattingh M, Dhir A, Ractham P, Ferraris A, Yahiaoui D. Factors mediating social media-induced fear of missing out (FoMO) and social media fatigue: a comparative study among Instagram and Snapchat users. Technol Forecast Soc Chang. 2022b;185:122099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techfore.2022.122099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Vale L, Silcock J, Rawles J. An economic evaluation of thrombolysis in a remote rural community. BMJ. 1997;314(7080):570–0. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.314.7080.570.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Cooper B, Eva N, Fazlelahi FZ, Newman A, Lee A, Obschonka M. Addressing common method variance and endogeneity in vocational behavior research: a review of the literature and suggestions for future research. J Vocat Behav. 2020;121:103472. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20640-7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Hair JF. (2009). Multivariate data analysis.

  38. Ying L, Wu X, Lin C, et al. Prevalence and predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and depressive symptoms among child survivors 1 year following the Wenchuan Earthquake in China. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;22:567–75. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-013-0400-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. Guilford publications; 2017.

  40. Balakrishnan V, Ng KS, Rahim HA. To share or not to share – the underlying motives of sharing fake news amidst the COVID-19 pandemic in Malaysia. Technol Soc. 2021;66:101676. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techsoc.2021.101676.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Li JB, Yang A, Dou K et al (2020) Chinese public’s knowledge, perceived severity, and perceived controllability of COVID-19 and their associations with emotional and behavioural reactions, social participation, and precautionary behaviour: a national survey. BMC Public Health 20:1589. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-09695-1

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Wang W, Fu W, Wu J, Ma XC, Sun XL, Huang Y, Gao CG (2012) Prevalence of PTSD and depression among junior middle school students in a rural town far from the epicenter of the Wenchuan earthquake in China. PLoS ONE 7(7):e41665. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041665

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank all participants for their valuable contributions to this study. And we also sincerely acknowledge front line staffs, community workers, police and anyone who involves in the fight against the Wenchuan Earthquake.

Funding

This study is funded by Sichuan Mianyang Social Science Grant Program (MY2022YB084), Yijin Ren is the PI of this project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Chen Gong: writing original draft and editing, assistance in data analysis.Yijin Ren: conceptual-ization, experiment conducting, data processing.Two authors contributed equally to this work. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chen Gong.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Mianyang College of Administration. Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gong, C., Ren, Y. PTSD, FOMO and fake news beliefs: a cross-sectional study of Wenchuan earthquake survivors. BMC Public Health 23, 2213 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-17151-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-17151-z

Keywords