Skip to main content

Measurement of craving among gamers with internet gaming disorder using repeated presentations of game videos: a resting-state electroencephalography study

Abstract

Background

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is receiving increasing attention owing to its effects on daily living and psychological function.

Methods

In this study, electroencephalography was used to compare neural activity triggered by repeated presentation of a stimulus in healthy controls (HCs) and those with IGD. A total of 42 adult men were categorized into two groups (IGD, n = 21) based on Y-IAT-K scores. Participants were required to watch repeated presentations of video games while wearing a head-mounted display, and the delta (D), theta (T), alpha (A), beta (B), and gamma (G) activities in the prefrontal (PF), central (C), and parieto-occipital (PO) regions were analyzed.

Results

The IGD group exhibited higher absolute powers of DC, DPO, TC, TPO, BC, and BPO than HCs. Among the IGD classification models, a neural network achieves the highest average accuracy of 93% (5-fold cross validation) and 84% (test).

Conclusions

These findings may significantly contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of the neurological features associated with IGD and provide potential neurological markers that can be used to distinguish between individuals with IGD and HCs.

Peer Review reports

Background

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) is generally defined as problematic and compulsive use of internet gaming, leading to significant impairment in social, educational, and/or occupational activities. IGD has emerged as a social problem in adolescents and young adults because of its high prevalence and various possible comorbidities [1, 2]. Additionally, IGD was included in Section III of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) and has been classified as a tentative disorder, warranting further research, and is fully recognized as an independent clinical disorder [3]. In 2019, gaming disorder (GD) was defined as a mental illness according to the 11th revision of the World Health Organization’s International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) [4]. Furthermore, IGD was classified as being most similar to pathological gambling (or “gambling disorder”) by the DSM-5 and was defined as including the following nine criteria [5, 6]: (1) preoccupation with internet games (IG); (2) withdrawal symptoms from IG; (3) tolerance of and increasing engagement in IG; (4) unsuccessful attempts to stop or reduce IG; (5) loss of interest in other hobbies or activities; (6) gameplay (e.g. binge and continuous excessive gaming); (7) deception regarding the amount of time spent engaged in IG; (8) escape or relief from a negative mood; and (9) jeopardized or lost relationship, job, educational or career opportunity. Other studies have defined IGD as engaging in IG for over 14 hours per week for a minimum of one year [7] and reported IG as a major online activity [8].

Internet gaming is a popular and enjoyable activity. However, IGD has received increasing attention because of its negative effects on job and academic performance, normal daily life, and social and psychological functioning [3, 9]. Furthermore, IGD is known to be strongly associated with various comorbid psychological states, such as depressed mood [10] and anxiety [10, 11], psychiatric disorders [12], attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [10, 11], and obsessive-compulsive disorder [10, 13]. Based on 37 cross-sectional studies, IGD is a serious issue worldwide, with a 1.4% prevalence in Norway (among 16–74-year-olds), 1.6% in seven European countries (14–17-year-olds), 4.3% in Hungary (among 15–16-year-olds), 5.5% in Germany (among 13–20-year-olds), 8.5% in the United States (among 8–18-year-olds), 17% in Iran (14–15-year-olds) [14], and 5.9% in South Korea (among 14–15-year-olds) [15]. Evidently, IGD is not a problem confined to individuals but a grave social issue that threatens society. Therefore, research dedicated to relieving this condition is required.

First, an accurate and robust method for measuring symptoms in individuals with IGD should be developed; however, the lack of consistency in screening tools is a major issue in this field [16]. Consequently, several studies have tried to measure IGD characteristics using self-reporting, behavioral responses, physiological responses, and brain function tests, as follows. (1) Several studies have proposed that a GD (or addiction) can be measured by self-reporting using the internet addiction test (IAT) [17], internet-related problem questionnaire [18], 7-item game addiction scale [19], and the video game addiction test [20]. (2) Research into behavioral responses has found that individuals with IGD exhibit a decreased eye blinking rate and saccadic movement [21] and an increased number of regressions in eye movement [22]. (3) Some studies have shown that IGD is associated with physiological responses such as skin conductance responses [23], respiratory rate changes [21], reduction in the standard deviation of normal-to-normal intervals [21] and high frequency of HRV [22, 24, 25]. (4) Other studies have identified the IGD phenomenon in electroencephalogram (EEG) oscillations, brain activity, event-related potentials (ERPs), and activity in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Participants with IGD show increasing delta and theta powers in the frontal area [23] and decreasing beta and gamma bands in the frontal and parietal lobes [26, 27]. In addition, the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), superior parietal lobe, and paralimbic and orbital frontal lobes are negatively associated with response inhibition performance in individuals with IGD [28, 29]. However, positive correlations have been observed in the activity of the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, inter-hemispheric insula connectivity, right inferior temporal cortex, primary somatosensory cortex, inferior parietal lobule, middle occipital gyrus, and bilateral DLPFC [28, 30, 31]. In an ERP study associated with inhibition function, participants with IGD showed that N2 latency at the central [32] and P3 latency at the midline centro-parietal areas [33] were delayed. The N2 amplitude in the frontal area [32] and late positive potential (LPP) in the centro-parietal area [21] increased, and P3 amplitudes at the midline centro-parietal [33,34,35] and N1 amplitudes in the midline fronto-central regions [35] decreased compared with those in healthy controls (HCs). In fMRI studies, participants with IGD showed higher levels of activation in the lateral and prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, right medial orbitofrontal cortex, bilateral supplementary motor area, superior frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, temporal gyrus, left postcentral gyrus, striatum, precuneus, putamen, pallidum, left anterior cingulate (ACC), and left caudate than those in the HCs group, which was related to craving experience, risk-evaluation network, goal-directed behavior, default mode network, cognitive control network, and executive functions [29, 36,37,38,39,40]. These studies used stimuli or tasks to measure the responses of the IGD group, including the resting state [9, 25, 27, 33, 37, 41], playing online or video games [22, 24], watching online gameplay videos or images [36], viewing game videos or images [28, 30, 42, 43], Go/No-Go task [32, 38], cue reactivity task [21, 29], probability discounting task [38], oddball task [33,34,35], and Stroop task [31, 39].

Several previous studies have investigated the cortical activity associated with IGD to help diagnose, prevent, or treat the condition. However, these studies mainly attempted to confirm activation of the cortex in IGD based on non-repeated stimuli, including playing games and viewing game videos or images. However, they did not examine the physiological responses to repeated stimuli. Some researchers contend that exposure to repeatedly presented stimuli over a short or long period may lower the thresholds of the visual sensory system, partly because of habituation effects [44,45,46]. However, the psychophysiological responses caused by repetitive stimuli should be investigated when examining the EEG correlates of specific diseases such as unconscious addiction symptoms. Such addiction symptoms may arise from long-term exposure to visual or auditory stimulation via both conscious and unconscious pathways. Synaptic actions are activated by trains of neural impulses, and subtle but reliable changes can be observed at the cortical level. Repeated stimulation more reliably produces strong cortical activity than single stimulation in which information is processed simultaneously along both conscious and unconscious pathways. In addition, dual processing in the cognitive control system reflects habituation and sensitization. Salient stimuli elicit both processes, and electrophysiological outputs reflect the summation of these processes. It has also been reported that cumulative physiological responses to repeatedly presented stimuli do not decrease in participants but rather increase [45, 47]. The tendency toward habituation responses is related to the presence of numerous identical repetitions, whereas sensitization responses present progressively stronger activation in response to repeated stimuli [48]. We considered that physiological responses to repeated stimuli in individuals with IGD might differ significantly from those in HCs because disorders or addictions are affected by differential habituation and sensitization between HCs and IGD groups in response to repeated or prolonged presentation of a stimulus [49, 50]. Thus, we investigated the EEG correlates of IGD using repeatedly presented stimuli, which can induce more robust cortical activity in IGD groups than in previous methods.

There are many previous studies on IGD based on resting state EEG [9, 25, 27, 34, 37, 41]; however, there are few studies regarding its use for IGD measurement or other practical uses in this context. Quantitative measurement of IGD can have positive effects. Accordingly, this study aims to classify participants into HCs and IGD groups by conducting various machine-learning algorithms. In order to better distinguish them, we determined the effect of repeated presentation of a stimulus (gameplay video) on HCs and IGD groups, based on their EEG responses. The hypothesis was as follows: viewing gameplay video could induce different neural activities in individuals from the HCs and the IGD group. As previously mentioned, these activities can be correlated to habituation and sensitization. In this study, we determined the spectral power as features of resting state EEG. We also hypothesized that the whole brain area and all frequency bands were subject to analysis because many previous studies on resting state EEG have reported differences in several frequency bands on various areas. The whole brain was divided into three areas: prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital. The frequency bands included delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma bands.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-two male adolescents ranging in age from 14 to 22 years (mean age, 19.31 ± 2.51 years) were recruited for this study. Internet-related disorder has emerged as a social problem in male adolescents, who tend to invest more time playing games than their female peers [51,52,53]. Therefore, only male adolescents were included in this study. They were required to respond to questionnaire items that measured IGD symptoms using the Korean version of Young’s Internet Addiction Test (IAT) [54], a translated and validated Korean version of the Internet Addiction Test (Y-IAT-K). According to the Y-IAT-K, 42 participants were assigned to two groups, as follows: (1) 21 HCs, ranging in age from 15 to 22 years (mean age, 19.65 ± 2.58 years), with a score of 40 or less and (2) 21 participants with IGD, ranging in age from 14 to 22 years (mean age, 18.80 ± 2.33 years), with a score of 60 or above. Twenty participants who did not meet the inclusion criteria (Y-IAT-K score of 41–59) were excluded from this study. We used the independent sample t-test to confirm that there were no demographic differences in terms of age between the two groups (t [39] = − 0.936, p = 0.355). We investigated the average gameplay hours per day for 1 week before the experiment in all participants (HCs: 1.02 ± 1.46, IGD: 5.52 ± 2.95). Two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) was applied in this study and required a sample size of 40 based on G*power calculations (ANOVA: Repeated measures, between factors; effect size f = 0.40, α = 0.05, 1 − β = 0.80, number of group: 2, number of measurements: 2, corr among rep measures: 0.5). This study satisfied the sample size criteria with 84 samples (pre- vs. post-resting state [within-participant factors], 42 samples; IGD group vs. HCs [between-participants factors], 42 samples).

Participation was voluntary, and participants were paid US$ 53.07. All participants were right-handed and had no family or medical history of central nervous system disease. By self-reported questionnaire including asking “Do you have any psychiatric disorder, including other addiction?”, we screened other psychiatric disorders out, such as anxiety, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obsessive-compulsive disorder, or substance/behavioral addictions. Especially, we tested their rate of anxiety, ADHD, depression, impulsiveness, and aggression by the Korean-Beck Anxiety Inventory, Conners-Wells’ Adolescent Self-Report Scale or Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales-Self Report, Korean-Beck Depression Inventory-II, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale version 11, Aggression Questionnaire-Korean version, respectively. Finally, HCs had no psychiatric disorder and IGD group also had only IGD. The HCs had previously experienced the games used in this study at least once but did not enjoy the game. The IGD group preferred the games used in this study and enjoyed playing them. They were required to abstain from game-play, alcohol, smoking, and caffeine for at least 24 hours before the experiment and sleep according to their normal schedule. Each participant was informed of the experimental procedure (but not of the research purpose) before providing written informed consent. The experiments were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all protocols in this study were approved by the institutional review board.

Stimuli and experimental procedure

Game videos were used in this study to increase the craving experience for internet gaming. The steps for selecting the stimuli are shown in Fig. 1 and described below. (1) First, as stimuli, we selected three top-ranking games from the best online games in 2017 based on online game ranking in Korea (Naver Inc., Korea), including the League of Legends (Riot Games Inc., USA), Sudden Attack (Nexon Inc., Korea), and FIFA Online 3 (Electronic Arts Inc., USA). We collected 72 video sources of game highlights and fantastic play scenes with numerous hits. (2) We also amassed 72 neutral emotion videos associated with natural landscapes from an internet survey to recover the response to the game stimulus presented above. (3) Game and neutral stimuli were used to survey their suitability, based on subjective ratings (5-point scale), of 30 male adolescents (19.63 ± 2.33 years) for craving and relaxation experience, respectively. Following the subjective ratings, 72 stimuli (36 games and 36 neutral videos) were selected based on their high craving and relaxation scores, as shown in Fig. 1. Subsequently, we recruited participants who enjoyed the game used in the experiment and excluded those who mainly enjoyed other games. Therefore, before the experiment, the participants were controlled such that there was no difference in their familiarity with the videos. The videos selected for this study are available at https://youtu.be/5JL3DVmXaJk.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Procedure for stimuli selection. b Examples of still images from the game (top; FIFA Online 3, Sudden Attack, and League of Legends) and neutral (bottom) videos

Figure 2 illustrates the experimental procedure and environment. The participants watched the video stimuli using head-mounted display virtual reality (VR) devices (Oculus Rift S, Oculus VR Inc., USA) to increase their engagement with the content while sitting on a comfortable chair. Thus, we tested the data in all channels using a simple test before the experiment. No abnormalities in the EEG signals have been reported. The experiment consisted of watching neutral and game videos for 25 s each and resting for 5 s between viewing each stimulus image (one trial). After viewing the game video in each trial, the participants were required to respond to their craving experience (5-point scale) to play a game. In this experiment, 36 gameplay videos (three types of games × 12 examples) and 36 neutral videos were used. We applied counterbalancing to the game types. As there were 12 different videos for each game type, the videos could be arranged randomly such that runs of the same type did not occur. In addition, 36 neutral videos were randomly selected. Each trial consisted of watching one neutral video and one video game. The various stimulus sets consisted of 36 trials organized in this manner. Finally, we randomly selected one of the stimulus sets to use with a given participant. The trial intervals lasted 10 s, and the pre- and post-resting states were in a relaxed environment for 5 min each before and after the experiment. EEG signals were measured during the experiment. As this study aimed to investigate the effect of the habituation process by repeated presentation of a stimulus on the HCs and IGD groups, EEG activity was analyzed in the pre- and post-resting states for 5 min each. Spectral EEG activity (i.e., power ratio) in the post-resting state was compared to subtle changes recorded in the pre-resting state. EEG data recorded during observation of gaming and neutral videos were not included in the data analysis.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Experimental procedure (left) and environment (right)

Data acquisition and signals processing

The EEG data were recorded at a sampling rate of 2048 Hz from 64 channels mounted on an EEG electrode cap (Active-two, BioSemi S.V., Amsterdam, Netherlands) arranged in the international 10–20 montage, and the ground and reference electrodes were replaced by the common mode sense (CMS)/driven right leg (DRL), which is specific to BioSemi systems (cf. http://www.biosemi.com/faq/cms&drl.htm for further information). To analyze the EEG signals, they were downsampled to 512 Hz and re-referenced using a common average referencing (CAR) procedure. CAR was calculated by subtracting each channel from the average potential over all channels at each time step. This is also known as removing the global background activity and maintaining the activity from local sources beneath the electrodes [55], whereas other resting-state EEG studies have applied the CAR procedure [56]. The EEG signals were then processed using a band-pass filter (Butterworth type of order six) of 0.5–55 Hz. However, because EEG channels can be contaminated by noise such as ocular and muscular artifacts, these artifacts were removed from the EEG signals using artifact subspace reconstruction [57]. Among the total 5 min pre- and post-resting state data, we used only the first part, 2 min data for calculating power spectral density (PSD). The PSD was analyzed using Welch’s method and the parameters were as follow: window size, 5 seconds; window overlap size, 1 seconds; frequency resolution, 0.2 Hz. The EEG spectrum was divided according to the frequency band into the following ranges: delta (D) 1–4 Hz, theta (T) 4–8 Hz, alpha (A) 8–13 Hz, beta (B) 13–30 Hz, and gamma (G) 30–50 Hz [58, 59]. Brain regions in this study were defined as the prefrontal (FP1, FPZ, FP2, and AFZ), central (C3, C1, CZ, C2, and C4), and parieto-occipital areas (PO3, POZ, PO4, and OZ), and the absolute powers from the averaged signals in each brain region were extracted. The EEG signals were measured before, during, and after stimulus presentation. However, the EEG data recorded during the observation of game and neutral videos were not included in the data analysis because this study aimed to investigate subtle changes in EEG activity between the pre- and post-resting states in the HCS and IGD groups. All signal processing and data analyses were performed using EEGLAB, a toolbox in MATLAB Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA).

Statistical analysis

This study was designed to compare changes in cortical activity between the pre- and post-resting states (within-participants factor) between the IGD and HCS groups (between-participants factor). Next, ANOVA was performed to investigate group-by-time interaction effects and each main effect on the EEG spectral power. Comparisons of subjective ratings (i.e., craving scores) between the HCS and IGD groups were performed using the Mann–Whitney U test with a smaller sample size. To confirm practical significance, the effect size was calculated based on a two-way ANOVA (partial eta-squared, ƞp2) and Mann–Whitney U test (r). The standard partial eta-squared (ƞp2) values of 0.01, 0.06, and 0.14 for effect size are generally regarded as small, medium, and large, respectively [60]. In the case of non-parametric tests (absolute value of r), standard values of 0.10, 0.30, and 0.50 for effect size are generally considered small, medium, and large, respectively [61]. The expected effect size as a partial eta-squared value (ƞp2) was 0.087 (α = 0.05, 1 – β = 0.80, and total sample size = 84), as calculated by G*power software. To correct inflated type I errors caused by multiple comparisons, statistical significance was adjusted using the Benjamini–Hochberg (BH) false discovery rate (FDR) correction [62]. BH correction controls p-values more effectively than the traditional Bonferroni correction [62]. Additionally, the BH correction has been applied in many neurophysiological studies to handle the increasing rate of type I errors caused by multiple null hypothesis testing in a statistically valid manner [63]. In this study, the alpha level of 0.05 was used as the FDR criterion. As shown in the Results section, the p-value for the prefrontal theta was the highest p-value smaller than the critical value (0.0228 < 0.0233). All values above this value (i.e. those with lower p-values) were considered significant. Therefore, the adjusted alpha level was 0.0228 after BH. Thus, we conducted a correlation analysis between significant EEG features, the Y-IAT-K, and craving scores using Spearman’s rank correlation. All statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 21.0, for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Classification

To determine the best classification algorithm for our features, we used three basic machine learning algorithms: Discriminant Analysis (DA), Support Vector Machine (SVM), and Neural Network (NN) [64,65,66,67]. To train the above classifiers, the Classification Learner App from the MATLAB toolbox (2022b, Mathworks Inc.) was used. We obtained the parameters for each classifier using the hyperparameter optimizer in the classification learner application. The optimization options included: optimizer, Bayesian optimization; acquisition function, expected improvement per second plus; iterations, 100; and training time limit, no. The classifiers were validated 5-fold cross-validation with cross-validated portion including shuffled samples and tested with tested portion 0.3. We trained the above classifiers with optimized parameters. In this study, the number of features for classifiers were 15. And the number for total samples were 168 by augmenting samples. Total samples were randomly allocated to training, validation and test by using ‘cvpartition’ function with hold out option (0.3) and kfold option (5-fold). Of these samples, 120 were allocated to training and validation, while the remaining 48 were designated as test samples. Sample augmentation was conducted by dividing 5-minute data into 2-minute segmentation was conducted by dividing 5-minute data into 2-minute segments with 50% overlap, and ensuring that the training, validation, and test samples did not include data from the same subjects. Validation accuracy was calculated as the average accuracy for 5-fold cross-validated data, while test accuracy was determined as the average accuracy for test data on each fold. We also reported the area under the curve (AUC) of the ROC curve to represent its performance. Here, AUC is the area under the ROC curve (X-axis: 1 − specificity, Y-axis: sensitivity). The AUC value lies between 0 and 1, where 0 denotes a bad classifier and 1 denotes an excellent classifier [68].

Results

Craving scores

The Mann–Whitney U test demonstrated that the subjective rating for craving experience (mean of 36 trials) was significantly higher in the IGD group than in the HCS (3.76 ± 0.84 vs. 1.99 ± 0.54; U = 24.00, p < 0.001, r = 0.78, with a large effect size). In all trials, craving scores tended to be maintained without any significant differences, whereas both groups experienced repetitive game stimuli. The craving scores of the IGD group were higher than those of the HCS group (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Results of subjective ratings for craving experience (mean and scores from 36 trials) in the healthy controls (HCs) and internet gaming disorder (IGD) groups (*** p < 0.001)

EEG activity

In this study, the EEG features in the brain area were defined by the following abbreviations: (1) delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma power in the prefrontal region: DPF, TPF, APF, BPF, and GPF, respectively. (2) Delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma powers in the central region: DC, TC, AC, BC, and GC, respectively. (3) Delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma powers in the parieto-occipital region: DPO, TPO, APO, BPO, and GPO. A two-way ANOVA was used for the entire EEG data set based on the results of the Shapiro–Wilk normality test (p > 0.05). Two-way ANOVA for delta power in the prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions revealed a significant main effect of the group in DC (F1, 38 = 7.711, p = 0.007, ƞp2 = 0.089) and DPO regions (F1, 38 = 7.031, p = 0.010, ƞp2 = 0.082), whereas DPF showed no significant difference (F1, 38 = 4.079, p = 0.049). The main effects of time on DPF (F1, 38 = 0.112, p = 0.739), DC (F1, 38 = 0.472, p = 0.494), and DPO (F1, 38 = 0.000, p = 0.988) were not significant. No significant group × time interaction was identified. In the post-hoc analysis for main effect of group, an independent samples t-test revealed that delta power in the IGD group was significantly larger than that in the HCs group in central (t [39] = − 2.777, p = 0.007) and parieto-occipital regions (t [39] = − 2.695, p = 0.009), as shown in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Comparisons of averaged delta power in prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions between healthy control (HC) and internet gaming disorder (IGD) groups in both pre- and post-resting states. a Results of two-way ANOVA. b Electroencephalogram (EEG) topography in the delta band (1–4 Hz) in pre- and post-resting states

Two-way ANOVA for theta power in the prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions revealed a significant main effect of the group in TC (F1, 38 = 5.506, p = 0.021, ƞp2 = 0.065) and TPO regions (F1, 38 = 10.153, p = 0.002, ƞp2 = 0.114), whereas TPF showed no significant difference (F1, 38 = 5.085, p = 0.027, ƞp2 = 0.060). The main effects of time on TPF (F1, 38 = 1.246, p = 0.268), TC (F1, 38 = 0.021, p = 0.885), and TPO (F1, 38 = 0.565, p = 0.455) were not significant. No significant group × time interaction was observed. In the post-hoc analysis for main effect of group, an independent samples t-test revealed that theta power in the IGD group was significantly larger than that in the HCs group in central (t [39] = − 2.415, p = 0.019) and parieto-occipital regions (t [39] = − 3.184, p = 0.002), as shown in Fig. 5.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Comparisons of averaged theta power in prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions between healthy control (HCs) and internet gaming disorder (IGD) groups in both pre- and post-resting states. a Results of two-way ANOVA. b Electroencephalogram (EEG) topography in the theta band (4–8 Hz) in pre- and post-resting states

Two-way ANOVA for alpha power in the prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions revealed no significant results for the main effects of the group in AFP (F1, 38 = 3.698, p = 0.058), AC (F1, 38 = 3.151, p = 0.080), APO (F1, 38 = 4.226, p = 0.043), or time in AFP (F1, 38 = 0.592, p = 0.444), AC (F1, 38 = 0.152, p = 0.698), and APO (F1, 38 = 1.345, p = 0.250). In addition, no significant group × time interaction was found, as shown in Fig. 6.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Comparisons of averaged alpha power in prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions between healthy control (HC) and internet gaming disorder (IGD) groups in both pre- and post-resting states. a Results of two-way ANOVA. b Electroencephalogram (EEG) topography in the alpha band (8–13 Hz) in pre- and post-resting states

Two-way ANOVA for beta power in the prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions revealed a significant main effect of the group in BC (F1, 38 = 5.768, p = 0.019, ƞp2 = 0.068) and BPO regions (F1, 38 = 10.272, p = 0.002, ƞp2 = 0.115), but BPF showed no significant results (F1, 38 = 0.034, p = 0.854). The main effects of time on BPF (F1, 38 = 0.017, p = 0.897), BC (F1, 38 = 0.483, p = 0.489), and BPO (F1, 38 = 0.051, p = 0.822) were not significant. No significant group × time interaction was found. In the post-hoc analysis for main effect of group, an independent samples t-test revealed that beta power in the IGD group was significantly larger than that in the HCs group in central (t [39] = − 2.480, p = 0.016) and parieto-occipital regions (t [39] = − 3.249, p = 0.002), as shown in Fig. 7.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Comparisons of averaged beta power in prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions between healthy control (HC) and internet gaming disorder (IGD) groups in both pre- and post-resting states. a Results of two-way ANOVA. b Electroencephalogram (EEG) topography in the beta band (13–30 Hz) for in pre- and post-resting states

Two-way ANOVA for gamma power in the prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions revealed no significant results for the main effect of the group in GFP (F1, 38 = 0.378, p = 0.540), GC (F1, 38 = 0.667, p = 0.417), and GPO (F1, 38 = 4.151, p = 0.045) or time in GFP (F1, 38 = 0.010, p = 0.922), GC (F1, 38 = 0.639, p = 0.427), and GPO (F1, 38 = 0.578, p = 0.450). No significant group × time interaction was found, as shown in Fig. 8. The detailed results are shown in Table 1.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Comparisons of averaged gamma power in prefrontal, central, and parieto-occipital regions between healthy control (HC) and internet gaming disorder (IGD) groups in both pre- and post-resting states. a Results of two-way ANOVA. b Electroencephalogram (EEG) topography in the gamma band (30–50 Hz) in pre- and post-resting states

Table 1 Summary table of two-way ANOVA. The findings confirmed statistical significance is bolded (p < 0.0228)

Classification

Feature design

In this study, we compared EEG power between the HCS and IGD groups. We calculated and used “Power difference” as a feature for classification to clarify the differences between EEG power in the post-resting and pre-resting states as post-resting EEG after undergoing the experimental protocol affected HC and IGD differently and each participant had different pre-resting state EEG.

$$\boldsymbol{Power}\ \boldsymbol{difference}=\boldsymbol{Post}\ \boldsymbol{EEG}\ \boldsymbol{power}\ \left(\boldsymbol{dB}\right)-\boldsymbol{Pre}\ \boldsymbol{EEG}\ \boldsymbol{power}\ \left(\boldsymbol{dB}\right)$$

The following features were used for classification: power ratio for all frequency bands over the entire brain area (15 features).

Performance

As shown in Table 2, we classified participants to distinguish between the HCS and IGD groups. According to the three classifiers (DA, SVM, and NN), we achieved the following respective values for accuracy (0.87, 0.86, and 0.93), sensitivity (0.85, 0.89, and 0.96), specificity (0.89, 0.85, and 0.91), and AUC (0.90, 0.89, and 0.94) on 5-fold cross validation. On test data, the performance was respectively accuracy (0.73, 0.75, and 0.84), sensitivity (0.76, 0.77, and 0.82), specificity (0.73, 0.73, and 0.87), and AUC (0.80, 0.85, and 0.89). The ROC curves for three classification methods are shown in Fig. 9.

Table 2 Performance of different types of classifiers discriminated by healthy control (HC) and internet gaming disorder (IGD) groups
Fig. 9
figure 9

Receiver operating characteristics curves for three classifiers on (a) 5-fold cross-validation and (b) Test

Fig. 10
figure 10

The distribution for the three classifiers for the permutation test (p < 0.01). a Discriminant Analysis. b Support Vector Machine. c Neural Network

Discussion and conclusion

The primary aim of this study was to determine the electrophysiological features of individuals with IGD by comparing the EEG activity of the HCS and IGD groups during the repeated presentation of a stimulus (game video). Participants were assigned to the HCS and IGD groups based on their Y-IAT-K scores, and their craving scores for gaming were measured while performing the task. The subjective ratings for craving scores in the HCs and IGD groups indicated that participants in the IGD group experienced cravings for gaming, whereas those in the HCs group did not. We confirmed the results of the subjective rating of the craving experienced by classifying the two groups according to significant differences. EEG features showed significant differences between the IGD and HCs groups. Because these features were significant on two-way ANOVA on group effect, they could distinguish between the HCs and IGD groups, as well. Among the algorithms used to classify the participants into IGD and HCs groups, the NN algorithm demonstrated the highest average recognition accuracy of 93% (5-fold cross-validation) and 84% (test). Hence, NN was found to be the most suitable classifier for the IGD group.

The IGD group showed higher absolute powers of DC, DPO, TC, TPO, BC, and BPO than the HCs group after repeated presentations of the game stimuli. A two-way ANOVA revealed that there was no statistically significant interaction between the effects of group and time. The main effects of the group showed a significant difference, but time was not significant. These results can be interpreted to mean that the group did have a statistically significant effect on absolute powers of DC, DPO, TC, TPO, BC, and BPO. Previous studies have reported that patients with addictive disorders exhibit flow experiences with feelings of enjoyment arising from deep immersion (high-level attention) [69], dysfunction of behavioral inhibition in the prefrontal areas [21, 33,34,35]; and activation of the reward circuit (impaired executive control) [23, 41, 70, 71]. The present findings indicate a significant difference in delta, theta, and beta powers between the IGD and HCs groups, which is associated with a high-level attention state, based on previous study results. Delta, theta, and beta oscillations are strongly related to many cognitive processes including attention, memory operations, decision-making and action control, memory recognition, and mental workload in the frontal, central, temporal, and parietal regions [23, 72,73,74]. In addition, changes in delta and beta activities are associated with behavioral inhibition [75, 76] and reward networks [77, 78], respectively. Thus, the changes in delta, theta, and beta powers found in this study can be interpreted as being strongly related to flow experience, dysfunction of behavioral inhibition, and activation of the reward circuit. Our results provide evidence that supports the potential use of changes in delta, theta, and beta powers as electrophysiological features of the traits investigated in this study, which is consistent with findings from previous studies on patients with addictive disorders as follows: (1) delta, theta [79], and beta powers [79, 80] are increased in gambling disorders; (2) delta [81, 82], theta [81], and beta powers [81, 83] are increased in alcohol use disorders; (3) delta, theta [84], and beta powers [85] are increased in food disorders; (4) beta [86] power is increased in smokers; and (5) theta [87] power is increased in cocaine users.

As mentioned above, because delta and theta powers are strongly related to cognitive processes and behavioral inhibition, HCs can be interpreted as having a low attention level and normal function for behavioral inhibition caused by the repeated presentation of game stimuli. In addition, theta oscillations are likely generated in the ACC and subcortical limbic structures such as the hippocampus [88, 89], and decreasing theta oscillations are associated with low cognitive control [90, 91]. Moreover, decreasing delta oscillations have been reported to be related to mental workload [92, 93]. Thus, the low interest (attention) in the stimuli and high mental workload of the participants in the HCs group indicated that they found repeated presentations of game videos boring and mental stress. The difference between the two groups was related to the degree of interest in the game; therefore, the EEG response to repeated presentation stimuli may be used as an electrophysiological feature to distinguish between the IGD and HCs groups.

Several previous studies related to pure resting-state EEG have reported that subjects suffering from IGD reveal increasing delta and theta powers in the frontal area [23] and decreasing beta and gamma bands in the frontal and parietal lobes [26, 27]. This is inconsistent with the findings of our study. The results of pure resting-state EEG may be different from our findings because disorders or addictions are affected by differential habituation and sensitization between IGD and HCs groups from repeated or prolonged presentation of a stimulus [49, 50]. This study used repeated or prolonged stimulus to induce a distinct difference in the resting-state EEG between the IGD and HCs groups. Repeated stimulation can lead to different habituation or sensitization processes depending on the person’s interest in stimulation, further enhancing cortical activity. Repeated stimulus paradigms generate stronger cortical activity than does a single stimulus. Moreover, repeated stimuli cause cumulative physiological responses to increase [49, 50]. For example, since the HCs group had little or no interest in the game stimuli, the habituation process (i.e., boredom or disinterest) was induced. In contrast, the IGD group, which had a high interest in game stimuli, induced a sensitization process (i.e., craving). Previous studies related to brain activity have reported EEG responses in the pure resting state. (1) The beta power in the IGD group was higher than that in the HCs group [82]. (2) Patients with IGD showed increased resting-state EEG in slow-wave activity, such as delta and theta, compared with HCs [94]. The results of previous studies were inconsistent, but their findings on the delta, theta, and beta powers were consistent with the results of the present study. These results suggest that cortical activity is enhanced by the repeated or prolonged presentation of a stimulus. Generally, strict protocols are more distinctive between individuals and reproducible compared to those without tasks (i.e., resting-state) [95]. This suggests that the method proposed here can better induce electrophysiological responses in IGD groups than that proposed in previous studies and helps in distinguishing IGD from HCs. As mentioned above, because delta and beta powers are strongly related to behavioral inhibition and the reward circuit, our results can be interpreted as maintaining the dysfunction of behavioral inhibition in the prefrontal area and activation of the reward circuit, even after exposure to repeated stimuli.

In this study, we attempted to develop a quantitative method for the measurement of IGD and provide evidence to confirm its suitability for this purpose. If a quantitative diagnosis can be developed, it can have positive effects, such as (1) providing feedback to doctors or therapist to treat symptoms, (2) allowing therapeutic restrictions on playing games to be rationally based on IGD level (i.e., time spent playing the games), and (3) decreasing the development of IGD in high-risk groups by early measurement of IGD.

This study had several limitations. (1) Because additional groups were not considered in the experimental design of this study, the pattern of EEG features found in this study may be related to other behavioral addictions as well. IGD is also associated with depressed mood, anxiety, psychiatric disorders, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. The possible comorbidity of IGD with other mental disorders should be considered when determining electrophysiological features of IGD. Thus, it cannot be conclusively stated that the findings of this study can distinguish IGD from other addictions or mental diseases; our findings require confirmation through further research. (2) Only male participants were included in this study because male adolescents tend to invest more time in playing games than female participants [53, 96]. The occurrence of IGD is higher in men than in women; however, IGD is also often reported in women. Thus, female participants should be considered in future studies on IGD. (3) The experiment was conducted based on a VR environment. None of the participants had any VR experience prior to the experiment. Therefore, this inter-participant difference from the VR-effect might be low. Although some participants could adapt to VR better than others, Pöhlmann et al. [97] reported that the discomfort from VR caused different effects such as illusion strength. This point is known to cause inter-participant differences according to degree of discomfort [97]. Therefore, the feelings of the participant after the VR experiment should be considered. (4) We believe that repeated presentation stimuli can give rise to different processes of habituation or sensitization depending on the person’s interest in stimuli of gaming and neutral video, further enhancing cortical activity. However, while watching both gaming and neutral video, it is possible that the person suffering from IGD is unknowingly also addicted to screen (TV, monitor, or mobile) regardless of the content type being watched. To clarify this issue, it is essential to separately compare pre- and post-resting state EEGs when neutral and gaming videos are being watched.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available in the devhaji / IGD_datasets repository (github), [https://github.com/devhaji/IGD_dataset/blob/main/README.md].

References

  1. Peeters M, Koning I, van den Eijnden R. Predicting internet gaming disorder symptoms in Young adolescents: a one-year follow-up study. Comput Hum Behav. 2018;80:255–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Jeong H, Yim HW, Lee SY, Lee HK, Potenza MN, Jo SJ, et al. A partial mediation effect of father-child attachment and self-esteem between parental marital conflict and subsequent features of internet gaming disorder in children: a 12-month follow-up study. BMC Public Health. 2020;20:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-08615-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. American Psychological Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  4. King DL, Potenza MN. Not playing around: gaming disorder in the international classification of diseases (ICD-11). J Adolesc Health. 2019;64:5–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.10.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. King DL, Delfabbro PH. The cognitive psychology of internet gaming disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34:298–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.03.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf HJ, Mößle T, et al. An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction. 2014;109:1399–406. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Yao YW, Chen PR, Chen C, Wang LJ, Zhang JT, Xue G, et al. Failure to utilize feedback causes decision-making deficits among excessive internet gamers. Psychiatry Res. 2014;219:583–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Yao YW, Chen PR, Li S, Wang LJ, Zhang JT, Yip SW, et al. Decision-making for risky gains and losses among college students with internet gaming disorder. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0116471. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116471.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Park S, Ryu H, Lee JY, Choi A, Kim DJ, Kim SN, et al. Longitudinal changes in neural connectivity in patients with internet gaming disorder: a resting-state EEG coherence study. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:252. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00252.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Pearcy BT, McEvoy PM, Roberts LD. Internet gaming disorder explains unique variance in psychological distress and disability after controlling for comorbid depression, OCD, ADHD, and anxiety. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2017;20:126–32. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Yen JY, Liu TL, Wang PW, Chen CS, Yen CF, Ko CH. Association between internet gaming disorder and adult attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and their correlates: impulsivity and hostility. Addict Behav. 2017;64:308–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.04.024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Kim NR, Hwang SS, Choi JS, Kim DJ, Demetrovics Z, Király O, et al. Characteristics and psychiatric symptoms of internet gaming disorder among adults using self-reported DSM-5 criteria. Psychiatry Investig. 2016;13:58–66. https://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2016.13.1.58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Starcevic V, Aboujaoude E. Internet gaming disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and addiction. Curr Addict Rep. 2017;4:317–22. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0158-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Saunders JB, Hao W, Long J, King DL, Mann K, Fauth-Bühler M, et al. Gaming disorder: its delineation as an important condition for diagnosis, management, and prevention. J Behav Addict. 2017;6:271–9. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.039.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Yu H, Cho J. Prevalence of internet gaming disorder among Korean adolescents and associations with non-psychotic psychological symptoms, and physical aggression. Am J Health Behav. 2016;40:705–16. https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.40.6.3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Király O, Bőthe B, Ramos-Diaz J, Rahimi-Movaghar A, Lukavska K, Hrabec O, et al. Ten-item internet gaming disorder test (Igdt-10): measurement invariance and cross-cultural validation across seven language-based samples. Psychol Addict Behav. 2019;33:91–103. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Young KS. Caught in the net: how to recognize the signs of internet addiction—and a winning strategy for recovery. Wiley: John Wiley & Sons; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Armstrong L, Phillips JG, Saling L. Potential determinants of heavier internet usage. Int J Hum Comput Stud. 2000;53:537–50. https://doi.org/10.1006/ijhc.2000.0400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Development and validation of a game addiction scale for adolescents. Media Psych. 2009;12:77–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213260802669458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. van Rooij AJ, Schoenmakers TM, van den Eijnden RJ, Vermulst AA, van de Mheen D. Video game addiction test: validity and psychometric characteristics. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2012;15:507–11. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kim SN, Kim M, Lee TH, Lee JY, Park S, Park M, et al. Increased attentional Bias toward visual cues in internet gaming disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: an event-related potential study. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:315. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00315.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Lee TH, Kim M, Kwak YB, Hwang WJ, Kim T, Choi JS, et al. Altered eye-movement patterns during text Reading in obsessive-compulsive disorder and internet gaming disorder. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:248. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00248.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Balconi M, Campanella S, Finocchiaro R. Web addiction in the brain: cortical oscillations, autonomic activity, and behavioral measures. J Behav Addict. 2017;6:334–44. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.041.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Hong SJ, Lee D, Park J, Namkoong K, Lee J, Jang DP, et al. Altered heart rate variability during gameplay in internet gaming disorder: the impact of situations during the game. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:429. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00429.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Lee D, Hong SJ, Jung YC, Park J, Kim IY, Namkoong K. Altered heart rate variability during gaming in internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2018;21:259–67. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Park W, Ha J, Kim L. OP-81: neurophysiological indicators of internet gaming addiction in resting state electroencephalography. J Behav Addict. 2017;6:39–40.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Sun Y, Wang H, Bo S. Altered topological connectivity of internet addiction in resting-state EEG through network analysis. Addict Behav. 2019;95:49–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.02.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lai C, Altavilla D, Mazza M, Scappaticci S, Tambelli R, Aceto P, et al. Neural correlate of internet use in patients undergoing psychological treatment for internet addiction. J Ment Health. 2017;26:276–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638237.2017.1294745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Liu L, Yip SW, Zhang JT, Wang LJ, Shen ZJ, Liu B, et al. Activation of the ventral and dorsal striatum during Cue reactivity in internet gaming disorder. Addict Biol. 2017;22:791–801. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12338.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Ko CH, Liu GC, Yen JY, Chen CY, Yen CF, Chen CS. Brain correlates of craving for online gaming under Cue exposure in subjects with internet gaming addiction and in remitted subjects. Addict Biol. 2013;18:559–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00405.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Zhang Y, Lin X, Zhou H, Xu J, Du X, Dong G. Brain activity toward gaming-related cues in internet gaming disorder during an addiction Stroop task. Front Psychol. 2016;7:714. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00714.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Kim M, Lee TH, Choi JS, Kwak YB, Hwang WJ, Kim T, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of altered response inhibition in internet gaming disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: perspectives from impulsivity and compulsivity. Sci Rep. 2017;7:41742. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep41742.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Park M, Kim YJ, Kim DJ, Choi JS. Sustained dysfunctional information processing in patients with internet gaming disorder: 6-month follow-up ERP study. Medicine (Baltimore). 2017;96:e7995. https://doi.org/10.1097/MD.0000000000007995.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Park M, Choi JS, Park SM, Lee JY, Jung HY, Sohn BK, et al. Dysfunctional information processing during an auditory event-related potential task in individuals with internet gaming disorder. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6:e721. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.215.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Park M, Kim YJ, Choi JS. PM320. Different patterns of ERP components in internet gaming disorder and alcohol use disorder: an auditory event-related potential study. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;19:14. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyw041.320.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Dong G, Wang L, Du X, Potenza MN. Gaming increases craving to gaming-related stimuli in individuals with internet gaming disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017;2:404–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.01.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Han X, Wang Y, Jiang W, Bao X, Sun Y, Ding W, et al. Resting-state activity of prefrontal-striatal circuits in internet gaming disorder: changes with cognitive behavior therapy and predictors of treatment response. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:341. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00341.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Lin X, Zhou H, Dong G, Du X. Impaired risk evaluation in people with internet gaming disorder: FMRI evidence from a probability discounting task. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2015;56:142–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.08.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Wang L, Zhang Y, Lin X, Zhou H, Du X, Dong G. Group independent component analysis reveals alternation of right executive control network in internet gaming disorder. CNS Spectr. 2018;23:300–10. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852917000360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Zha R, Tao R, Kong Q, Li H, Liu Y, Huang R, et al. Impulse control differentiates internet gaming disorder from non-disordered but heavy internet gaming use: evidence from multiple behavioral and multimodal neuroimaging data. Comp Hum Behav. 2022;130:107184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2022.107184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Wang Y, Yin Y, Sun YW, Zhou Y, Chen X, Ding WN, et al. Decreased prefrontal lobe interhemispheric functional connectivity in adolescents with internet gaming disorder: a primary study using resting-state FMRI. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0118733. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118733.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Liu J, Li W, Zhou S, Zhang L, Wang Z, Zhang Y, et al. Functional characteristics of the brain in college students with internet gaming disorder. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016;10:60–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9364-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Zhang JT, Yao YW, Potenza MN, Xia CC, Lan J, Liu L, et al. Effects of craving behavioral intervention on neural substrates of Cue-induced craving in internet gaming disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;12:591–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.09.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Andreassi JL. Psychophysiology: human behavior and physiological response. Psychology Press; 2010.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  45. Coppola G, Di Lorenzo C, Schoenen J, Pierelli F. Habituation and sensitization in primary headaches. J Headache Pain. 2013;14:65. https://doi.org/10.1186/1129-2377-14-65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Stern RM, Ray WJ, Quigley KS. Psychophysiological recording. Oxford University Press; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Denny BT, Fan J, Fels S, Galitzer H, Schiller D, Koenigsberg HW. Sensitization of the neural salience network to repeated emotional stimuli following initial habituation in patients with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;175:657–64. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17030367.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Pace-Schott EF, Shepherd E, Spencer RM, Marcello M, Tucker M, Propper RE, et al. Napping promotes inter-session habituation to emotional stimuli. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;95:24–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.10.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Audrain-McGovern J, Strasser AA, Wileyto EP. Can repetitive mental simulation of smoking engender habituation. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2016;24:415–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/pha0000099.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Heinz A, Daedelow LS, Wackerhagen C, Di Chiara G. Addiction theory matters-why there is no dependence on caffeine or antidepressant medication. Addict Biol. 2020;25:e12735. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Chen KH, Oliffe JL, Kelly MT. Internet gaming disorder: an emergent health issue for men. Am J Mens Health. 2018;12:1151–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1557988318766950.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Fumero A, Marrero RJ, Bethencourt JM, Peñate W. Risk factors of internet gaming disorder symptoms in Spanish adolescents. Comput Hum Behav. 2020;111:106416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Vollmer C, Randler C, Horzum MB, Ayas T. Computer game addiction in adolescents and its relationship to Chronotype and personality. SAGE Open. 2014:4. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244013518054.

  54. Lee K, Lee HK, Gyeong H, Yu B, Song YM, Kim D. Reliability and validity of the Korean version of the internet addiction test among college students. J Korean Med Sci. 2013;28:763–8. https://doi.org/10.3346/jkms.2013.28.5.763.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Lew E, Chavarriaga R, Silvoni S, Millán JR. Detection of self-paced reaching movement intention from EEG signals. Front Neuroeng. 2012;5:13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneng.2012.00013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Zhang R, Yao D, Valdés-Sosa PA, Li F, Li P, Zhang T, et al. Efficient resting-state EEG network facilitates motor imagery performance. J Neural Eng. 2015;12:066024. https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-2560/12/6/066024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Mullen T, Kothe C, Chi YM, Ojeda A, Kerth T, Makeig S, et al. Real-Time Modeling and 3D Visualization of Source Dynamics and Connectivity Using Wearable EEG. In: 2013 35th annual international conference of the IEEE engineering in medicine and biology society (EMBC); 2013. p. 2184–7. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2013.6609968.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  58. Lin YP, Wang CH, Jung TP, Wu TL, Jeng SK, Duann JR, et al. EEG-based emotion recognition in music listening. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2010;57:1798–806. https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2010.2048568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Thomas KP, Vinod AP. EEG-based biometric authentication using gamma band power during rest state. Circuits Systems Signal Processing Circuits. 2018;37:277–89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00034-017-0551-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Huck SW, Cormier WH, Bounds WG. Reading statistics and research. New York: Harper & Row; 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Park S, Won MJ, Lee EC, Mun S, Park MC, Whang M. Evaluation of 3D cognitive fatigue using heart-brain synchronization. Int J Psychophysiol. 2015;97:120–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Royal Statistic Soc: Series B (Statist Method). 1995;57:289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Korthauer K, Kimes PK, Duvallet C, Reyes A, Subramanian A, Teng M, et al. A practical guide to methods controlling false discoveries in computational biology. Genome Biol. 2019;20:118. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13059-019-1716-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Rish I, Thyreau B, Thirion B, Plaze M, Paillere-martinot ML, Martelli C, et al. Discriminative network models of schizophrenia. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst. 2009;22:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Overall JE, Klett CJ. Applied multivariate analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Altman NS. An introduction to kernel and nearest-neighbor nonparametric regression. J Am Statist Assoc. 1992;46:175–85.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Narsky I, Porter FC. Statistical analysis techniques in particle physics. Wiley Online Library; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  68. Saito T, Rehmsmeier M. Precrec: fast and accurate precision-recall and ROC curve calculations in R. Bioinformatics. 2017;33:145–7. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btw570.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Sprong ME, Griffiths MD, Lloyd DP, Paul E, Buono FD. Comparison of the video game functional assessment-revised (VGFA-R) and internet gaming disorder test (IGD-20). Front Psychol. 2019;10:310. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00310.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Li Q, Wang Y, Yang Z, Dai W, Zheng Y, Sun Y, et al. Dysfunctional cognitive control and reward processing in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Psychophysiology. 2020;57:e13469. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Wang Y, Wu L, Zhou H, Lin X, Zhang Y, Du X, et al. Impaired executive control and reward circuit in internet gaming addicts under a delay discounting task: independent component analysis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2017;267:245–55. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-016-0721-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Gaume A, Dreyfus G, Vialatte FB. A cognitive brain-computer Interface monitoring sustained attentional variations during a continuous task. Cogn Neurodyn. 2019;13:257–69. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-019-09521-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Harper J, Malone SM, Bachman MD, Bernat EM. Stimulus sequence context differentially modulates inhibition-related Theta and Delta band activity in a go/no-go task. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:712–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12604.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Knyazev GG, Slobodskoj-Plusnin JY, Bocharov AV. Event-Related Delta and Theta synchronization during explicit and implicit emotion processing. Neuroscience. 2009;164:1588–600. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.057.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Knyazev GG. Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:377–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Putman P. Resting state EEG Delta-Beta coherence in relation to anxiety, behavioral inhibition, and selective attentional processing of threatening stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 2011;80:63–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.01.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Andreou C, Frielinghaus H, Rauh J, Mußmann M, Vauth S, Braun P, et al. Theta and high-Beta networks for feedback processing: a simultaneous EEG-FMRI study in healthy male subjects. Transl Psychiatry. 2017;7:e1016. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.287.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. HajiHosseini A, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Marco-Pallarés J. The role of Beta-gamma oscillations in unexpected rewards processing. Neuroimage. 2012;60:1678–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Massar SA, Kenemans JL, Schutter DJ. Resting-state EEG Theta activity and risk learning: sensitivity to reward or punishment. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;91:172–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.10.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Kim KM, Choi SW, Lee J, Kim JW. EEG correlates associated with the severity of gambling disorder and serum BDNF levels in patients with gambling disorder. J Behav Addict. 2018;7:331–8. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Saletu-Zyhlarz GM, Arnold O, Anderer P, Oberndorfer S, Walter H, Lesch OM, et al. Differences in brain function between relapsing and abstaining alcohol-dependent patients, Evaluated by EEG Mapping. Alcohol Alcohol. 2004;39:233–40. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agh041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Son KL, Choi JS, Lee J, Park SM, Lim JA, Lee JY, et al. Neurophysiological features of internet gaming disorder and alcohol use disorder: a resting-state EEG study. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e628. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.124.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Mumtaz W, Vuong PL, Malik AS, Rashid RBA. A review on EEG-based methods for screening and diagnosing alcohol use disorder. Cogn Neurodyn. 2018;12:141–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-017-9465-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Imperatori C, Fabbricatore M, Innamorati M, Farina B, Quintiliani MI, Lamis DA, et al. Modification of EEG functional connectivity and EEG power spectra in overweight and obese patients with food addiction: an eLORETA study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2015;9:703–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-014-9324-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Tammela LI, Pääkkönen A, Karhunen LJ, Karhu J, Uusitupa MI, Kuikka JT. Brain electrical activity during food presentation in obese binge-eating women. Clin Physiol Funct Imaging. 2010;30:135–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-097X.2009.00916.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Knott V, Cosgrove M, Villeneuve C, Fisher D, Millar A, McIntosh J. EEG correlates of imagery-induced cigarette craving in male and female smokers. Addict Behav. 2008;33:616–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Reid MS, Flammino F, Howard B, Nilsen D, Prichep LS. Topographic imaging of quantitative EEG in response to smoked cocaine self-Administration in Humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:872–84. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300888.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Mitchell DJ, McNaughton N, Flanagan D, Kirk IJ. Frontal-midline Theta from the perspective of hippocampal “Theta”. Prog Neurobiol. 2008;86:156–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Smith EH, Horga G, Yates MJ, Mikell CB, Banks GP, Pathak YJ, et al. Widespread temporal coding of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22:1883–91. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0494-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Angelidis A, Hagenaars M, van Son D, van der Does W, Putman P. Do not look away! Spontaneous frontal EEG Theta/Beta ratio as a marker for cognitive control over attention to mild and high threat. Biol Psychol. 2018;135:8–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Cavanagh JF, Shackman AJ. Frontal midline Theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: Meta-analytic evidence. J Physiol Paris. 2015;109:3–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Harmony T, Fernández T, Silva J, Bernal J, Díaz-Comas L, Reyes A, et al. EEG Delta activity: an Indicator of attention to internal processing during performance of mental tasks. Int J Psychophysiol. 1996;24:161–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00053-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Kumar N, Kumar J. Measurement of cognitive load in HCI systems using EEG power Spectrum: an experimental study. Procedia Comput Sci. 2016;84:70–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2016.04.068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Kim YJ, Lee JY, Oh S, Park M, Jung HY, Sohn BK, et al. Associations between prospective symptom changes and slow-wave activity in patients with internet gaming disorder: a resting-state EEG study. Medicine (Baltimore). 2017;96:e6178. https://doi.org/10.1097/MD.0000000000006178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Chan H, Kuo PC, Cheng CY, Chen YS. Challenges and future perspectives on electroencephalogram-based biometrics in person recognition. Front Neuroinform. 2018;12:66. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2018.00066.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Macur M, Pontes HM. Internet gaming disorder in adolescence: investigating profiles and associated risk factors. BMC Public Health. 2021;2021(211):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11394-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Pöhlmann KMT, O’Hare L, Dickinson P, Parke A, Föcker J. Action video game players do not differ in the perception of contrast-based motion illusions but experience more Vection and less discomfort in a virtual environment compared to non-action video game players. J Cogn Enhancement. 2022;6:3–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-021-00215-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the Institute of Information & Communications Technology Planning & Evaluation (IITP) grant funded by the Korean government (MSIT) [grant number 2017–0-00432] for the development of a non-invasive integrated BCI SW platform to control home appliances and external devices through an AR/VR interface.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, SP, and LK; methodology, SP; investigation, JH, and WA; data analysis, SP, and JH; writing—original draft preparation, SP; writing—review and editing, LK; visualization, JH, and WA; supervision, LK; project administration, JH, WA, and SP. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laehyun Kim.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Korea Institute of Science and Technology (KIST), Seoul, South Korea (No. 2017–013).

This study have included participants aged 16 years and less who are considered as minors, and informed consent was obtained from the parents and/or their legal guardians doe participation in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare(s) that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Park, S., Ha, J., Ahn, W. et al. Measurement of craving among gamers with internet gaming disorder using repeated presentations of game videos: a resting-state electroencephalography study. BMC Public Health 23, 816 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-15750-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-15750-4

Keywords