Skip to main content

The Seeking Mental Health Care model: prediction of help-seeking for depressive symptoms by stigma and mental illness representations

Abstract

Background

Only about half the people with depression seek professional health care services. To constitute the different predictors and associating variables of health care utilisation, we model the process and aim to test our hypothesised Seeking Mental Health Care Model. The model includes empirical influences on the help-seeking process to predict actual behaviour and incorporates superordinate (stigma, treatment experiences) as well as intermediate attitudinal variables (continuum and causal beliefs, depression literacy and self-efficacy).

Method

All variables are examined in an online study (baseline, three- and six-month follow-up). The sample consisted of adults with depressive symptoms (PHQ-9 sum score ≥ 8), currently not receiving mental health care treatment. To examine the prediction of variables explaining help-seeking behaviour, a path model analysis was carried out (lavaan package, software R).

Results

Altogether, 1368 participants (Mage = 42.38, SDage = 15.22, 65.6% female) were included, 983 participating in at least one follow-up. Model fit was excellent (i.e., RMSEA = 0.059, CFI = 0.989), and the model confirmed most of the hypothesised predictions. Intermediary variables were significantly associated with stigma and experiences. Depression literacy (ß = .28), continuum beliefs (ß = .11) and openness to a balanced biopsychosocial causal model (ß = .21) significantly influenced self-identification (R2 = .35), which among the causal beliefs and self-efficacy influenced help-seeking intention (R2 = .10). Intention (ß = .40) prospectively predicted help-seeking behaviour (R2 = .16).

Conclusion

The Seeking Mental Health Care Model provides an empirically validated conceptualisation of the help-seeking process of people with untreated depressive symptoms as a comprehensive approach considering internal influences. Implications and open questions are discussed, e.g., regarding differentiated assessment of self-efficacy, usefulness of continuum beliefs and causal beliefs in anti-stigma work, and replication of the model for other mental illnesses.

Trial registration

German Clinical Trials Register: DRKS00023557. Registered 11 December 2020. World Health Organization, Universal Trial Number: U1111–1264-9954. Registered 16 February 2021.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Only around one in two people contact professional support services for their depressive symptoms, despite considering a broad range of services, including psychiatric or psychotherapeutic help, general practitioners, and counselling [1]. Even in regions with high healthcare density, there is a substantial proportion of people who do not seek professional help for their depressive symptoms [2]. The pathway to treatment is influenced by various individual, system, and disease factors [3], including availability and accessibility of services and structural stigma [4]. Even though all these factors are relevant, this work is focused on investigating personal attitudinal and cognitive variables to identify and overcome potential barriers for seeking mental healthcare [5] due to the importance of attitudinal barriers to mental health treatment for mild and moderate symptoms [6]. We provide a comprehensive Seeking Mental Health Care Model explaining the help-seeking process, as well as influences of superordinate and intermediate variables (see Fig. 1). In the following paragraphs, we will summarise key empirical findings on attitudinal and cognitive variables influencing professional help-seeking. We refer to the illness representations of the Common Sense Model of Self-Regulation (CSM, [7]) as the underlying theoretical framework, which Scott et al. [3] also used in their model.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Seeking Mental Health Care Model.

(Note: Superordinate and intermediary variables and their direct or indirect influences on the help-seeking process for mental health problems. In brackets: Illness representations from the Common-Sense Model of Illness Regulation assigned to the respective variables. Hypothesis on self-efficacy to seek help on behaviour was not postulated in the study protocol, but incorporated retrospectively due to previous findings and with the aim of analysing a comprehensive model)

Help-seeking for mental illness is understood as a process of experiencing symptoms, identifying them as such, forming an intention, and lastly actual help-seeking [7,8,9]. The identification of symptoms of mental illness is a crucial starting point to forming intention [10,11,12,13]. However, a symptom is subjectively not necessarily conceptualised as illness because subjective experience and perceived functional changes influence symptom identification [14]. Therefore, the help-seeking process must start with becoming aware of one’s complaints as a symptom of mental health issues (i.e., self-identification).

Moreover, previous treatment experience influences the help-seeking process, including self-identification and intention [15] by reducing stigmatising attitudes [16]. Stigma in turn has been identified as a major barrier, hindering both self-identification and help-seeking initiation [13]. The types of stigma [17] differ in their influence towards help-seeking [18]. While evidence of the impact on perceived public and anticipated stigma on help-seeking is mixed [19, 20], internalized stigma has consistent negative influences on help-seeking attitudes and intention [13, 20,21,22,23,24]. In order to explain help-seeking for mental health problems, it is important to consider these outlined superordinate variables originating from the public and social reactions as well as personal experiences [3]. They might influence present internal beliefs and attitudes (i.e., intermediary variables) with direct influences on the help-seeking process. Figure 1 presents our Seeking Mental Health Care Model [7], which includes hypotheses most interesting for the total model whilst controlling for others. This is especially relevant for the multiple empirical associations between treatment experience and intermediary variables that are mentioned in the following sections. Therefore, instead of formulating every single correlational hypotheses to replicate each empirical finding, we see treatment experiences as superordinate influence that correlates with several model variables, but only formulate specific hypotheses of interest. In the next section we will present the intermediary variables (Fig. 1).

Continuum beliefs of mental health and illness are associated with lower stigmatising attitudes towards people with mental illness [25]. Concerning, for example, drinking behaviour, the understanding of drinking along a continuum is currently discussed as a way to promote self-identification as part of a broad operationalisation of problem recognition [26, 27]. Evidence is sparse for continuum beliefs of people with depression [25], however there are indications that a continuum model increases perceived similarities with a depression vignette [28].

Mental health literacy, the capacity to access, process, and apply information about mental illness and treatment [29], is recognised as an intermediary variable. It is associated with lower stigma and higher self-identification as well as being positively influenced by treatment experience [13, 15]. However, self-stigma seems to act as a barrier between mental health literacy and help-seeking, therefore these associations need further reassurance [30].

Causal beliefs concerning one’s mental health problems are associated with higher self-identification and are influenced by treatment experience [15]. However, there is conflicting evidence on various causal beliefs (e.g., biomedical, childhood related) and their relation to stigma (e.g., [31, 32]). A balanced biopsychosocial model based on biopsychosocial model of health [33] might reflect a realistic and effective causal belief model which influences help-seeking behaviour while balancing possible adverse effects of stigma [34, 35].

Self-efficacy [36] for engaging in health behaviours has mainly been investigated for physical health [37, 38]. For mental health help-seeking, it might be important to differentially consider task-specific self-efficacy. On the one hand, seeking professional help self-efficacy has been shown to be beneficial for help-seeking [9]. On the other hand, self-help self-efficacy might decrease professional help-seeking in favour of tendencies to help oneself [39, 40], however this has rarely been investigated. Concerning the association between treatment experience and self-efficacy we assume that they are positively associated, because treatment is shown to reduce self-stigma which is in turn negatively associated with self-efficacy [41,42,43].

Expanding the view to general health-related decision making, the Common-Sense Model of Self-Regulation (CSM; [7]) is applied. CSM has been investigated extensively in the context of physical health [44], but has also been replicated for the mental health context [45]. It assumes a dynamic process from becoming aware of health threats, their emotional processing to treatment use through an interplay of illness representations [14]. These include representations of the identity (labelling) and timeline expectations of symptom development, their causes, perceived controllability, coherence with internal concepts of illness, and consequences of past and present illnesses [14, 46]. In a recent meta-analysis, Cannon et al. [45] found large effects of illness representations on treatment use, however, only a few studies investigate treatment use for mental illness resulting in a need for further research in this field [45]. Therefore, we theoretically match the CSM illness representations to the above mentioned empirical variables influencing professional help-seeking behaviour (see Fig. 1). The subjective sense of illness is matched with the consequence representation and self-identification of having a mental illness with the identity representations. Especially the intermediary variables can roughly be assigned: The extent towards a person’s belief in a continuum of mental health and illness can be understood as the representation coherence. Depression literacy includes people’s concept of illness timeline, causal beliefs refer to causes representation, and self-efficacy represents the aspect of perceived controllability. These illness representations develop and change throughout the process of the illness, they interact with illness perceptions, subjective complaints, and experienced emotions. As a consequence, they respectively influence future processing of health threats and an integration of new experiences. Stigma negatively influences the CSM representations, as found for most of the described variables [47].

Our aim is to test the Seeking Mental Health Care Model, with the novel approach of incorporating superordinate stigma and experiences and five intermediary internal processes of illness perception that empirically influence help-seeking. Therefore, we use a prospective study on help-seeking in a population sample with currently untreated depressive symptoms [7].

Methods

This study is part of a project funded by the German Research Foundation, with a published study protocol [7] and preregistration in the German Clinical Trial Register (DRKS00023557). Data was collected by the online panel respondi AG via their online platform Mingle between January and September 2021. If screened eligible for study participation the panellists were informed about study content and procedure after which they could give electronic informed consent. The study included a quasi-experimental intervention design manipulating the intermediary variables. For the analyses, pre-intervention data is used, except for the behaviour variable which could only be assessed three to six months post intervention. However, this should not be an issue for testing interrelations of the variables, as the study has a fractioned factorial design. Furthermore, this study is not part of the original analysis but a secondary data analysis testing the model as a whole. For more information on the study design and interventional manipulation, refer to the study protocol [7].

Sample & power analysis

Participants without current professional treatment and with at least mild depressive symptoms were included (PHQ-9 sum score ≥ 8; 48). In total, N = 1867 participants completed the questionnaire, which assessed stigmatising attitudes, intermediary variables and help-seeking variables. The final sample size consisted of N = 1368, because participants were excluded if they completed the study under half the median duration [48] or had apparent monotone answer profiles (n = 116), if their PHQ-9 score was < 8 after the second assessment time 36 hours later (n = 362), or due to conflicting information about an individual’s gender between the study points (n = 12). Our sample only consisted of n = 9 people who reported being of diverse gender. Because of the small case number we decided to exclude them from the analyses, however we emphasize the importance of focusing on hard to reach, yet especially vulnerable gender groups [49], because otherwise this methodologically reasonable exclusion only reproduces discrimination regarding minorities access to care [50]. All participants were contacted for follow-ups after three and six months in which help-seeking behaviour was assessed. Altogether, N = 829 participated till the end of the study six months later. Refer to the study protocol for information on participant recruitment and content of assessment [7].

Because the path analysis is done in addition to the analyses reported in the study protocol [7] we recalculated the necessary sample size for this specific analysis. The estimated minimum sample size had to be N = 200 (as proposed by Boomsma [51];). With a stricter rule of thumb, with five to ten needed observations per estimated parameter [52, 53], we estimated a required sample size between N = 645 to 1290. Therefore, we concluded that our sample size and statistical power is sufficient.

Measures

All measures used for the path model analysis are listed in Table 1. The operationalisation of help-seeking intention and behaviour as well as other adapted variables are elucidated in further detail below. For extensive descriptions of all measures employed in the study refer to the study protocol [7].

Table 1 Overview of the measures used in the current study

With the Brief Illness Perception Questionnaire (B-IPQ; [46]) we assessed the participants subjective sense of illness. Eight items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale. One item assessing causal beliefs was excluded due to its open response format. A mean score was calculated, representing the degree to which the illness is perceived as threatening or benign, which we assume to be an important trigger point for the model. A higher score reflects a more threatening view of the illness. This operationalisation is suggested by Broadbent [64].

Causal beliefs of mental illness were assessed with a list of 18 possible causes [57]. Participants were asked to rate whether they believe a cause (e.g., “loneliness”) could be responsible for their experienced complaints. Rating was assessed on a 5-point Likert scale from 1=“definitely is not a cause” to 5=“definitely is a cause”. Internal consistency was acceptable with α = .78. With the aim of determining a balanced biopsychosocial causal model we calculated the BPS-CM index, which represents to what extent five different factors (i.e., biogenetic, psychological, social, environmental, comparable to Stolzenburg et al. [16]) are seen as possible causes for one’s own mental complaints. Between-factor variance is considered. A higher score indicates a more heterogeneous belief system. To determine the index, the 18 items were subsumed into factors and the within-factor agreement as well as the between-factor variance was calculated and multiplied with each other to consider the homogeneity of the belief system. Then, all items were recoded into a binary format: 0 = “definitely is not a cause” and 1 = “definitely is a cause”. The within-factor agreement was determined 0% representing the persons belief that none of the items belonging to one factor are a possible cause whilst 100% represents the belief that all items of one factor are considered to be possible causes (each factor contributing 20% equally). The between-factor variance was calculated to consider the homogeneity of the belief system. Higher inverted variance can be interpreted as higher similarity between factor means. The BPS-CM index ranges from 2.12 to 42.80. The range is specific to the sample, seeing as the variance is used to calculate the index.

Previous treatment experience was assessed with the question “Have you ever received treatment for mental illness in your life?”. Multiple responses were possible for: “medical treatment”, “psychotherapy”, “art-, music- and/or sport-therapy”, “self-help groups”, “coaching and counselling”, and “online or telephone therapy”. The answers were collapsed into a dichotomous variable with 0 = “no experience”, 1 = “treatment experience”.

Intention to seek help from a health professional was assessed with an 15-item list adapted from Pescosolido and Boyer [62]. Participants rated the probability of seeking help from potential persons (e.g., psychiatrist) and institutions (e.g., counselling centre) on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 = “extremely unlikely” to 7 = “extremely likely”. Internal consistency was good with α = .87. To operationalize intention to seek professional help, maximum scores across the items counselling centre, general practitioner, psychologists, psychotherapists, psychiatrists, and neurologist are determined reflecting the diversity of professional help sources. The final choice was based on the results of an explorative factor analysis in which these items loaded best together.

Help-seeking behaviour was assessed with the same list as intention. Respectively, participants were asked if they sought help in the past 3 months (0 = “no”, 1 = “yes”). When stating “yes”, a subsequent question specified whether they sought help for their psychological complaints (1 = “yes, exclusively”, 2=“yes, amongst other complaints”, 3=“no, because of other complaints). Only if they stated 1 or 2, were their answers coded as seeking help for their depressive complaints. To recode this, we collapsed responses for both follow-ups into dichotomous variables of help-seeking within the last three or six months with 0=“did not seek help” and 1=“sought help during either the three or six month follow-up”. The same persons and institutions described above were used to operationalize professional help-seeking behaviour.

Statistical analysis

No missing scale values are detected within the relevant data. Total drop-out rate was 28.14% between baseline assessment and both follow-ups together (i.e., participation in either three- or six-month follow up). To examine potential reasons for attrition we conducted logistic regressions in which dropout is defined as not providing any follow-up data, the variable is dummy-coded as 0 = “no missing variable” and 1 = “missing value due to drop-out” [65]. Predictors were self-identification, general health condition, depression severity, treatment experience, and various sociodemographic variables chosen broadly to accurately analyse possible reasons for attrition. We report Odds Ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for the significant coefficients.

To assess the proposed influences as shown in the Seeking Mental Health Care Model (Fig. 1), we conducted a path model analysis [66,67,68]. To check the hypothesised assumptions concerning the associations between the respective variables we performed Pearson’s product-moment and Kendal-Tau correlation analysis. We then conducted the path model analysis with the lavaan package [69] using the statistics software R version 4.0.3 [70]. The algorithm for the path model correlation matrix, exact specifications, and fitting commands, i.e., WLSMV estimator and pairwise deletion of missing values (refer to [71]), can be seen in the supplement (Table S1). We controlled for depressive complaints (PHQ sum score), age, and gender (dummy variable, “female” is the reference category), as well as for exogenous variables without specific hypotheses. All continuous variables were z-standardised. Standardised path estimates (interpretable as partial β-coefficients) for the different paths as well as corrected R2 estimates for the respective explained variances for the endogenous variables are reported. The usual model fit indices Χ2 (df and p-value), RMSEA, CFI, NFI, SRMR, and AGFI are reported.

Results

Sample characteristics

The final sample size consisted of N = 1368, of which 65.6% identified as female and the mean age was 42.38 (SD = 15.22). The mean depression score was 12.50 (SD = 4.08), indicating that the severity of depressive symptoms ranged mainly from mild to moderate [72]. People graduated school after 12/13 years (52.4%), 10 years (34.1%), or 9 years of schooling (11.3%) and some were still in school or had not graduated (2.2.%). 49.2% had professional training or finished an apprenticeship, and 22.7% had a university degree (i.e., bachelor’s degree = 7.8%, master’s degree = 13.9%, PhD = 1.0%). Collapsed sample size after 6 months was N = 983. Attrition analysis revealed that dropout was more likely if the participants were younger (OR = 0.963; CI = 0.952, 0.974), came from bigger households (OR = 1.174; CI = 1.047, 1.317), and if they had nine compared to 12 years of schooling (OR = 1.788; CI = 1.141, 2.803) while 11 other variables had no significant influence on dropout.

Path model analysis: the Seeking Mental Health Care model

The associations between the superordinate, intermediary, and help-seeking process variables are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 Correlation matrix for all superordinate, intermediary, and help-seeking process variables

Figure 2 shows the path model for seeking mental health care from a professional source (i.e., general practitioner, psychologist, therapist, psychiatrist, neurologist, or counselling centre). Significant standardised path estimates and non-significant yet hypothesised associations are reported. In the supplementary material we report the full model with all coefficients predicting or associated with the endogenous variables (Table S2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Path model for seeking mental health care from a professional source.

(Note: N = 1368. Standardized estimates with solid lines indicating significant relationships at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; broken lines non-significant, hypothesised relationships. Double headed arrows represent associations between the variables. One headed arrows represent predictions on endogenous variables. In brackets the estimated R2 for the endogenous variables. Modell is controlled for depression severity, age, and gender)

The model confirmed most of the proposed predictions of the help-seeking process and the associations between the superordinate and intermediary variables except for the prediction of previous treatment experience on self-efficacy to seek help and the prediction of self-efficacy to seek help on help-seeking behaviour. Overall, the model explained 35% of the variance of self-identification, 10% of intention to seek help and 16% of the variance of seeking a health-care professional after either three or six months. Model fit was excellent with Χ2 = 24.968, df = 7, p < .001, RMSEA = 0.059, CFI = 0.989, NFI = 0.988, SRMR = 0.012, and AGFI = 0.999.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to test a comprehensive Seeking Mental Health Care Model in a sample of people with currently untreated depressive symptoms. The model contains key internal influencing variables, from superordinate over intermediary to help-seeking process variables, to explain professional help-seeking behaviour due to mental health problems. A path analysis confirmed the general structure of the model with an excellent model fit, significantly predicting help-seeking behaviour while revealing reciprocal influences between process variables. Therefore, we can conclude that there are interdependent attitudinal and cognitive variables determining how symptoms are identified and processed in order to seek professional help. They support a theoretical proposed process comparable to processing according to CSM.

The subjective sense of illness is the starting point of the model triggering further intra-psychological variables and representing the dynamic nature of the model with subsequent variables relying on preceding processes. We operationalised the B-IPQ-R variables using the mean score, according to Broadbent [64], even if this is less subtle then the CSM would suggest [14]. When the illness is subjectively perceived as more threatening, the mean self-identification as having mental illness increased, which is in line with the work from O’Mahen et al. [73]. As expected, self-identification was higher the more heterogeneous the causal belief system and the higher depression literacy, which is in line with our previous findings [13, 15]. Further, on average, participants were more likely to recognize their current complaints as signs of mental illness if they report higher belief in a continuum of mental health and illness. To our knowledge, this is an addition to previous findings, which were rather focused on enhancing problem recognition for at-risk drinking through continuum beliefs [27]. The direct link between continuum beliefs and self-identification of having a mental illness should be investigated further (e.g., [74]).

For the prediction of help-seeking intention all hypothesized paths were significant. Higher self-identification lead to higher help-seeking intention meaning that identifying one’s own complaints as signs of a mental illness is an antecedent of intention to seek help [11,12,13]. Self-efficacy for professional help had the strongest influence on intention. The belief in a balanced causal model [35] as well as self-efficacy to help oneself had positive but small (ß < .10) influences. We did not assume the positive effect of self-help self-efficacy on intention, as this self-efficacy implies a desire to help oneself found to reduce the intention to seek professional help [75]. However, the moderate correlation between the two forms of self-efficacy supports that forming an intention to search for help could also be partly interpreted as an act of self-help. Self-help should therefore not solely be understood as having to deal with problems completely by oneself but rather as a set of behaviours engaged towards strengthening one’s health and self-care [76]. In contrast to other studies [8, 9], help-seeking intention was predicted with less explained variance, probably due to the lack of the well-established predictor help-seeking attitudes which was omitted because we aimed to examine less established variables.

For help-seeking behaviour, we found a well-established pattern of results. The influences of most variables were mediated through help-seeking intention as the strongest predictor [9, 13]. Along with that, our hypothesized influence of self-efficacy for professional help against our expectation was not significant, once again speaking for indirect effects of internal variables on behaviour via intention. Furthermore, having had treatment experience increased the probability to seek help, which is in line with other findings [13, 77, 78]. Our path model was able to explain 16% of variance of help-seeking behaviour. Keeping in mind that the strength of our study is that we used an outcome of actual utilisation of professional care within 6 months, uncontrolled external influences are probable. Moreover, the investigation of structural barriers was outside of our research scope, although their influences on help-seeking behaviour are evident as well [5, 79]. It should be noted that the resulting healthcare use might be influenced by the Covid-19 pandemic which was shown to overall decrease help-seeking behaviour for mental health [80]. Reinforced gender differences in help-seeking during Covid-19 were found for young people [81], which points to important future research directions. Moreover, sources of treatment like virtual treatments or other related (non-mental health) professionals like priests, teachers, police etc. should be investigated. This was outside our scope but could be differently affected by the influencing variables analysed in this study.

Regarding superordinate variables, treatment experience was significantly associated with stigma with small to moderate coefficients, which is in line with other research [16, 82]. The proposed associations between treatment experience and self-efficacy to seek help was not significant. Treatment experience per se might not influence the self-efficacy to seek help for subsequent health care utilisation. However, we didn’t include quality of treatment experience although it might be worthwhile to further investigate how quality of treatment experience actually affects stigma [83] and how specific experiences can be highlighted in interventions. Furthermore, it might be interesting to investigate how treatment experience and self-efficacy might both contribute to another construct, e.g., disease self-management [84], or are moderated, e.g., through trust towards healthcare providers [85]. Also, multiple associations of treatment experience with other variables were controlled for as to not overload the scope of this work.

We hypothesised associations between stigma and intermediary variables, i.e., continuum beliefs, mental health literacy, and causal beliefs. Lowest and mostly non-significant influences emerged with stereotype awareness replicating findings of its low influence on intrapsychic help-seeking processes [78]. We found a significant negative correlation of β = −.30 for stereotype agreement and β = −.22 for self-stigma of seeking help with continuum beliefs, which is in line with the overall negative associations found in public samples [25]. There is an absence of comparable samples, except for the work by Thibodeau et al. [86] who reported non-significant results. Because our results refer to a sample of people reporting depressive symptoms themselves, these results are an addition to existing literature, showing a tendency towards the effectiveness of continuum messages. Similarly, directed associations could be found for stigma and depression literacy opposing findings that stigma isn’t influenced by depression literacy [87]. For causal beliefs, we opted for a balanced biopsychosocial model, which we operationalised integrating the homogeneity of the belief system across different causal factors. Our aim was to balance the stigmatising potential of different causal beliefs [16, 57]. Still, our results indicate that more heterogeneous consideration of types of causes is associated with higher stigma. Due to these consistent findings across different operationalisations, future studies should use caution when incorporating causal belief messages as part of psychoeducative public health campaigns that aim to reduce stigma on an individual level. Importantly, we leave investigations of people with clinical diagnoses to future studies [4], since we wanted to include a population without current treatment.

Limitations

Our analyses are based exclusively on self-reported data. New instruments yet lacking full psychometric validation were used for continuum beliefs, causal beliefs, self-efficacy measures and treatment experience. However, the new questionnaires are based on existing validated items and therefore should still be interpretable, yet not fully comparable to other studies and thus need validation. We opted for a broad operationalisation of help-seeking including general practitioners, counselling centres, or neurologists, additional to psychotherapeutic or psychiatric professionals. We aimed to reflect help-seeking with high external validity since, for example, nearly three of four people with depression are treated by general practitioners [88]. Future studies could, on the one hand, investigate single health care providers to understand specific paths to health care, and on the other hand, incorporate informal sources as a mediating first step towards professional health care use [89].

Additionally, limitations to the representativeness of the sample are raised. On the one hand, we investigated a sample in Germany, limiting generalisability to other countries, partly due to differences regarding healthcare accessibility and statutory health insurance. Moreover, we limited our analyses to intrapersonal processes and their influencing factors. It would be important to replicate our analyses with focus on structural barriers, including factors of perceived and actual costs, availability and accessibility (e.g., transportation, hours of work, childcare requirements). Furthermore, even though we controlled for the influence of age, gender, and depression severity, there might be an influence of the socioeconomic status [79], including education, occupation and income. We did not include these control variables, due to the models already extensive inclusion of multiple variables and our aim of analysing internal processes of help-seeking. An interplay between internal and external factors could be a beneficial adaptation of the model, though the complexity might be an issue when statistically analysing such a comprehensive model.

The study is likely to contain a self-selection bias, as only motivated people with computer skills and access to the online panel might have been interested and able to participate. This longitudinal study assessed help-seeking behaviour over a period of up to three to six months with a drop-out from baseline to follow-ups. Attrition analyses have shown that those who drop out systematically differ from the rest of the sample in certain characteristics, further limiting representativeness. Additionally, this attrition is an indication of variables to address in further research, such as household size. So far, we limited our analyses to direct influences of the intermediary variables to provide insight on the influences of direct processes not considering combined influences nor possible reciprocal interactions between the intermediary variables. Correlational analyses revealed that they are partly negatively associated, suggesting that further analyses on the most effective combinations are needed, e.g., for drawing in-depth practical implications for creating help-seeking interventions with combined psychoeducational content.

Conclusion

In summary, we were able to replicate findings of various influences on the help-seeking process expanding existing research of help-seeking variables and stigma through an intermediary level of subjective illness representations according to the Common-Sense Model of Self-Regulation [14]. We examined a comprehensive approach and focused on internal attitudes of people currently not receiving treatment. Further studies should allow for an interaction between intermediary variables and expand the model to include both internal and external factors, as well as reciprocal associations. Although results need assurance in a sample with diagnosed depression, we see clinical implications of the discussed findings. Strengthening intermediary variables to decrease stigmatising attitudes and increase help-seeking behaviour in an interventional setting needs to be done with a comprehensive and holistic understanding of such a multifactorial model, for example by focusing on a destigmatising way to increase the likelihood that people with ill-health self-identify as having mental health symptoms. In sum, the Seeking Mental Health Care Model provides an empirically validated framework to consider the help-seeking process of people with untreated depressive symptoms within a widened approach considering many internal variables.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

B-IPQ:

Brief Illness Perception Questionnaire

BRAHMS:

Berlin Risk Appraisal and Health Motivation Study, 1996

BPS-CM:

Biopsychosocial Causal belief Model index

CSM:

Common Sense Model of Self-Regulation

DFG:

Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (i.e., German Research Foundation)

PHQ-9:

Patient Health Questionnaire (Depression Sub-Scale)

SELF-I:

Self-identification as having a mental illness

SSMI-public & -self-SF:

Short-Form of the Self-Stigma of Mental Illness Questionnaire

SSOSH-SF:

Short-Form of the Self-Stigma for Seeking Help Questionnaire

References

  1. Nübel J, Müllender S, Hapke U, Jacobi F. Epidemie der Depression? : Prävalenzentwicklung und Inanspruchnahme von Hilfs- und Versorgungsangeboten. Der Nervenarzt. 2019;90(11):1177–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Robert-Koch-Institut. The utilization of psychiatric and psychotherapeutic services in Germany – individual determinants and regional differences. 2019.

  3. Scott SE, Walter FM, Webster A, Sutton S, Emery J. The model of pathways to treatment: conceptualization and integration with existing theory. The British Journal of Health Psychology. 2013;18(1):45–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Magaard JL, Seeralan T, Schulz H, Brütt AL. Factors associated with help-seeking behaviour among individuals with major depression: a systematic review. PLoS One1. 2017;12(5):e0176730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Tomczyk S, Schmidt S, Muehlan H, Stolzenburg S, Schomerus G. A prospective study on structural and attitudinal barriers to professional help-seeking for currently untreated mental health problems in the community. The Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research. 2020;47(1):54–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Andrade LH, Alonso J, Mneimneh Z, Wells JE, Al-Hamzawi A, Borges G, et al. Barriers to mental health treatment: results from the WHO world mental health surveys. Psychological Medicine. 2014;44(6):1303–17.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. McLaren T, Peter L-J, Tomczyk S, Muehlan H, Stolzenburg S, Schomerus G, et al. How can the utilisation of help for mental disorders be improved? A quasi-experimental online study on the changeability of stigmatising attitudes and intermediate variables in the process of utilisation. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):2124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Schomerus G, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. Attitudes that determine willingness to seek psychiatric help for depression: a representative population survey applying the theory of planned behaviour. Psychological Medicine. 2009;39(11):1855–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Tomczyk S, Schomerus G, Stolzenburg S, Muehlan H, Schmidt S. Ready, willing and able? An investigation of the theory of planned behaviour in help-seeking for a community sample with current untreated depressive symptoms. Prevention Science. 2020;21(6):749–60.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Horsfield P, Stolzenburg S, Hahm S, Tomczyk S, Muehlan H, Schmidt S, et al. Self-labeling as having a mental or physical illness: the effects of stigma and implications for help-seeking. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2020;55(7):907–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kagstrom A, Alexova A, Tuskova E, Csajbók Z, Schomerus G, Formanek T, et al. The treatment gap for mental disorders and associated factors in the Czech Republic. European Psychiatry. 2019;59:37–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Picco L, Abdin E, Pang S, Vaingankar JA, Jeyagurunathan A, Chong SA, et al. Association between recognition and help-seeking preferences and stigma towards people with mental illness. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2018;27(1):84–93.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Schomerus G, Stolzenburg S, Freitag S, Speerforck S, Janowitz D, Evans-Lacko S, et al. Stigma as a barrier to recognizing personal mental illness and seeking help: a prospective study among untreated persons with mental illness. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2019;269(4):469–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Leventhal H, Phillips LA, Burns E. The common-sense model of self-regulation (CSM): a dynamic framework for understanding illness self-management. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2016;39(6):935–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Stolzenburg S, Freitag S, Evans-Lacko S, Speerforck S, Schmidt S, Schomerus G. Individuals with currently untreated mental illness: causal beliefs and readiness to seek help. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2019;28(4):446–57.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Stolzenburg S, Freitag S, Schmidt S, Schomerus G. Associations between causal attributions and personal stigmatizing attitudes in untreated persons with current mental health problems. Psychiatry Research. 2018;260:24–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Corrigan P, Druss BG, Perlick DA. The impact of mental illness stigma on seeking and participating in mental health care. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 2014;15(2):37–70.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Corrigan P, Wassel A. Understanding and influencing the stigma of mental illness. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services. 2008;46(1):42–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Golberstein E, Eisenberg D, Gollust SE. Perceived stigma and help-seeking behavior: longitudinal evidence from the healthy minds study. Psychiatric Services (Washington, D C). 2009;60(9):1254–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Pattyn E, Verhaeghe M, Sercu C, Bracke P. Public stigma and self-stigma: differential association with attitudes toward formal and informal help seeking. Psychiatric Services (Washington, D C). 2014;65(2):232–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Corrigan P, Watson AC. The paradox of self-stigma and mental illness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2002;9(1):35–53.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Fox AB, Smith BN, Vogt D. How and when does mental illness stigma impact treatment seeking? Longitudinal examination of relationships between anticipated and internalized stigma, symptom severity, and mental health service use. Psychiatry Research. 2018;268:15–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kulesza M, Pedersen E, Corrigan P, Marshall G. Help-seeking stigma and mental health treatment seeking among young adult veterans. Military Behavioral Health. 2015;3(4):230–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Schnyder N, Panczak R, Groth N, Schultze-Lutter F. Association between mental health-related stigma and active help-seeking: systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2017;210(4):261–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Peter LJ, Schindler S, Sander C, Schmidt S, Muehlan H, McLaren T, Tomczyk S, Speerforck S, Schomerus G. Continuum beliefs and mental illness stigma: a systematic review and meta-analysis of correlation and intervention studies. Psychol Med. 2021;51(5):716–26. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721000854.

  26. Morris J, Albery IP, Heather N, Moss AC. Continuum beliefs are associated with higher problem recognition than binary beliefs among harmful drinkers without addiction experience. Addictive Behaviors. 2020;105:106292.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Smith JJ, Spanakis P, Gribble R, Stevelink SAM, Rona RJ, Fear NT, et al. Prevalence of at-risk drinking recognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2022;235:109449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Buckwitz V, Bommes JN, Hinshaw SP, Schomerus G. Continuum beliefs and the perception of similarities and differences to a person with depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2022;116:152314.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Rodgers B, Pollitt P. “Mental health literacy”: a survey of the public's ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. The Medical Journal of Australia. 1997;166(4):182–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Brijnath B, Protheroe J, Mahtani KR, Antoniades J. Do web-based mental health literacy interventions improve the mental health literacy of adult consumers? Results from a systematic review. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2016;18(6):e165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Kvaale EP, Gottdiener WH, Haslam N. Biogenetic explanations and stigma: a meta-analytic review of associations among laypeople. Social Science and Medicine. 2013;96:95–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Rüsch N, Todd AR, Bodenhausen GV, Corrigan P. Biogenetic models of psychopathology, implicit guilt, and mental illness stigma. Psychiatry Research. 2010;179(3):328–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. World Health Organisation. Constitution of the World Health Organization - Basic Documents [Supplement]. 45th ed. 2006.

  34. Carter L, Read J, Pyle M, Morrison AP. Are causal beliefs associated with stigma? A test of the impact of biogenetic versus psychosocial explanations on stigma and internalized stigma in people experiencing psychosis. Stigma Health. 2019;4(2):170–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Nolan A, O’Connor C. The effect of causal attributions for depression on help-seeking and treatment preferences. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2019;257:477–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review. 1977;84(2):191–215.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Sheeran P, Maki A, Montanaro E, Avishai-Yitshak A, Bryan A, Klein WMP, et al. The impact of changing attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy on health-related intentions and behavior: a meta-analysis. Health Psychology. 2016;35(11):1178–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Zhang C-Q, Zhang R, Schwarzer R, Hagger MS. A meta-analysis of the health action process approach. Health Psychology. 2019;38(7):623–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Carpinello SE, Knight EL, Markowitz FE, Pease EA. The development of the mental health confidence scale: a measure of self-efficacy in individuals diagnosed with mental disorders. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal. 2000;23(3):236–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Clarke J, Proudfoot J, Birch M-R, Whitton AE, Parker G, Manicavasagar V, et al. Effects of mental health self-efficacy on outcomes of a mobile phone and web intervention for mild-to-moderate depression, anxiety and stress: secondary analysis of a randomised controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. 2014;14:272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Del Rosal E, González-Sanguino C, Bestea S, Boyd J, Muñoz M. Correlates and consequences of internalized stigma assessed through the internalized stigma of mental illness scale for people living with mental illness: a scoping review and meta-analysis from 2010. Stigma Health. 2021;6(3):324–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Rüsch N, Angermeyer MC, Corrigan P. Mental illness stigma: concepts, consequences, and initiatives to reduce stigma. European Psychiatry. 2005;20(8):529–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Corrigan P, Watson AC, Barr L. The self–stigma of mental illness: implications for self–esteem and self–efficacy. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2006;25(8):875–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Hagger MS, Koch S, Chatzisarantis NLD, Orbell S. The common sense model of self-regulation: Meta-analysis and test of a process model. Psychological Bulletin. 2017;143(11):1117–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Cannon M, Credé M, Kimber JM, Brunkow A, Nelson R, McAndrew LM. The common-sense model and mental illness outcomes: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2022;29(4):1186-202.

  46. Broadbent E, Petrie KJ, Main J, Weinman J. The brief illness perception questionnaire. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2006;60(6):631–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Mak WWS, Chong ESK, Wong CCY. Beyond attributions: Understanding public stigma of mental illness with the common sense model. The American Journal of Orthopedics. 2014;84(2):173–81.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Rössler P. Kumulierte Evidenzen: Replikationsstudien in der empirischen Kommunikationsforschung. Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden; 2017. Available from: URL: https://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/kxp/detail.action?docID=5042242.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Melendez RM, Bonem LA, Sember R. On bodies and research: transgender issues in health and HIV research articles. Sex Res Soc Policy. 2006;3(4):21–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Cruz TM. Assessing access to care for transgender and gender nonconforming people: a consideration of diversity in combating discrimination. Social Science and Medicine. 2014;110:65–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Boomsma A. Nonconvergence, improper solutions, and starting values in lisrel maximum likelihood estimation. Psychometrika. 1985;50(2):229–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Bentler PM, Chou C-P. Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociol Methods Res. 1987;16(1):78–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Bollen KA. Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley; 1989.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  54. Corrigan P, Michaels PJ, Vega E, Gause M, Watson AC, Rüsch N. Self-stigma of mental illness scale--short form: reliability and validity. Psychiatry Research. 2012;199(1):65–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Brenner RE, Colvin KF, Hammer JH, Vogel DL. Using item response theory to develop revised (SSOSH-7) and ultra-Brief (SSOSH-3) self-stigma of seeking help scales. Assessment. 2021;28(5):1488–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191120958496.

  56. Freitag S, Stolzenburg S, Schomerus G, Schmidt S. Depressionswissen – Deutsche Übersetzung und Testung der Depression Literacy Scale. Psychiatrische Praxis. 2018;45(8):412–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Schomerus G, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. Causal beliefs of the public and social acceptance of persons with mental illness: a comparative analysis of schizophrenia, depression and alcohol dependence. Psychological Medicine. 2014;44(2):303–14.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Renner B, Hahn A, Schwarzer R. Berlin risk appraisal and health motivation study - Messinstrumente. 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Israel JI. Healthcare use self-efficacy in adults who are homeless. 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Schomerus G, Auer C, Rhode D, Luppa M, Freyberger HJ, Schmidt S. Personal stigma, problem appraisal and perceived need for professional help in currently untreated depressed persons. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012;139(1):94–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Schomerus G, Muehlan H, Auer C, Horsfield P, Tomczyk S, Freitag S, et al. Validity and psychometric properties of the self-identification as having a mental illness scale (SELF-I) among currently untreated persons with mental health problems. Psychiatry Research. 2019;273:303–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Pescosolido BA, Boyer CA. Understanding the context and dynamic social processes of mental health treatment. In: Scheid TL, Brown TN, editors. A handbook for the study of mental health: social contexts, theories, and systems. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2010. p. 420–38.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Löwe B. The patient health questionnaire somatic, anxiety, and depressive symptom scales: a systematic review. General Hospital Psychiatry. 2010;32(4):345–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Broadbent E. Brief Illness Perception Questionnaire (B-IPQ): Scoringsmanual. 2018. Cited 2022 Jun 22. Available from: URL: https://www.spoergeskemaer.dk/diverse-skemaer/.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Beller J, Geyer S, Epping J. Health and study dropout: health aspects differentially predict attrition. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2022;22(1):31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Crossman A. Understanding path analysis. 2020. Available from: URL: https://www.thoughtco.com/path-analysis-3026444.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Lleras C. Path Analysis. In: Kempf-Leonard K, editor. Encyclopedia of social measurement. Elsevier Inc.; 2005. p. 25–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-12-369398-5/00483-7.

  68. Wright S. Path coefficients and path regressions: alternative or complementary concepts? Biometrics. 1960;16(2):189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Rosseel Y. Lavaan : an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Soft. 2012;48(2).

  70. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Version 4.0.3. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020. Available from: URL: https://www.R-project.org/.

  71. Katsikatsou M. The pairwise likelihood method for structural equation modelling with ordinal variables and data with missing values using the R package lavaan. 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2001;16(9):606–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. O’Mahen HA, Flynn HA, Chermack S, Marcus S. Illness perceptions associated with perinatal depression treatment use. Archives of Women's Mental Health. 2009;12(6):447–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Fernandez DK, Deane FP, Vella SA. Effects of online continuum and categorical belief manipulations on schizophrenia stigma, help-seeking, and help-provision. Psychiatry Research. 2022;307:114293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Mojtabai R, Olfson M, Sampson NA, Jin R, Druss B, Wang PS, et al. Barriers to mental health treatment: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41(8):1751–61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Griffiths KM, Crisp DA, Jorm AF, Christensen H. Does stigma predict a belief in dealing with depression alone? Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011;132(3):413–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Simo B, Bamvita J-M, Caron J, Fleury M-J. Predictors of mental health service use among individuals with high psychological distress and mental disorders. Psychiatry Research. 2018;270:1122–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Schomerus G, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC. The stigma of psychiatric treatment and help-seeking intentions for depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2009;259(5):298–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Chekroud AM, Foster D, Zheutlin AB, Gerhard DM, Roy B, Koutsouleris N, et al. Predicting barriers to treatment for depression in a U.S. National Sample: a cross-sectional, proof-of-concept study. Psychiatric Services (Washington, D C). 2018;69(8):927–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Yonemoto N, Kawashima Y. Help-seeking behaviors for mental health problems during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2022;323:85–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Saunders NR, Kurdyak P, Stukel TA, Strauss R, Fu L, Guan J, et al. Utilization of physician-based mental health care services among children and adolescents before and during the COVID-19 pandemic in Ontario, Canada. JAMA Pediatrics. 2022;176(4):e216298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Clement S, Schauman O, Graham T, Maggioni F, Evans-Lacko S, Bezborodovs N, et al. What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on help-seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative studies. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45(1):11–27.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  83. Rüsch N, Müller M, Lay B, Corrigan P, Zahn R, Schönenberger T, et al. Emotional reactions to involuntary psychiatric hospitalization and stigma-related stress among people with mental illness. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2014;264(1):35–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Wilski M, Tasiemski T. Illness perception, treatment beliefs, self-esteem, and self-efficacy as correlates of self-management in multiple sclerosis. Acta Neurol Scand. 2016;133(5):338–45.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  85. Wu H-C, Yang C-Y, Chen Y-C. The role of self-efficacy in the recovery trajectory of mental health consumers. British Journal of Social Work. 2021;51(3):907–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Thibodeau R. Continuum belief, categorical belief, and depression stigma: correlational evidence and outcomes of an online intervention. Stigma Health. 2020;5(4):404–12 Available from: URL: http://libguides.sjfc.edu/citations.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Wang J, Lai D. The relationship between mental health literacy, personal contacts and personal stigma against depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2008;110(1–2):191–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Gaebel W, Kowitz S, Zielasek J. The DGPPN research project on mental healthcare utilization in Germany: inpatient and outpatient treatment of persons with depression by different disciplines. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2012;262(Suppl 2):S51–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Albani C, Blaser G, Geyer M, Schmutzer G, Brähler E. Ambulante Psychotherapie in Deutschland aus Sicht der Patienten. Psychotherapeut. 2010;55(6):503–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. The study was funded by the DFG (German Research Foundation, www.dfg.de: SCHO 1337/4–2 and SCHM 2683/4–2). The funding body is neither involved in the design of the study, the preparation, collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, nor in the writing of this article and deciding to submit it for publication. The study design was approved by external consultants of the funding body.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The manuscript was equally developed and drafted by TM and LJP. TM and LP contributed to the design of the study and the acquisition of the data. TM and LJP analysed and interpreted the data. ST and HoM contributed to the conception of the work. ST, HoM, SiS, and GS revised the draft. GS and SiS manage the project. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Thomas McLaren or Lina-Jolien Peter.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study procedure was developed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and has been approved by the Ethics Commission of the University Medicine Greifswald (BB 061/18) and Leipzig (514/19-lk). Participants included in the study gave electronic informed consent following comprehensive written information about the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

R syntax and fitting commands for the path model analysis for the Seeking Mental Health Care Model. Table S2. Identified path model for seeking mental health care from a professional source.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McLaren, T., Peter, LJ., Tomczyk, S. et al. The Seeking Mental Health Care model: prediction of help-seeking for depressive symptoms by stigma and mental illness representations. BMC Public Health 23, 69 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-14937-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-14937-5

Keywords