Skip to main content

Associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with health-related quality of life and behavioral problems among children attending preschools

Abstract

Background

Both excessive screen time and early screen exposure have been linked to children’s health outcomes, but few studies considered these two exposures simultaneously. The aim of this study was to explore the independent and interactive associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with health-related quality of life (HRQOL) and behavioral problems among Chinese children attending preschools.

Methods

A cross-sectional study of 4985 children aged between 3 and 6 years was conducted in Chengdu, China. Each parent has finished an online questionnaire regarding their children’s screen use, HRQOL, and behavioral problems. Children with screen time over 1 h/day were considered as having excessive screen time. Early screen exposure was defined if the children had started using screen-based media before the age of 2 years. HRQOL was assessed by the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory version 4.0 (PedsQL 4.0), while behavioral problems were confirmed with the 48-item Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-48).

Results

Of the 4985 children (2593 boys and 2392 girls) included, the mean age was 4.6 (SD: 1.0) years. After adjustment for confounders and early screen exposure, excessive screen time was significantly associated with worse HRQOL scores in all dimensions and summary scales, as well as each type of behavioral problems (all p values < 0.05). We also found that compared to children with later initiation of screen exposure, those with screen use before the age of 2 years had significantly lower emotional functioning score (β: − 2.13, 95%CI: − 3.17, − 1.09) and psychosocial health summary score (β: − 0.82, 95%CI: − 1.54, − 0.10) of HRQOL, as well as higher risks of conduct problems, learning problems, psychosomatic problems, impulsive-hyperactive, and hyperactivity index, which were independent of excessive screen use. Furthermore, there were significant interactive effects of excessive screen time and early screen exposure on emotional functioning domain of HRQOL scores and conduct problems.

Conclusion

Excessive screen time and early screen exposure are two independent and interactive factors to children’s HRQOL and behavioral problems. Our findings support current guidelines to limit screen exposure in children. Appropriate screen use may represent an important intervention target to improve children’s HRQOL and reduce their behavioral problems.

Peer Review reports

Background

Screen-based media has become a central part of daily life in the current young generations. According to recommendations by the World Health Organization (WHO), screen time should be avoided for children less than 2 years old, and no more than 1 hour per day for children aged between 2 and 5 years [1]. In China, the 2021 Physical Activity Guidelines even have stricter criteria [2]. It recommended no screen-based media for children aged between 0 and 2 years, less than 1 hour of screen time per day for children aged between 3 and 5 years, and less than 2 hours of screen time per day for those aged between 6 and 17 years [2]. Despite above-mentioned guidelines, excessive screen time is still prevalent among preschool children in China [3, 4]. For example, a cross-sectional study of Chinese children aged between 3 and 6 years has found that around 55% of the children spent > 1 hour per day on screen-based media [3]. Another cross-sectional study with 29,461 preschool children in China has also shown that approximately 62% of the children had daily screen time ≥ 1 hour, and 60% of them had started using screen-based media before the age of 2 years [4].

A growing body of evidence suggests that excessive screen time and early screen exposure have detrimental impacts on children [5,6,7,8,9]. For example, a multi-country cohort study has revealed a link between excessive screen time and an increased risk of overweight or obesity in children aged between 2 and 11 years [5]. Likewise, other studies have demonstrated significant associations of excessive screen time with shorter sleep duration, poor sleep quality, and poor cognitive development in children [6,7,8]. Furthermore, early screen exposure has also been found to be a risk factor for language delay in children [9].

In addition to above-mentioned health issues, excessive screen time and early screen exposure could also affect children’s behaviors and health-related quality of life (HRQOL) [10,11,12,13]. A cross-sectional study of Chinese children aged between 3 and 6 years has found that daily screen time over 2 hours was associated with increased risks of behavioral problems [10]. Another longitudinal study in Australia has further shown that adolescents with excessive screen time had significantly lower scores in multiple domains of HRQOL compared to those with moderate screen time exposure [11]. In regard to the detrimental impact of early screen exposure on children, a cross-sectional study has reported significant associations between early screen exposure and emotional/behavioral difficulties among Singapore children aged between 2 and 5 years [12]. A cross-sectional study of Chinese preschool children has also shown that the risk of autistic-like behaviors was 1.90 times higher in children with screen exposure before 3 years old, compared to their counterparts who had never been exposed to screen-based media [13].

Although HRQOL is an important indicator that reflects a person’s physical, psychological, and social well-being [14], its associations with excessive screen time and early screen exposure were rarely discussed in children attending preschools. Furthermore, the independent and interactive impact of excessive screen time and early screen exposure on children’s HRQOL and behavioral problems also warrants further investigations. Therefore, in this cross-sectional study, we aimed to first explore the independent associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with HRQOL scores and behavioral problems among children attending preschools in Chengdu, China. Then, its interactive impact on children’s outcomes was also evaluated.

Methods

Study design and population

This cross-sectional study was conducted from May to July 2021 in Chengdu, a mega-city located in western China. It is comprised of 12 urban districts, 5 county-level cities, and 3 counties. To select representative children attending preschools, a multistage sampling strategy was used. In the first stage, 4 urban districts, 2 county-level cities, and 1 county were randomly selected. In the second stage, two preschools were further randomly chosen from each selected area. A total of 14 preschools were finally included. All the children and their parents in the randomly selected preschools were invited to join this study. During the recruitment period, caregivers of 5102 children have finished an online questionnaire (response rate: 86.5%). We have further excluded 23 children with questionnaires completed by other caregivers, rather than their parents, and 94 children with missing data on age or with mis-report of age, as the eligible age of a child for enrollment is between 3 and 6 years in these 14 participating preschools. Finally, a total of 4985 children aged between 3 and 6 years were included in the current analysis (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of participants selection

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University (Reference number: 2021[116]). Each parent has signed an informed consent before attending this study.

Excessive screen time and early screen exposure

Children’s daily screen time was reported by their parents using the following questions: “On average, how many hours per day does your child usually spend on the computer, tablet, mobile phone, TV, or e-reader on a typical weekday and weekend day, respectively?”. The average daily screen time was calculated as (5 × daily screen time on weekdays + 2 × daily screen time on weekends) /7. We further categorized daily screen time into the moderate screen time group (≤ 1 h/day) and the excessive screen time group (> 1 h/day), according to previous guidelines [1] and studies [3, 15].

Furthermore, parents reported the specific age when their child first had electronic screen exposure by the question: “How old is your child when he/she first had access to electronic devices, including computer, tablet, mobile phone, TV, or e-reader?”. Early screen exposure was confirmed if the children had started using screen-based media before the age of 2 years according to the WHO guideline; otherwise, he or she was considered as having later initiation of screen exposure [1].

Measurement of children’s HRQOL and behavioral problems

Children’s HRQOL was measured using the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory version 4.0 (PedsQL 4.0) [16], which has been demonstrated to be reliable and valid in Chinese children [17, 18]. The instrument has 21 items for children aged between 2 and 4 years, and 23 items for children aged between 5 and 7 years. Both versions have four dimensions, including emotional functioning (5 items), physical functioning (8 items), social functioning (5 items), and school functioning (3 items for 2–4 years old children, and 5 items for 5–7 years old children). To calculate HRQOL scores, each item was first rated based on a five-point Likert scale (0 = never a problem, 1 = almost never a problem, 2 = sometimes a problem, 3 = often a problem, and 4 = almost always a problem). Then, the items were reverse-scored and linearly transformed to a 0–100 scale, so that higher scores indicated better HRQOL. A psychosocial health summary score was calculated as the average scores of the emotional functioning, social functioning, and school functioning dimensions. A total scale score of the overall HRQOL was further calculated as the average scores of all items in the four dimensions.

Children’s behavioral problems were assessed by the 48-item Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-48) [19], a validated questionnaire to evaluate behavioral problems in Chinese children aged between 3 and 18 years [20]. It measures six different types of behavioral problems, including conduct problems (12 items), learning problems (4 items), psychosomatic problems (5 items), impulsive-hyperactive (4 items), anxiety (4 items), and hyperactivity index (10 items). Each item was scored based on a four-point Likert scale, i.e., 0 = never, 1 = occasionally, 2 = often, and 3 = very often. The final score of each type of behavioral problem was calculated by averaging the scores of the corresponding items. For each dimension, the abnormality was defined as a score ≥ 90th percentile based on the norm distribution in China [20].

Covariates

Children

Children’s information on age, gender, status of single child, and primary caregiver were reported by their parents. The status of single child was classified as having only one child in the family (yes) or having two or more children in the family (no). The primary caregivers of children were classified into three groups as mothers, fathers, and others (grandparents or other people).

Parent

Parents self-reported their age, marital status, education level, monthly per-capita income, family harmony level, parenting styles, and negative emotional states. Marital status was categorized as currently married or unmarried. The latter included single, divorced, separated, and widowed. Education level was grouped into four categories: (1) junior high school or below, (2) senior high school, (3) bachelor’s degree, and (4) master’s degree or above. Monthly per-capita income was divided into five categories as (1) ≤ 5000 RMB; (2) 5001–10,000 RMB; (3) 10001–15,000 RMB; (4) > 15,000 RMB; and (5) uncertain, where 1 US$ ≈ 7.11 RMB (RMB stands for Renminbi, the official name of China’s currency). The level of family harmony was assessed with the Family Harmony Scale-5 (FHS-5), a validated questionnaire with 5 items including effective communication, conflict resolution, forbearance, family identity, and quality time with family [21]. Each item was rated from strongly disagree (score 1) to strongly agree (score 5). A total score (range: 5–25) was computed by summing the 5 items, with a higher total score indicating better family harmony. Parenting styles were assessed with the validated Chinese version of Short Egna Minnen Beträffande Uppfostran Parent Form (s-EMBU-C), which consists of 21 items and three subscales, including rejection (6 items), emotional warmth (7 items), and overprotection/control (8 items) [22]. All items were rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = never, 2 = seldom, 3 = often, and 4 = most of the time). The score of each type of parenting style was calculated by summing the items in the corresponding dimension. Parental negative emotional states, including depression, anxiety, and stress, were assessed by self-reported Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21 (DASS-21) [23], a valid and reliable questionnaire in Chinese population [24]. It is consisted of 21 items that were rated based on a 4-point Likert scale (from 0 = “did not apply to me at all” to 3 = “applied to me very much, or most of the time”). The score of each negative emotion was calculated by adding up the seven items in the corresponding dimension, and higher scores indicated more negative emotions. Negative emotional states of depression, anxiety, and stress were further defined using cut-off points of 9, 7, and 14, respectively.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive data were presented as mean (standard deviation, SD) or frequencies (%), where appropriate. Characteristics between excessive and moderate screen time groups, or between early screen exposure and later initiation of screen exposure groups, were compared by independent student t-test for continuous data and Chi-square test for categorical data. If Chi-square tests were significant for variables with more than 2 categories, post-hoc tests were further applied for multiple comparisons.

To examine the impact of excessive screen time and early screen exposure on children’s HRQOL scores and behavioral problems, linear and logistic regression models were established, respectively. For daily screen time, moderate screen time (≤ 1 h/day) was treated as the reference group, while for age of first screen use, later initiation of screen exposure (≥2 years) was used as the reference group. The associations were first assessed in crude models. The adjusted models were further controlled for children’s age, gender, status of single child, primary caregiver, parental age, marital status, education level, monthly per-capita income, family harmony level, parenting styles, and parental negative emotional states. To confirm the independent impact of excessive screen time and early screen exposure on the outcomes, these two factors were also mutually adjusted in the multivariate models. To further explore the interactive effect of excessive screen time and early screen exposure on children’s HRQOL scores and behavioral problems, we performed stratified analyses by different groups of the two exposures (i.e., 1. screen time ≤ 1 h/d and age at first screen use ≥2 years, 2. screen time ≤ 1 h/d and age at first screen use < 2 years, 3. screen time > 1 h/d and age at first screen use ≥2 years, and 4. screen time > 1 h/d and age at first screen use < 2 years) and tests for interaction. Regression models included the same confounders listed in the adjusted model, except for the two exposures.

Since parents with negative emotional states might underreport the HRQOL scores and overreport the behavioral problems of their children [25, 26], we reanalyzed the independent and interactive associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with the outcomes by excluding parents with negative emotional states of depression, anxiety, and stress in the sensitivity analysis.

For all linear regression models, assumptions of linear regression models, including linearity, normality, homoscedasticity, and absence of multicollinearity were checked. Beta coefficient (β) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CI) were reported by linear regression analyses, while odds ratio (OR) with 95%CI were calculated by logistic regression analyses.

Data analyses were performed using STATA/SE version 15.1. A two-sided p value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Of the 4985 children included, 2593 (52.0%) were boys and the mean age was 4.6 (SD: 1.0) years. The average time of daily exposure to screen-based media was 0.98 (SD: 0.87) hours. Approximately 34.8% (n = 1734) of the children attending preschools had excessive screen time over 1 h/day, and 11.9% (n = 592) had started using screen-based media before the age of 2 years. Compared to children with moderate screen time, those with excessive screen time were more likely to have primary caregivers other than parents. Parents of children with excessive screen time were younger, less educated, had lower monthly per-capita income, and were more likely to adopt rejecting and overprotective parenting styles and have negative emotional states of depression, anxiety, and stress. In addition, children with early screen exposure were younger, were more likely to live in a family with low levels of family harmony, and their parents were more likely to have negative emotional states of anxiety and stress, compared to those who started using screen-based media after 2 years old (Table 1).

Table 1 Comparison of characteristics by children’s status of excessive screen time and early screen exposure

Table 2 presents the comparison of children’s HRQOL scores and behavioral problems by excessive screen time and early screen exposure groups, respectively. We found that compared to children with moderate screen time, those with excessive screen time had lower scores in all domains and summary scales of HRQOL (emotional functioning: 90.1 vs 92.2; social functioning: 75.5 vs 77.6; school functioning: 68.8 vs 71.4; physical functioning: 80.3 vs 83.4; psychosocial health summary score: 78.1 vs 80.4; and total scale score: 78.7 vs 81.2; all p values < 0.001), as well as more behavioral problems (conduct problems: 14.7% vs 9.1%; learning problems: 24.9% vs 15.8%; psychosomatic problems: 26.1% vs 20.4%; impulsive-hyperactive: 21.6% vs 12.4%; anxiety: 15.7% vs 12.0%; and hyperactivity index: 16.3% vs 9.7%, all p values < 0.001). Furthermore, compared to children with later initiation of screen exposure, those who started using screen-based media before the age of 2 years had significantly lower emotional functioning score (88.8 vs 91.8, p <  0.001) and psychosocial health summary score (78.6 vs 79.7, p = 0.005). The prevalence of behavioral problems was also significantly higher in children with early screen exposure, when compared to their counterparts who started using screen after 2 years old (conduct problems: 16.2% vs 10.4%, p <  0.001; learning problems: 24.2% vs 18.2%, p = 0.001; psychosomatic problems: 28.7% vs 21.5%, p < 0.001; impulsive-hyperactive: 19.9% vs 15.0%, p = 0.002; and hyperactivity index: 17.2% vs 11.3%, p < 0.001).

Table 2 Comparison of children’s HRQOL scores and behavioral problems by their status of excessive screen time and early screen exposure

The associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children’s HRQOL scores are shown in Table 3. After adjusting for confounders, excessive screen time over 1 h/day was associated with poorer emotional functioning (β = − 1.30, 95%CI: − 2.02, − 0.59), social functioning (β = − 0.97, 95%CI: − 1.63, − 0.32), school functioning (β = − 1.27, 95%CI: − 2.12, − 0.43), and physical functioning (β = − 1.38, 95%CI: − 2.55, − 0.22). The psychosocial health summary score and total scale score were also significantly lower in children with excessive screen time, compared to their counterparts with moderate screen time (β = − 1.18, 95%CI: − 1.68, − 0.68 for psychosocial health summary score; β = − 1.23, 95%CI: − 1.76, − 0.71 for total scale score). In addition, early screen exposure before the age of 2 years was significantly associated with lower emotional functioning score (β = − 2.13, 95%CI: − 3.17, − 1.09), and psychosocial health summary score (β = − 0.82, 95%CI: − 1.54, − 0.10).

Table 3 Associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children’s HRQOL scores

We further evaluated the associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children’s behavioral problems (Table 4). With inclusion of confounders into the models, excessive screen time was associated with increased risk of conduct problems (OR = 1.43, 95%CI: 1.17, 1.75), learning problems (OR = 1.49, 95%CI: 1.28, 1.74), psychosomatic problems (OR = 1.23, 95%CI: 1.06, 1.42), impulsive-hyperactive (OR = 1.61, 95%CI: 1.36, 1.91), anxiety (OR = 1.23, 95%CI: 1.02, 1.47), and hyperactivity index (OR = 1.49, 95%CI: 1.23, 1.80). Furthermore, compared to children who started using screen after 2 years old, those with early screen exposure were more likely to have conduct problems (OR = 1.59, 95%CI: 1.22, 2.07), learning problems (OR = 1.35, 95%CI: 1.08, 1.68), psychosomatic problems (OR = 1.41, 95%CI: 1.15, 1.72), impulsive-hyperactive (OR = 1.35, 95%CI: 1.06, 1.72), and hyperactivity index (OR = 1.57, 95%CI: 1.21, 2.02) in the multivariate models.

Table 4 Associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children’s behavioral problems

Table 5 presents the interactive effect of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with HRQOL scores and behavioral problems in children. We only observed statistically significant interactive effect of excessive screen time and early screen exposure on emotional functioning domain of HRQOL and conduct problems. In sensitivity analyses, consistent results were also found after excluding parents with negative emotional states (Supplementary Tables 13).

Table 5 The interactive effect of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children’s HRQOL scores and behavioral problems

Discussion

In this cross-sectional study of 4985 children attending preschools, we found that excessive screen time over 1 h per day and screen exposure before the age of 2 years were two independent risk factors of lower HRQOL scores and behavioral problems. Furthermore, we observed significant interaction effect between these two factors on the emotional functioning domain of HRQOL and conduct problems.

Our findings are consistent with previous studies that have shown a significant association between excessive screen time and lower scores of HRQOL [27,28,29]. For example, a cross-sectional study in Hong Kong with 7555 children aged between 6 and 17 years has found that excessive screen time was negatively associated with PedsQL physical summary score, psychosocial summary score, and total scale score in both boys and girls [27]. Another cross-sectional study of children aged between 2 and 12 years in Australia has also demonstrated significant associations between excessive screen time and reduced HRQOL scores [28]. Furthermore, in line with several previous studies, we found that excessive screen time was also associated with an increased risk of behavioral problems [30, 31]. A cross-sectional analysis using data from All Our Families research has revealed a positive dose-response association between daily screen time and risk of behavioral problems among children aged 3 years [30]. Similar results have also been found in a birth cohort conducted in China, which has identified significant associations of excessive screen time in early childhood with later emotional and behavioral problems in children aged 4 years [31].

Several possible mechanisms may explain the significant associations of excessive screen time with lower HRQOL scores and increased risk of behavioral problems in children. First, increased screen time was associated with lower fractional anisotropy and higher radial diffusivity in brain white matter tracts that support executive functions, reading skills, and language development, which could further shape the well-being and behaviors in preschool children [32]. Second, according to the time displacement hypothesis, screen viewing may displace the time that could otherwise have been used for intellectually demanding activities or health-enhancing physical activities [33,34,35]. Lack of such activities might consequently lead to impaired HRQOL and more behavioral problems in children [36,37,38,39]. Third, the social withdrawal hypothesis has suggested that excessive screen time could hinder children’s social interaction with their peers and parents, which may subsequently jeopardize children's psychosocial well-being and prosocial behaviors [40, 41]. Fourth, children with excessive screen time usually have attention problems [42], which have been demonstrated to be associated with lower scores of HRQOL and more behavioral problems [43, 44]. Therefore, the significant associations we observed between excessive screen time and lower HRQOL scores and more behavioral problems were plausible. Nevertheless, research is still needed to further confirm the potential mechanisms.

In terms of the associations between early initiation of screen use and adverse outcomes, a case-control study with children aged between 15 to 48 months has found that compared to children who started watching television after 1 year old, those with early screen exposure were more likely to have problems in language development [9]. A longitudinal study has further demonstrated that earlier age of onset of screen exposure was associated with decreased cognitive function in preschoolers [45]. In line with these findings, our study also showed that early initiation of screen use was associated with reduced HRQOL scores and increased risk of behavioral problems among children attending preschools. One possible explanation for this association was related to the possible impairment of brain development during childhood caused by early initiation of screen use. It is well-recognized that the period from birth to 2 years of age is important for cognition establishment and behaviors that last a lifetime [46]. During this period, cortical grey matter undergoes robust development, along with rapid myelination and maturation of the microstructure of existing white-matter tracts [47,48,49,50,51]. When children exposed to screen-based media before 2 years, their normal development of brain network might be interrupted with neurochemical and anatomical brain changes, and eventually lead to poor HRQOL and behavioral problems. However, future studies are warranted to investigate the mechanisms.

The present study further extends the findings of existing literatures by showing the independent role of excessive screen time and early screen exposure on children’s HRQOL and behavioral problems. We additionally found significant interactive effect of the two exposures on children’s emotional functioning domain of HRQOL and conduct problems, but not for other domains. One possible explanation might due to the small sample size of children with both excessive screen time and early screen exposure (N = 222), resulting in low statistical power. Another possible explanation might be related to the cross-sectional nature of this study. The daily time spent on screen might have changed since they first had access to screen-based media, which cannot be captured by current study design. Additional research with repeated data from follow-up studies is needed to further investigate the interactive role of excessive screen time and early exposure on health outcomes.

Although our study has included a large sample size and adjusted for several potential confounders, there are still some limitations that deserve discussion. First, the cross-sectional design precludes us from confirming the causal relationship. Also, we were unable to evaluate the moderating role of age in the associations between excessive screen use and early screen exposure with HRQOL scores and behavioral problems, as we cannot ensure that whether children have changed their screen-usage patterns. Further well-designed longitudinal studies with accumulated data are needed to clarify the temporal associations of screen use habits with HRQOL and behaviors in children attending preschools. Second, while self-reporting of screen use, HRQOL, and behaviors are more accurate, it should be noted that these data were measured by proxy because of children’s young age, which is a common limitation faced by child health studies [52, 53]. Third, studies have demonstrated that high-quality programming and parents’ co-viewing with children were beneficial to children’s social skills, sleep patterns, and prosocial behaviors [54,55,56]. Nevertheless, our study did not collect these data. Further research is needed to explore the associations of co-viewing high-quality programming with children’s HRQOL and behavioral problems. Fourth, inconsistent findings have been reported between the types of devices used and children’s health outcomes [57, 58]. We were unable to illustrate on this issue due to data unavailability. In addition, although previous studies have suggested that background exposure of screen use (i.e., with screen on, but no one is watching) was associated with adverse health outcomes in children, such as decreased language skills and executive function [56, 59], we did not take it into consideration in the current study. The associations of background exposure and device-specific screen use with children’s outcomes should be further elucidated in future studies. Last, although we have adjusted for several confounders in the analysis, we could not rule out the possibility of residual confounding effects that may distort the associations, such as children’s temperament, parental physical or mental illness, and parents’ attitude towards children’s screen use [52, 53, 60,61,62].

Conclusion

In conclusion, excessive screen time and early screen exposure were found to have independent and interactive effects on poor HRQOL and behavioral problems in children attending preschools after adjusting for confounding factors. Our findings support current guidelines regarding appropriate screen use in children. The study also implicates that avoiding screen use during infancy and reducing daily screen time may preserve HRQOL and prevent behavioral problems in children attending preschools. However, future longitudinal studies are needed to confirm the conclusions.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available for ethical and privacy reasons but are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for Children under 5 Years of Age. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/325147/WHO-NMH-PND-2019.4-eng.pdf.

  2. Composing and editorial Board of Physical Activity Guidelines for Chinese. Physical activity guidelines for Chinese (2021). Chin J Epidemiol. 2022;43:5–6.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Xie G, Deng Q, Cao J, Chang Q. Digital screen time and its effect on preschoolers’ behavior in China: results from a cross-sectional study. Ital J Pediatr. 2020;46(1):9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Chen J-Y, Strodl E, Wu C-a, Huang L-H, Yin X-N, Wen G-M, et al. Screen time and autistic-like behaviors among preschool children in China. Psychol Health Med. 2020;26(5):607–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Guzmán V, Lissner L, Arvidsson L, Hebestreit A, Solea A, Lauria F, et al. Associations of sleep duration and screen time with incidence of overweight in European children: the IDEFICS/I.Family cohort. Obes Facts. 2022;15(1):55–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Helm AF, Spencer RMC. Television use and its effects on sleep in early childhood. Sleep Health. 2019;5(3):241–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Chen B, van Dam RM, Tan CS, Chua HL, Wong PG, Bernard JY, et al. Screen viewing behavior and sleep duration among children aged 2 and below. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Vanderloo LM, Janus M, Omand JA, Keown-Stoneman CDG, Borkhoff CM, Duku E, et al. Children’s screen use and school readiness at 4-6 years: prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Chonchaiya W, Pruksananonda C. Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97(7):977–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Wu X, Tao S, Rutayisire E, Chen Y, Huang K, Tao F. The relationship between screen time, nighttime sleep duration, and behavioural problems in preschool children in China. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(5):541–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Gopinath B, Hardy LL, Baur LA, Burlutsky G, Mitchell P. Physical activity and sedentary behaviors and health-related quality of life in adolescents. Pediatrics. 2012;130(1):e167–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Lin JY, Magiati I, Chiong SHR, Singhal S, Riard N, Ng IHX, et al. The relationship among screen use, sleep, and emotional/behavioral difficulties in preschool children with neurodevelopmental disorders. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(7):519–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Chen JY, Strodl E, Huang LH, Chen YJ, Yang GY, Chen WQ. Early electronic screen exposure and autistic-like behaviors among preschoolers: the mediating role of caregiver-child interaction, sleep duration and outdoor activities. Children (Basel). 2020;7(11):200.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Matza LS, Swensen AR, Flood EM, Secnik K, Leidy NK. Assessment of health-related quality of life in children: a review of conceptual, methodological, and regulatory issues. Value Health. 2004;7(1):79–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Zhu R, Fang H, Chen M, Hu X, Cao Y, Yang F, et al. Screen time and sleep disorder in preschool children: identifying the safe threshold in a digital world. Public Health. 2020;186:204–10.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS. PedsQL 4.0: reliability and validity of the pediatric quality of life inventory version 4.0 generic core scales in healthy and patient populations. Med Care. 2001;39(8):800–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Duan X, Zhang S, Xiao N. Reliability and validity of the PedsQL™ generic Core scales 4.0 for Chinese children with epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2012;23(4):431–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Luo S, Lin L, Chen W, Li C, Ren Y, Zhang M, et al. Association between maternal intimate partner violence and health-related quality of life in their preschool children: the mediating role of maternal parenting styles. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:996971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Goyette CH, Conners CK, Ulrich RF. Normative data on revised Conners parent and teacher rating scales. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1978;6(2):221–36.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Fan Juan DY, Liwei W. The norm and reliability of the conners parent symptom questionnaire in Chinese urban children. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry. 2005;17(06):321–3.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Kavikondala S, Stewart SM, Ni MY, Chan BH, Lee PH, Li KK, et al. Structure and validity of family harmony scale: an instrument for measuring harmony. Psychol Assess. 2016;28(3):307–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Jiang L, Zhengrong L, Bijing J, Yan X. Preliminary revision of the Chinese version of Short-Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostran. Psychol Dev Educ. 2010;26(01):94–9.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the Beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(3):335–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Wang K, Shi HS, Geng FL, Zou LQ, Tan SP, Wang Y, et al. Cross-cultural validation of the depression anxiety stress Scale-21 in China. Psychol Assess. 2016;28(5):e88–e100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Wong RSM, Yu EYT, Guo VYW, Wan EYF, Chin WY, Wong CKH, et al. A prospective cohort study to investigate parental stress and child health in low-income Chinese families: protocol paper. BMJ Open. 2018;8(2):e018792.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Richters J, Pellegrini D. Depressed mothers’ judgments about their children: an examination of the depression-distortion hypothesis. Child Dev. 1989;60(5):1068–75.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Wong CKH, Wong RS, Cheung JPY, Tung KTS, Yam JCS, Rich M, et al. Impact of sleep duration, physical activity, and screen time on health-related quality of life in children and adolescents. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2021;19(1):145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Tooth LR, Moss KM, Mishra GD. Screen time and child behaviour and health-related quality of life: effect of family context. Prev Med. 2021;153:106795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Wu XY, Han LH, Zhang JH, Luo S, Hu JW, Sun K. The influence of physical activity, sedentary behavior on health-related quality of life among the general population of children and adolescents: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2017;12(11):e0187668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. McArthur BA, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen time and developmental and behavioral outcomes for preschool children. Pediatr Res. 2021;91(6):1616–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Liu W, Wu X, Huang K, Yan S, Ma L, Cao H, et al. Early childhood screen time as a predictor of emotional and behavioral problems in children at 4 years: a birth cohort study in China. Environ Health Prev. 2021;26:3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Hutton JS, Dudley J, Horowitz-Kraus T, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Associations between screen-based media use and brain white matter integrity in preschool-aged children. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):e193869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Lizandra J, Devís-Devís J, Valencia-Peris A, Tomás JM, Peiró-Velert C. Screen time and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity changes and displacement in adolescence: a prospective cohort study. Eur J Sport Sci. 2019;19(5):686–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Neuman SB. The displacement effect: assessing the relation between television viewing and reading performance. Read Res Q. 1988;23:414–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Przybylski AK. Digital screen time and pediatric sleep: evidence from a preregistered cohort study. J Pediatr. 2019;205:218–223.e211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Khan A, Lee E-Y, Rosenbaum S, Khan SR, Tremblay MS. Dose-dependent and joint associations between screen time, physical activity, and mental wellbeing in adolescents: an international observational study. Lancet Child Adolesc. 2021;5(10):729–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Clarke AT. Coping with interpersonal stress and psychosocial health among children and adolescents: a Meta-analysis. J Youth Adolesc. 2006;35(1):10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Sundell AL, Angelhoff C. Sleep and its relation to health-related quality of life in 3-10-year-old children. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):1043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Sivertsen B, Harvey AG, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Torgersen L, Ystrom E, Hysing M. Later emotional and behavioral problems associated with sleep problems in toddlers: a longitudinal study. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(6):575–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Park JH, Choi JM. Smartphone overdependence and quality of life in college students: focusing on the mediating effect of social withdrawal. Front Public Health. 2022;10:997682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Rubin KH, Coplan RJ, Bowker JC. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:141–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Santos RMS, Mendes CG, Marques Miranda D, Romano-Silva MA. The association between screen time and attention in children: a systematic review. Dev Neuropsychol. 2022;47(4):175–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Danckaerts M, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Döpfner M, Hollis C, et al. The quality of life of children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19(2):83–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Berenguer C, Roselló B, Colomer C, Baixauli I, Miranda A. Children with autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Relationships between symptoms and executive function, theory of mind, and behavioral problems. Res Dev Disabil. 2018;83:260–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Supanitayanon S, Trairatvorakul P, Chonchaiya W. Screen media exposure in the first 2 years of life and preschool cognitive development: a longitudinal study. Pediatr Res. 2020;88(6):894–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Gilmore JH, Knickmeyer RC, Gao W. Imaging structural and functional brain development in early childhood. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(3):123–37.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Knickmeyer RC, Gouttard S, Kang C, Evans D, Wilber K, Smith JK, et al. A structural MRI study of human brain development from birth to 2 years. J Neurosci. 2008;28(47):12176–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Gilmore JH, Shi F, Woolson SL, Knickmeyer RC, Short SJ, Lin W, et al. Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(11):2478–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Remer J, Croteau-Chonka E, Dean DC 3rd, D'Arpino S, Dirks H, Whiley D, et al. Quantifying cortical development in typically developing toddlers and young children, 1-6 years of age. Neuroimage. 2017;153:246–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Dubois J, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kulikova S, Poupon C, Hüppi PS, Hertz-Pannier L. The early development of brain white matter: a review of imaging studies in fetuses, newborns and infants. Neuroscience. 2014;276:48–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Geng X, Gouttard S, Sharma A, Gu H, Styner M, Lin W, et al. Quantitative tract-based white matter development from birth to age 2 years. Neuroimage. 2012;61(3):542–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Guo VY, Yu EYT, Wong RSM, Ip P, Tiwari AFY, Wong CKH, et al. Maternal mental quality of life mediates the associations between intimate partner abuse against mothers and their children's behaviours and quality of life in low-income Chinese families. Qual Life Res. 2017;26(12):3297–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Guo VYW, Wong CKH, Wong RSM, Yu EYT, Ip P, Lam CLK. Spillover effects of maternal chronic disease on Children's quality of life and behaviors among low-income families. Patient. 2018;11(6):625–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Gentile DA, Reimer RA, Nathanson AI, Walsh DA, Eisenmann JC. Protective effects of parental monitoring of children's media use: a prospective study. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(5):479–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Kostyrka-Allchorne K, Cooper NR, Simpson A. The relationship between television exposure and children’s cognition and behaviour: a systematic review. Dev Rev. 2017;44:19–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Linebarger DL, Barr R, Lapierre MA, Piotrowski JT. Associations between parenting, media use, cumulative risk, and children's executive functioning. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014;35(6):367–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Swing EL, Gentile DA, Anderson CA, Walsh DA. Television and video game exposure and the development of attention problems. Pediatrics. 2010;126(2):214–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Parkes A, Sweeting H, Wight D, Henderson M. Do television and electronic games predict children's psychosocial adjustment? Longitudinal research using the UK millennium cohort study. Arch Dis Child. 2013;98(5):341–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. McArthur BA, Hentges R, Christakis DA, McDonald S, Tough S, Madigan S. Cumulative social risk and child screen use: the role of child temperament. J Pediatr Psychol. 2021;47(2):171–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Lee HE, Kim JY, Kim C. The influence of parent media use, parent attitude on media, and parenting style on children’s media use. 2022;9(1):37.

  62. Guo Y, Zhang Y-Q, Wu C-A, Yin X-N, Zhang J-Y, Wu J-B, et al. Bidirectional associations between parenting styles and conduct problems in Chinese preschool children: the Shenzhen Longhua child cohort study. Psychol Health Med. 2021;27(9):2007–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the participating preschools and their parents for the time they dedicated to taking part in the research.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant number 82204069), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, Sun Yat-sen University (Grant number 22qntd4201), the start-up fund from the Sun Yat-sen University (Grant number 51000–18841211), and the Chengdu Science and Technology Bureau (Grant number 2019-YF09–00240-SN).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization: VYG; Methodology: HX and VYG; Data curation: LL, CL, and VYG; Formal analysis and investigation: HX and LL; Writing - original draft preparation: HX and VYG; Writing - review and editing: all authors; Funding acquisition: CL and VYG. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Chunrong Li or Vivian Yawei Guo.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The research was conducted by the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments, and obtained ethical approval from the Ethics Committee of School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University (Ethical approval number: 2021[116]). After the intention and procedure of this study had been fully explained in detail, each parent has signed an informed consent before attending this study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplementary Table 1.

Associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children's HRQOL scores, excluding parents with negative emotional states. Supplementary Table 2. Associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children's behavioral problems, excluding parents with negative emotional states. Supplementary Table 3. The interactive effect of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with children's HRQOL scores and behavioral problems, excluding parents with negative emotional states.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Xiang, H., Lin, L., Chen, W. et al. Associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with health-related quality of life and behavioral problems among children attending preschools. BMC Public Health 22, 2440 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-14910-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-14910-2

Keywords