Skip to main content

Modeling the effects of physical activity, education, health, and subjective wealth on happiness based on Indonesian national survey data

Abstract

Background

Studies on physical activity’s psychological benefits are generally fewer than those on its physiological benefits, and these limited studies have mostly investigated its impact on cognitive functions. Studies exclusively investigating physical activity’s effects on happiness are rare. This study aims to investigate the effect of physical activity on psychological functions, especially on happiness.

Methods

Analysis was based on a large field of nationally representative Indonesian adult data. Data were compiled based on face-to-face interviews with 12,051 adults. Participants provided measures of physical activity, subjective health, and happiness, and responses were recorded with computer-assisted personal interviewing (CAPI) software. Demographic data, including gender, subjective wealth, education, and age, were also included in the analysis. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted to determine the relationship between physical activity, health, subjective wealth, and happiness.

Results

The tested model of the association between physical activity, health, subjective wealth, and happiness indicated a good fit, based on χ2 (1, n = 12,051) = 48.733, p = .001, RMSEA = .063, and CFI = .97. Path analysis results showed that health conditions mediated the effects of physical activity on happiness. The result also showed positive effects of education level and subjective wealth on happiness.

Conclusion

This study provides evidence that engagement in physical activity has a positive impact on happiness. Indonesian adults should engage in more active lifestyles since more than one-third of Indonesians did not get enough physical activity.

Peer Review reports

Background

Studies on the physiological benefits of physical activity outnumber those of its psychological benefits [1]. Various studies show that regular physical activity has multiple physiological benefits. The intensity of physical activity contributes to lipoprotein profile, carbohydrate metabolism, lower blood pressure, and weight loss [2]. Physical activity also offers protection against cancers of the colon, breast endometrium, pancreas, prostate, lung, and ovary [3,4,5,6]. Other studies reported that physical activity helped control type II [7,8,9] and type I diabetes [10,11,12,13]. Blair [14] even concluded that low cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) was the highest cause of death than other factors—i.e., high blood pressure, smoking, high cholesterol, diabetes, and obesity.

Studies that reported the psychological effects of physical activity were mostly concentrated on cognitive functions. The effects of regular activity were observed across a variety of cognitive processes. However, the highest were found in the executive control process—i.e., planning, scheduling, working memory, interference control, and task coordination [15,16,17].

In aging adults, regular physical activity maintains cognitive condition and was associated with the decreasing risk of poor cognition and early cognitive decline [18,19,20]. Weuve and colleagues [19] studied the effects of long-term regular physical activity, mainly walking, on cognitive functions of women aged 70–81 (N = 18,766). Results showed that higher levels of regular physical activity were associated with better cognitive performance. Higher cardiorespiratory fitness was also associated with less memory decline across lifespan [19, 21].

Physical activity was also proven to reduce depression [22,23,24,25], reduce anxiety [23, 26, 27], and protect against stress [28]. A randomized experiment by Babyak and colleagues [22] showed that exercise was as effective as pharmacotherapy for depression. Furthermore, after 10 months, the exercise group had significantly lower relapse rates than the medication group [22].

The present study investigates the effect of physical activity on psychological functions, especially on subjective wellbeing or happiness. The terms subjective wellbeing and happiness are used interchangeably in the literature [29]. This study uses the term happiness because wellbeing usually consists of objective variables such as income and health [29,30,31]. Happiness in this study refers to the overall evaluation of life or life-satisfaction [32], rather than positive emotional states like contentment, joy, and excitement.

Studies examining the effects of physical activity exclusively on happiness are rare [33,34,35,36]. The World Database of Happiness [37], an online register of research on happiness, listed few studies that associated physical activity with happiness. A study on adolescents’ samples concluded that regular exercise was associated with psychological wellbeing and a lower propensity for eating disorder behaviors [38]. Another study on older adults found that physical exercise programs reduced pain intensity [39]. Participants also reported significant improvement in psychological wellbeing—feeling happier, less lonely, more life satisfaction, and less depression [39]. A study found that the intensity of physical activity was positively related to daily positive affect, which is a known determinant of happiness [40].

Why does physical activity affect happiness? Argyle [29] suggested it is partly due to social interaction with others. Physical activity can increase the opportunity for social relationships, which are noted to have a powerful effect on happiness [29]. Nevertheless, physical activity’s effect on happiness was also found in solitary exercise. Literature has long noted the “runner’s high” phenomena, which is suspected to be caused by the release of endorphins after strenuous physical activity [41]. A study measured the activity of the brain before and after strenuous physical activity. The results showed that a release of endogenous opioids occurred in the frontolimbic brain regions after running, and the level of euphoria was significantly increased [42].

The present study also addressed the effects of other variables related to happiness, such as economic status, education, and health.

Subjective wealth and happiness

The association between income and happiness has been the most enduring debate in the literature on subjective wellbeing [43,44,45]. Studies showed positive and negative effects of wealth on happiness. Earlier studies reported various levels of correlation between income and happiness—in some cases, no relation at all [29, 44, 46]. Studies favoring the positive effects of wealth argued that higher income boosts purchasing power, expands affordable goods, and increases consumption, which leads to improved wellbeing [47]. Experimental studies showed that the increase in income significantly affects the level of happiness. For example, unconditional cash transfers from government or NGO increased recipients’ happiness in Zambia [48] and Kenya [49]. A study using a multilevel model with data from World Values Survey (N = 64,923, k = 81 nations) showed consistent results [50].

In support of the adverse effects of wealth on happiness, a review study noted that income only accounted for 4% of the variance of subjective wellbeing [51]. Higher-income is related to less daily sadness, but not to daily happiness [52]. More recent studies depicted time- and money-spending behaviors as variables that should be accounted for in the relationship between wealth and happiness [48, 53,54,55,56,57].

This study used subjective wealth as a proxy for objective wealth. Previous studies showed that subjective wealth is associated with objective wealth [58]. There is also a consensus among researchers that subjective wealth is a predictor of general happiness [58, 59].

Education and happiness

The direct relationship between education and happiness is still unclear, though several studies found correlations between them [60,61,62,63,64]. Jongbloed [64] stated that higher education is significantly associated with happiness. One possible explanation is that higher education is associated with longer and healthier lives, successful marriages, higher quality of interpersonal relationships, and better opportunities on the labor market [62, 65]. Another study stated that non-monetary factors also play a role in the relationship between education and happiness, e.g., interpersonal networking and degree of cosmopolitanism. Better-educated people have broader social networks and involvement with the wider world, which is associated with happiness [66].

Nevertheless, an increasing number of studies established an insignificant or declined relationship between higher education and happiness [29, 62]. One study pointed out that income and occupation moderated the association between education and happiness [67]. When income and occupation were controlled, education had a negative effect on happiness [67]. A longitudinal study found similar results; participants with only secondary education (non-vocational) were healthier, happier, and wealthier when compared to other groups [68].

Education’s correlation with happiness seems to be affected by the country’s overall welfare, with high correlations in developing nations and low correlation in rich ones [69]. This is not because education breeds dissatisfaction but possibly due to scarcity of employment that matches the level of education or the fading of earlier advantages in the process of social equalizing [69].

Health and happiness

The hypothesis that health affects happiness is widely accepted. One study found that self-rated health correlated significantly with subjective wellbeing [70]. A recent study based on Asian samples also concluded that self-rated health greatly affected happiness, especially when physical health problems occurred [71]. Another study, based on a community sample of older adults, concluded that health status is one of the most influential predictors of happiness [72]. However, disease severity has little effect on happiness. People with cancer can be happier than people with allergies. A higher relationship was found between happiness and the degree to which disease disrupts daily functioning [72].

Based on the context that physical activity’s psychological benefits are limited and mostly related to cognitive functions, there is a need to examine the effect of physical activity on other psychological functions, i.e., happiness. The present study proposed a model to investigate the association between physical activity and happiness. Based on the aforementioned studies, the model includes other related variables—subjective wealth, health, and education. The analysis also controlled for age and gender. Figure 1 shows the hypothesized relationship model between physical activity, subjective wealth, education, health, and happiness.

$${\displaystyle \begin{array}{c}{\mathcal{y}}_1=\upalpha +{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_1+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_2+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_3+{\upzeta}_1\\ {}{\mathcal{y}}_2=\upalpha +{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_1+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_2+{\upgamma \mathcal{x}}_3+{\mathcal{y}}_2\uppsi +{\upzeta}_2\end{array}}$$
Fig. 1
figure 1

Hypothesized model of physical activity’s effects on happiness

Where 𝓎 is the endogenous variable (i.e., 𝓎1: health, 𝓎2: happiness), α is the intercept, γ is the regression coefficient, 𝓍 is the exogenous variable (i.e., 𝓍1: physical activity, 𝓍2: education, 𝓍3: subjective wealth), ψ is the residual variance of the endogenous variable, and ζ is the residual or error in the equation [73].

Materials and methods

This study is a quantitative study using a structural equation modeling (SEM). SEM is a multivariate technique combining aspects of factor analysis and multiple regression that enables the researcher to simultaneously examine a series of interrelated dependence relationships among variables [74]. The data were from a longitudinal socioeconomic and health survey in Indonesia, the Indonesian Family Life Survey (IFLS). IFLS has been conducted five times, in 1993, 1998, 2000, 2007, and 2014–2015. IFLS sampling scheme stratified on provinces and urban/rural location and then randomly sampled within the strata. The sampling method considered the cultural and socioeconomic diversity in Indonesia and represent four most populated islands in Indonesia—Java, Sumatra, Kalimantan, and Sulawesi—containing 83% of the population. The first IFLS sampling frame were based on 1993 SUSENAS (National Socioeconomic Survey), which was based on 1990 census [75].

The IFLS surveys were reviewed and approved by IRBs in the United States and in Indonesia at Universitas Gadjah Mada (UGM). The ethical clearance number from RAND’s Human Subjects Protection Committee (RAND’s IRB) was s0064-06-01-CR01.

Participants

From the total dataset, 12,051 participants were selected for the analysis. The gender composition was 47.4% male (n = 5711) and 52.6% female (n = 6340). Participant inclusion was based on: (1) ages 18 to 65, (2) completion the necessary variables in the questionnaire, and (3) not having a chronic disease (i.e., asthma, cancer or malignant tumor, memory-related disease, high-cholesterol).

Variables

Physical activities

The IFLS (Indonesian Family Life Survey) questionnaire includes physical activity variables in Book IIIB Section KK (Health condition). There are nine questions in the section measuring activities type and their duration. Participants were asked to quantify their activity in the previous 7 days. The levels of physical activity were vigorous activities, moderate physical effort, and walking [76]. Vigorous activities were described as those that made participants breathe much harder than usual, such as heavy lifting, digging, plowing, aerobics, fast bicycling, and cycling with loads. Moderate physical activities were those that made participants breathe somewhat harder than usual, such as carrying light loads, bicycling at a regular pace, and mopping the floor. Walking included daily walking at work, at home, to travel from place to place, or in any other context related to recreation, sport, exercise, or leisure. The intensity of physical activities was measured by the duration per day (less than 30 minutes to more than 4 hours) and the number of days performed in the previous 7 days.

Physical activity data were converted to metabolic equivalent value (MET) according to the Compendium of Physical Activities [77]. One MET is equal to the energy spent when a person sits still. The MET for walking is from two to eight, depending on speed and obstacles. In this study, vigorous physical activities were categorized as multiple household tasks (vigorous) in the Compendium of Physical Activities, which equals four MET. Moderate physical activities were categorized as multiple household tasks (medium), which equals 3.5 MET, while walking was categorized as equal to 2.5 MET. The participants’ METs were afterward converted to MET minutes (MET × 60 seconds). The participants’ average MET minutes were 69.6 (SD = 55.5).

Subjective wealth

Question SW01 from Book IIIA of the IFLS questionnaire measured the subjective wealth of the participants, who were asked to rate their overall wealth from 1 ‘Poorest’ to 6 ‘Richest’. The average response was 2.86 (SD = .8).

Education

Participants were asked about their highest level of education (question DL6 Book IIIA). Among them, 6.6% had at least a bachelor’s degree, and 5.9% had a diploma degree (3 years of vocational education after high school). Table 1 shows the complete statistics of participants’ education levels.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics

Health

Participants’ responses to question KK01 in Book IIIB were used to indicate health. Participants were asked about the general condition of their health. More than 88.6% stated that they were somewhat healthy or very healthy. Table 1 presents the complete statistics of the participants’ responses.

Happiness

Participants’ responses to question SW12 of the IFLS Book IIIA were used to indicate happiness. Participants were asked to rate their happiness. The scale ranged from 1 ‘Very unhappy’ to 4 ‘Very happy.’ Participants’ average self-rated happiness was 2.98 (SD = .4). More than 91% of the participants were happy or very happy.

Statistical analysis

In the present study, the model was tested using structural equation modeling or analysis of covariance structure. To assess the fitness of the model, it is necessary to report fit statistics such as χ2 value and degrees of freedom; the CFI (comparative fit index) or TLI (Tucker-Lewis Index); and RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation) [74, 78]. The threshold of fit indices was based on the recommendation of Hair et al. [74]. For a model with less than 12 observed variables and n more than 250, the suggested values are CFI ≥ .97 and RMSEA < .07 [74].

Results

When the hypothesized model was fitted to the data, the following fit indices resulted: χ2 (1, N = 12,051) = 48.733, p = .001, RMSEA = .063, CFI = .970. This fulfilled the requirements for a good model fit by Hair and colleagues [74]. Figure 2 depicts the structural model.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Final model of physical activity’s effects on happiness

The analysis was controlled for participants’ gender. Studies show that gender difference is relevant in happiness studies. Table 2 shows the fit statistics of the final model by gender.

Table 2 Fit statistics of the model by gender and field of study

As shown in Table 3, all standardized regression weights are positively significant except for the path from physical activity to happiness which is negatively significant. Table 3 and Fig. 2 show the mediating effects of health in the relationship between physical activity and happiness. The highest effect was found in the path from subjective wealth to happiness (β = .13), followed by health to happiness (β = .095), and education to happiness (β = .095). Education, subjective wealth, and physical activity positively affect health (β = .049, β = .048, and β = .025, respectively). The value above happiness (.04) in Fig. 2 is the square of multiple correlations and indicates the variability in happiness that is explained by other variables in the model. This means the variables in the model explained only 4% of the variability in happiness.

Table 3 Standardized regression weight of the predictors on happiness

Discussion

Based on data from a large survey in Indonesia, the present study examined the relationship between physical activity, subjective wealth, health, education, and happiness. Based on the structural model, the overall analysis showed that the hypothesized model is a good fit for the dataset.

Further analysis found that physical activity did not directly affect happiness. Physical activity has a negative effect on physical activity. However, health conditions mediated the relationship between physical activity and happiness. This result is consistent with prior studies on the mediating effects of health on happiness [33, 79, 80]. Few studies have investigated the mediating effect of health in general populations [79]; most focused on older populations [80, 81]. The present study makes a noteworthy contribution since the dataset was based on a large sample from the general population of a developing country.

A recent review highlighted the mediating effects of health on the relationship between physical activity and happiness [33]. Engaging in physical activity contributes to the perception of good health, thus potentially improving happiness [79]. Other studies also showed an indirect association between physical activity and happiness, mediated by health and social functioning [80]. A possible explanation is that less physically active people, such as physically limited or ill older adults, face difficulty socializing with others. This leads to poor social functioning related to low levels of happiness [80].

The negative effect of physical activity on happiness may partly be explained by the participants’ diverse physical activity levels. A high level of physical activity is often associated with a blue-collar occupation [82], lower socioeconomic status, and a low level of happiness. Further analysis of the model by controlling the level of physical activity—including only participants who have recommended physical activity [83, 84]—supported this argument. After controlling the level of physical activity, the model showed a non-significant direct effect of physical activity on happiness.

Another contribution of the present study is the use of a structural model rarely used by previous studies investigating physical activity and happiness. Structural equation modeling is the appropriate method for evaluating a series of simultaneous hypotheses about the impacts of latent and manifest variables on other variables while taking measurement errors into account [74, 85]. Therefore, the present study’s structural modeling gives a better understanding of the relationships between physical activity, education, subjective wealth, health, and happiness.

In the present study, the highest regression coefficient was in the path from subjective wealth to happiness. This finding supports prior studies regarding subjective wealth’s positive effects on happiness [43, 45, 50]. Classic economic theories posit that higher-income boosts purchasing power, expands affordable goods, and increases consumption, leading to improved wellbeing [47]. One study showed that wealth is positively and significantly associated with happiness in low-income and high-income countries; however, higher effects were found in the former than in the latter [50]. This relates to the ceiling effect on the relationship between income and happiness—i.e., income satiation. After reaching a certain level, income no longer affects happiness [57].

The structural model also showed a significant positive effect of education on happiness. These results support previous research on the association between education and happiness [60, 61, 63,64,65, 86]. However, an increasing number of studies depict an insignificant relationship between education and happiness [67,68,69]. One possible explanation for this is that most of these studies use life satisfaction as a proxy for happiness [86], which is inaccurate because it only measures the evaluative dimension of happiness, excluding the hedonic and eudaimonic dimensions [86].

Several limitations must be noted regarding the present study. First, most variables were based on subjective self-report, which is open to biases. Responses might be affected by social norms [29]; e.g., participants might under-report their wealth because humbleness is valued under Indonesian social norms. The second is that, evengthough SEM previously used to indicate a causality evidence [87], the cross-sectional design of this study prohibiting evidence for directionality relations. Future studies should consider using objective measurements—i.e., for health and wealth. The use of latent variables should also be considered. Including variables such as religiosity and social relationships would be interesting, especially in the Indonesian context.

Conclusion

This study suggests that health mediates physical activity’s effects on happiness. Active individuals would have better health compared to their sedentary counterparts. Health condition, as suggested by the model, was one of the biggest predictors of happiness. The fact that physical activity positively affects happiness should also motivate Indonesians to engage in more active lifestyles. This is important since the national health survey revealed that more than one-third (33.5%) of Indonesians did not get enough physical activity [88].

Availability of data and materials

This study used IFLS Public Use Data which are available in RAND website: https://www.rand.org/well-being/social-and-behavioral-policy/data/FLS/IFLS/access.html.

References

  1. Brannon L, Feist J, Updegraff J. Health psychology: an introduction to behavior and health. Wadsworth: Cengage Learning; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Blair SN, Kampert JB, Kohl HW III, et al. Influences of cardiorespiratory fitness and other precursors on cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality in men and women. JAMA. 1996;276(3):205–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Miles L. Physical activity and the prevention of cancer: a review of recent findings. Nutr Bull. 2007;32(3):250–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Kokila G, Smitha T. Cancer and physical activity. J Oral Maxillofac Pathol. 2017;21(1):4–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Kruk J, Czerniak U. Physical activity and its relation to cancer risk: updating the evidence. Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2013;14(7):3993–4003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Rezende LFM, Sa TH, Markozannes G, Rey-Lopez JP, Lee IM, Tsilidis KK, et al. Physical activity and cancer: an umbrella review of the literature including 22 major anatomical sites and 770 000 cancer cases. Br J Sports Med. 2018;52(13):826–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Araiza P, Hewes H, Gashetewa C, Vella CA, Burge MR. Efficacy of a pedometer-based physical activity program on parameters of diabetes control in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Metab Clin Exp. 2006;55(10):1382–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Way KL, Hackett DA, Baker MK, Johnson NA. The effect of regular exercise on insulin sensitivity in type 2 diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Diabetes Metab J. 2016;40(4):253–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Asano RY, Sales MM, Browne RA, Moraes JF, Coelho Junior HJ, Moraes MR, et al. Acute effects of physical exercise in type 2 diabetes: a review. World J Diabetes. 2014;5(5):659–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Conn VS, Hafdahl AR, LeMaster J, Ruppar T, Cochran J, Nielsen P. Meta-analysis of health behavior change interventions in type 1 diabetes. Am J Health Behav. 2008;32(3):315–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Colberg SR, Laan R, Dassau E, Kerr D. Physical activity and type 1 diabetes: time for a rewire? J Diabetes Sci Technol. 2015;9(3):609–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Burr JF, Shephard RJ, Riddell MC. Physical activity in type 1 diabetes mellitus: assessing risks for physical activity clearance and prescription. Can Fam Physician. 2012;58(5):533–5.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Colberg SR, Sigal RJ, Yardley JE, Riddell MC, Dunstan DW, Dempsey PC, et al. Physical activity/exercise and diabetes: a position statement of the American Diabetes Association. Diabetes Care. 2016;39(11):2065–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Blair SN. Physical inactivity: the biggest public health problem of the 21st century. Br J Sports Med. 2009;43(1):1–2.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kramer AF, Colcombe SJ, McAuley E, Scalf PE, Erickson KI. Fitness, aging and neurocognitive function. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26(1, Supplement):124–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Mandolesi L, Polverino A, Montuori S, Foti F, Ferraioli G, Sorrentino P, et al. Effects of physical exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing: biological and psychological benefits. Front Psychol. 2018;9:509.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Cox EP, O'Dwyer N, Cook R, Vetter M, Cheng HL, Rooney K, et al. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(8):616–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(9):3316–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004;292(12):1454–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, et al. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300(9):1027–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Wendell CR, Gunstad J, Waldstein SR, Wright JG, Ferrucci L, Zonderman AB. Cardiorespiratory fitness and accelerated cognitive decline with aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(4):455–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Babyak M, Blumenthal JA, Herman S, Khatri P, Doraiswamy M, Moore K, et al. Exercise treatment for major depression: maintenance of therapeutic benefit at 10 months. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(5):633–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Martinsen EW. Physical activity in the prevention and treatment of anxiety and depression. Nord J Psychiatry. 2008;62(s47):25–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kim DJ. Effects of physical activity on depression in adults with diabetes. Osong Public Health Res Perspect. 2018;9(4):143–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Mayor S. Physical activity reduces risk of depression at any age, finds study. BMJ. 2018;361:k1808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Anderson E, Shivakumar G. Effects of exercise and physical activity on anxiety. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Stonerock GL, Hoffman BM, Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA. Exercise as treatment for anxiety: systematic review and analysis. Ann Behav Med. 2015;49(4):542–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Paxton R, Motl R, Aylward A, Nigg C. Physical activity and quality of life—the complementary influence of self-efficacy for physical activity and mental health difficulties. Int J Behav Med. 2010;17(4):255–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Argyle M. The psychology of happiness. Sussex: Routledge; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Diener E, Lucas RE, Schimmack U, Helliwell JF. Defining well-being: Well-Being for Public Policy: Oxford University Press; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Helliwell JF, Putnam RD. The social context of well-being. In: Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B, editors. The science of well-being: Oxford University Press; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Marks N, Shah H. A well-being manifesto for a flourishing society. In: Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B, editors. The science of well-being: Oxford University Press; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Zhang Z, Chen W. A systematic review of the relationship between physical activity and happiness. J Happiness Stud. 2018;20:1305–22.

  34. Richards J, Jiang X, Kelly P, Chau J, Bauman A, Ding D. Don't worry, be happy: cross-sectional associations between physical activity and happiness in 15 European countries. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Harris MA. The relationship between physical inactivity and mental wellbeing: findings from a gamification-based community-wide physical activity intervention. Health Psychol Open. 2018;5(1):2055102917753853.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Lapa TY. Physical activity levels and psychological well-being: a case study of university students. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. 2015;186:739–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Veenhoven R. World database of happiness. Soc Indic Res. 1995;34(3):299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Gomes R, Gonçalves S, Costa J. Exercise, eating disordered behaviors and psychological well-being: a study with Portuguese adolescents. Rev Latinoam Psicol. 2015;47(1):66–74.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Tse MMY, Tang SK, Wan VTC, Vong SKS. The effectiveness of physical exercise training in pain, mobility, and psychological well-being of older persons living in nursing homes. Pain Manag Nurs. 2014;15(4):778–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Fortier MS, Guerin E, Williams T, Strachan S. Should I exercise or sleep to feel better? A daily analysis with physically active working mothers. Ment Health Phys Act. 2015;8(0):56–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Dishman RK, O'Connor PJ. Lessons in exercise neurobiology: the case of endorphins. Ment Health Phys Act. 2009;2(1):4–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Boecker H, Sprenger T, Spilker ME, Henriksen G, Koppenhoefer M, Wagner KJ, et al. The runner’s high: opioidergic mechanisms in the human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(11):2523–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Lucas RE, Schimmack U. Income and well-being: how big is the gap between the rich and the poor? J Res Pers. 2009;43(1):75–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Piff PK, Moskowitz JP. Wealth, poverty, and happiness: social class is differentially associated with positive emotions. Emotion. 2018;18(6):902–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gere J, Schimmack U. Benefits of income: associations with life satisfaction among earners and homemakers. Personal Individ Differ. 2017;119:92–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade D, Schwarz N, Stone AA. Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science. 2006;312(5782):1908–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Howell R, Howell CJ. The relation of economic status to subjective well-being in developing countries: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(4):536–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Natali L, Handa S, Peterman A, Seidenfeld D, Tembo G. Zambia cash transfer evaluation T. does money buy happiness? Evidence from an unconditional cash transfer in Zambia. SSM Popul Health. 2018;4:225–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Haushofer J, Shapiro J. The short-term impact of unconditional cash transfers to the poor: experimental evidence from Kenya*. Q J Econ. 2016;131(4):1973–2042.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Sarracino F. Determinants of subjective well-being in high and low-income countries: do happiness equations differ across countries? 2013. p. 51–66.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Lucas RE, Dyrenforth PS. Does the existence of social relationships matter for subjective well-being? In: Finkel EJ, Vohs KD, editors. Self and relationships: connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. p. 254–73.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Kushlev K, Dunn EW, Lucas RE. Higher income is associated with less daily sadness but not more daily happiness. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2015;6(5):483–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Aaker JL, Rudd M, Mogilner C. If money does not make you happy, consider time. J Consum Psychol. 2011;21(2):126–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Dunn EW, Gilbert DT, Wilson TD. If money doesn't make you happy, then you probably aren't spending it right. J Consum Psychol. 2011;21(2):115–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Kaun DE. Income and happiness: earning and spending as sources of discontent. J Socio-Econ. 2005;34(2):161–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Boyce CJ, Daly M, Hounkpatin HO, Wood AM. Money may buy happiness, but often so little that it Doesn't matter. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(4):544–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Jebb AT, Tay L, Diener E, Oishi S. Happiness, income satiation and turning points around the world. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(1):33–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Gasiorowska A. The relationship between objective and subjective wealth is moderated by financial control and mediated by money anxiety. J Econ Psychol. 2014;43:64–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Joo SH, Grable JE. An exploratory framework of the determinants of financial satisfaction. J Fam Econ Iss. 2004;25(1):25–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Nikolaev B, Rusakov P. Education and happiness: an alternative hypothesis. Appl Econ Lett. 2015;23(12):827–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Swiatek L. Education for sustainable happiness and well-being. High Educ Res Dev. 2017;36(6):1307–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Nikolaev B. Does higher education increase hedonic and Eudaimonic happiness? J Happiness Stud. 2016;19:483–504.

  63. Ruiu G, Ruiu ML. The complex relationship between education and happiness: the case of highly educated individuals in Italy. J Happiness Stud. 2018;20:2631–53.

  64. Jongbloed J. Higher education for happiness? Investigating the impact of education on the hedonic and eudaimonic well-being of Europeans. Eur Educ Res J. 2018;17(5):733–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Oreopoulos P, Salvanes KG. Priceless: the nonpecuniary benefits of schooling. J Econ Perspect. 2011;25(1):159–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Chen W-c. How education enhances happiness: comparison of mediating factors in four east Asian countries. Soc Indic Res. 2012;106(1):117–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Clark AE, Oswald AJ. Satisfaction and comparison income. J Public Econ. 1996;61(3):359–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Hartog J, Oosterbeek H. Health, wealth and happiness: why pursue a higher education? Econ Educ Rev. 1998;17(3):245–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Veenhoven R. Developments in satisfaction-research. Soc Indic Res. 1996;37(1):1–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Okun MA, George LK. Physician- and self-ratings of health, neuroticism and subjective well-being among men and women. Personal Individ Differ. 1984;5(5):533–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Park JH, Lee KS. Self-rated health and its determinants in Japan and South Korea. Public Health. 2013;127(9):834–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Angner E, Ghandhi J, Purvis KW, Amante D, Allison J. Daily functioning, health status, and happiness in older adults. J Happiness Stud. 2013;14:1563–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Jöreskog KG, Olsson UH, Wallentin FY. Multivariate analysis with LISREL: Springer International Publishing; 2016.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  74. Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate data analysis: Pearson Education Limited; 2014. p. 734.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Strauss J, Witoelar F, Sikoki B. The fifth wave of the Indonesia family life survey: overview and field report: RAND Corporation; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Strauss J, Witoelar F, Sikoki B, Wattie AM. The fourth wave of the Indonesia family life survey (IFLS4): overview and field report; 2009.

  77. Ainsworth BE, Haskell WL, Leon AS, Jacobs DR Jr, Montoye HJ, Sallis JF, et al. Compendium of physical activities: classification of energy costs of human physical activities. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):71–80.

  78. Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. 2nd ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2015. p. 462.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Ross A, Cloutier S, Searle M. The association between leisure time physical activity and happiness: testing the indirect role of health perception. J Community Psychol. 2019;47(5):1169–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. de Souto BP. Direct and indirect relationships between physical activity and happiness levels among older adults: a cross-sectional study. Aging Ment Health. 2014;18(7):861–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Lera-López F, Ollo-López A, Sánchez-Santos JM. How does physical activity make you feel better? The mediational role of perceived health. Appl Res Qual Life. 2017;12(3):511–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Korshøj M, Gupta N, Mortensen OS, Jørgensen MB, Holtermann A. Intensity of occupational physical activity in blue-collar workers: do self-reported rating and device-worn measurements agree? Eur J Appl Physiol. 2022;22(5):1293–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Jakicic JM, Otto AD. Physical activity considerations for the treatment and prevention of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82(1):226S–9S.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. WHO. Global recommendations on physical activity for health. Switzerland: WHO; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Lee S-Y. Structural equation modeling: a Bayesian approach. Sussex: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2007.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  86. Nikolaev B. Does higher education increase hedonic and Eudaimonic happiness? J Happiness Stud. 2018;19(2):483–504.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Bullock HE, Harlow LL, Mulaik SA. Causation issues in structural equation modeling research. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1994;1(3):253–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. MoH. Laporan nasional Riskesdas 2018. Jakarta: Badan Penelitian dan Pengembangan Kesehatan Kementrian Kesehatan RI; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The author confirms sole responsibility for the study conception and design, data analysis, interpretation of results, and manuscript preparation. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bhina Patria.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The research methods were performed in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of The Declaration of Helsinki. The IFLS surveys were reviewed and approved by IRBs in the United States and in Indonesia at Universitas Gadjah Mada (UGM). The ethical clearance number from RAND’s Human Subjects Protection Committee (RAND’s IRB) was s0064-06-01-CR01. All requirements for consent were met and approved by those IRBs. The data used in this study were anonymized before its use.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Patria, B. Modeling the effects of physical activity, education, health, and subjective wealth on happiness based on Indonesian national survey data. BMC Public Health 22, 959 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13371-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-13371-x

Keywords