Skip to main content

Health authorities’ health risk communication with the public during pandemics: a rapid scoping review

Abstract

Background

Responses from the H1N1 swine flu pandemic and the recent COVID-19 coronavirus pandemic provide an opportunity for insight into the role of health authorities’ ways of communicating health risk information to the public. We aimed to synthesise the existing evidence regarding different modes of communication used by health authorities in health risk communication with the public during a pandemic.

Methods

We conducted a rapid scoping review. MEDLINE and EMBASE were searched for publications in English from January 2009 through October 2020, covering both the full H1N1 pandemic and the response phase during the COVID-19 pandemic. The search resulted in 1440 records, of which 48 studies met our eligibility criteria.

Results

The present review identified studies across a broad interdisciplinary field of health risk communication. The majority focused on the H1N1 pandemic and the COVID-19 pandemic. A content analysis of the studies identified three categories for modes of communication: i) communication channels, ii) source credibility and iii) how the message is communicated. The identified studies on social media focused mainly on content and engagement, while studies on the effect of the use of social media and self-protective behaviour were lacking. Studies on the modes of communication that take the diversity of receivers in the field into account are lacking. A limited number of studies of health authorities’ use of graphic and audio-visual means were identified, yet these did not consider/evaluate creative communication choices.

Conclusion

Experimental studies that investigate the effect of health authorities’ videos and messages on social media platforms and self-protective behaviour are needed. More studies are needed across the fields of health risk communication and media studies, including visual communication, web design, video and digital marketing, at a time when online digital communication is central to reaching the public.

Peer Review reports

Background

A pandemic “is an epidemic occurring worldwide over a wide area, crossing international boundaries and usually affecting a large number of people…The agent must be able to infect humans, to cause disease in humans and to spread easily from human to human” [[1] p. 2019]. Examples of pandemics are the “Spanish flu” pandemic (1918–1919), the AIDS pandemic (1966-), the “swine flu” pandemic caused by the H1N1 virus (2009–2010), and the recent coronavirus disease 2019, “COVID-19”, caused by the SARS-COV-2 virus (2019-). Pandemic responses are unique in their dependence on expert-based agencies. For instance, pandemic responses during the H1N1 swine flu pandemic were driven by the bureaucratic expert judgement of public health agencies rather than by politicians guided by principles of political survival [2]. Literature reviews of demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviour during pandemic and infectious disease outbreaks have found that participants who see the disease as more severe are more likely to engage in protective behaviour [3,4,5]. Since pandemics are defined by geography and virology, not by severity, they may cause challenges for risk communication of pandemic infections with low lethality [6]. Furthermore, pandemics spread globally, causing disease in different places at different times. The pathogen can change with time and location and can have effects lasting several years with changing patterns of severity [7], with the potential for devastating impacts on health, economy, and quality of life worldwide [8,9,10,11]. Health authorities are challenged by the complexity of pandemic risk communication and the need to reach out to multiple groups of individuals in the public [12, 13].

Risk communication is an interdisciplinary field of research and practice [14]. In the context of public health emergencies, “health communication” is an integrated part of risk communication, which is emphasised by the literature using the term “health risk communication” [13, 15,16,17,18]. Health communication and risk communication in public health emergencies, including pandemics, aims to improve health outcomes by influencing, engaging and reaching out to different at-risk audiences with health-related information [12, 19, 20]. Due to the integrative nature of the literature, this review uses the term “health risk communication”. Trust, engagement and tailored communication are among the key concepts in health risk communication, and a lack of these concepts could hinder effective communication [12, 20].

Health risk communication can be transmitted according to different modes of communication. The term mode can be described as a way to be or to do things [21], and in the context of this review, it refers to how health authorities communicate risk to the public. New modes of communication and media technology have dramatically influenced health risk communication through the way the public seeks health information online and on social media [22]. However, the rapid transformation in communications technology, including the near-universal use of mobile telephones and the widespread use of digital media, has a major impact on traditional mass media (television, radio and newspapers). Online communication changes how people access and trust health information [12]. Online newspapers and social media had an important role in health risk communication related to the H1N1 pandemic [23, 24] and even more so in the COVID-19 pandemic [25,26,27]. Social media platforms also offer new possibilities for two-way communication – that is, speaking with and not only to the public [12, 27, 28]. However, these are not the only modes of communication health authorities use to reach out to the public. Videos, mass media, websites, and prints are examples of other modes of communication that health authorities use in health risk communication with the public.

Previous systematic reviews have synthesised knowledge on risk communication in response to emergent infectious diseases [12, 28] and the H1N1 pandemic [7, 13, 18, 29]. Other reviews have focused on the role of social media during emergent infectious diseases [23, 26] and the COVID-19 pandemic [25, 27, 30] and the role of mass media and public health communication in the COVID-19 pandemic [31]. However, there is currently a lack of synthesised knowledge related to health authorities’ use of diverse modes of communication in pandemic risk communication. Therefore, the aim of this rapid scoping review was to synthesise the evidence regarding the different modes of communication used by health authorities in health risk communication with the public during a pandemic. More specifically, we aimed to obtain a broad overview of the evidence pertaining to diverse modes of communication, irrespective of the study quality, to clarify some key topics and types of outcomes (e.g., knowledge, trust, health literacy, adherence to recommendations) and identify research gaps in correspondence with the scoping review approach [32].

Methods

The present study applied a rapid scoping review approach that supports a streamlined approach to data identification, extraction, and synthesis [33, 34]. We conducted our review using an adapted version of the Rapid Review approach advocated by the World Health Organization (WHO) [34], guidance for following systematic scoping reviews in healthcare [33], and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist [32]. The WHO defines a rapid review as “…a type of knowledge synthesis in which systematic review processes are accelerated and methods are streamlined to complete the review more quickly than is the case for typical systematic reviews.” [[34] p. 3]. Rapid reviews essentially accelerate or reduce different parts of traditional review methods, which reduces the time taken to move through the process [34]. Scoping reviews are used to obtain a broad overview of the evidence pertaining to a topic and are useful when examining areas that are emerging, such as pandemic risk communication. The scoping review is used to clarify key concepts and identify gaps in line with the review aim [33].

A protocol was drafted and agreed upon with the wider research group prior to commencing the review but not formally registered in line with the rapid review approach [34].

Eligibility criteria

The “Population-Concept-Context” (PCC) approach was used to specify our rationale and eligibility criteria [32].

Concept

We included studies of modes of communication concerning pandemic health risk communication from health authorities to the public. Modes of communication in this paper included but were not limited to web-based information, social media, television, newspapers, video, texts, and narratives. We conceptualised health authorities from a broad perspective, including governments, official health experts, healthcare professionals as official spokespersons, health authority officials, health agencies, and official health bureaucrats, at the regional, national, or international level (i.e., the WHO). We excluded studies concerning health communication between individuals, such as a medical doctor and a patient (e.g., e-health, telemedicine), or between healthcare professionals (e.g., digital educational methods, digital solutions).

Context

Pandemics included but were not limited to swine flu (H1N1) and COVID-19. The COVID-19 pandemic is of primary interest, but as it is currently ongoing, searches on this topic could only provide studies on the response phase and use of diverse modes of communication; thus, we included the swine flu pandemic, as it might include knowledge on a long-term perspective on the pandemic. We excluded studies concerning infectious diseases without pandemic potential. Only studies after 2009 were included, which reflects the timeframe of the evidence generated following the last large-scale pandemic (swine flu) and the need for evidence about communication modes to reflect the scale of technological change over the past decade.

Population

We included studies of communication to the public and specific target groups in the public without any predetermined categorisation. Commentaries, reviews, opinion pieces, or other papers not reporting primary empirical research were excluded. English-language articles for both qualitative and quantitative peer-reviewed empirical studies were included.

Search and information sources

We limited our searches to the two bibliographic databases MEDLINE and EMBASE, as advised for rapid reviews [34]. To accelerate the research process and ensure quality through peer review, the search was restricted to peer-reviewed published studies, and no grey literature searches were conducted [34]. In line with the PRISMA guidelines [32], the selection of databases, search terms, and search methodology was determined in collaboration with a university library technician who designed the final search. The final search results were exported to EndNote, and duplicates were removed by a university library technician. After screening pilot searches, the main search was conducted on 28th Oct 2020. We searched using the terms health, risk, mass, crisis, or media communication, communication methods, modes of communication, sources of communication, and H1N1, COVID-19 and/or pandemic. No filters were added in MEDLINE and EMBASE (e.g., language). The searches were limited to 2009-current. The full electronic search strategy for EMBASE and MEDLINE is found in Additional file 1.

Selection of sources of evidence

The search yielded a total of 1440 hits, of which 492 hits were in EMBASE and 948 were in MEDLINE. Removing duplicates resulted in 1053 unique hits. A total of 127 articles were read in full text and assessed for eligibility, 79 articles were excluded, and 48 articles were included in the review, as displayed in the PRISMA flow diagram (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram

As part of the adaptations for undertaking a rapid scoping review [34], one reviewer (SHB) undertook the screening, inclusion, data extraction and charting for included studies, in dialogue with JKOH and SW. SHB screened titles and abstracts using the eligibility criteria outlined above; see additional file 2 for screening questions. A pilot screening was conducted with a pilot search on 16. October 2020 to improve the final search and consistency of the screening process. Full-text screening proceeded against the inclusion criteria to produce a final list of included papers. We did not undertake reference screening or contact with paper authors [34].

Data charting process and analysis

Following guidance for completing scoping reviews [33], SHB extracted data from included papers in a matrix prior to synthesis: author, year of publication, country of origin, aims/purpose, study population, methodology and sample description, concept, outcomes and key findings relating to the research objectives. Data synthesis was achieved through qualitative content analysis [35]. Pilot testing of the data extraction form was conducted by extracting information from three studies. The pilot-testing was reviewed by JKOH [32]. The results relevant to the review question were summarised, coded, and categorised inductively into three main categories. The analysis was conducted by SHB in collaboration with JKOH, MTS and SW and validated by the co-authors. In keeping with the rapid scoping review approach, we did not undertake an appraisal of the included studies [33]. The main categories described are presented descriptively within each category close to the original findings of the included studies [35]. A table was made to describe the included studies’ reference, context, aim, method and sample, types of outcomes and modes of communication reported (see Table 2). KKB validated the content in Table 2.

Results

Study characteristics

Of the 48 articles included, 33 included studies concerning the COVID-19 coronavirus pandemic, 12 studies concerning the H1N1 swine flu pandemic, and three studying pandemic influenza in general. The studies’ data collection was conducted in North America (N = 15, of which 11 were of US origin), Asia (N = 13, of which eight were of Chinese origin), Europe (N = 9, of which two were collected in multiple European countries), Africa (N = 1), and Australia (N = 1). No studies were of South American origin. Nine studies collected data across continents/global studies, of which three studies collected data across continents, five studies collected data on global media use, and one study examined the WHO [60]. The data collection distribution by continents/globally is displayed in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Choropleth of data collection distribution by continents/globally

The methods used were cross-sectional surveys (N = 14), one comparative survey [37] and one mixed-method survey and qualitative interview [48]. None of the survey studies had a longitudinal design. Six of the included studies were qualitative interview studies [52, 55, 68, 76, 79, 80]. The study of Kavaliunas et al. [78] was a policy analysis, and the study of King et al. [48] was a case study with multiple qualitative methods. Four studies had an experimental design [73,74,75, 77], of which the study of Okuhara et al. [75] was a randomised controlled trial (RCT). Eighteen of the included studies applied a quantitative statistical analysis in terms of content analysis or infodemiology studies of diverse media types (television, social media platforms, and YouTube) or webstudies. Three of these studies examined media trends over time (one- to four-month period) [15, 59, 64]. Two studies applied a qualitative content/thematic analysis of media types [54, 56]. The methodological designs in the included studies are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1 Methodological design in the included studies

The 48 articles that were included in the review are displayed in Table 2.

Table 2 Included studies reporting on health authorities and modes of communications

Analysis of included studies

The categories describe the evidence related to types of outcomes and the key topics related to how health authorities communicate risk to the public. These were brought together into three main categories: i) communication channels, which describe what media types are used by health authorities, and two additional categories, which describe how and why health authorities use these media types, categorised as ii) source credibility and iii) how the message is communicated.

Communication channels

Communication channels describe the outcomes related to how people receive pandemic information from health authorities through multiple communication channels, traditional mass media, governmental websites, social media platforms and YouTube videos. Key topics were related to multiple channels: framing, engagement, misinformation, health literacy, self-efficacy, accessibility, and timeliness of the updates.

Multiple communication channels

Fourteen of the included studies used survey methodologies with self-reported use of communication channels, with health authorities among the information sources [36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51]. These studies found that people receive pandemic health risk information through a broad spectrum of communication channels and information sources and that they are influenced not only by newspapers, television, printed information, governmental websites, scientific articles, radio, and SMS from the government but also by interpersonal and informal sources, such as friends, family, healthcare professionals, and social media [36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50]. Hence, people are not passive receivers of health risk information but are influenced by the totality of the information they receive from various and multiple communication channels and information sources in addition to health authorities’ health risk communication.

Traditional mass media

Five of the included studies of health authorities’ health risk communication and traditional mass media (television and newspapers) reported on outcomes related to framing and the use of mass media as a communication channel [52,53,54,55,56]. A central topic was how framing messages could affect public responses. Framing is defined by Nisbeth [[81], p. 216] as “interpretative story lines that set a specific train of thought in motion, communicating why an issue might be a problem or pose a threat or what might be responsible for it, and what should be done about it”. Studies of media coverage in mass media during the H1N1 pandemic found that the mass media used sensationalist framing of their messages, conflict frames and war metaphors [52,53,54, 56]. Tabloid papers used risk-amplifying frames when presenting press releases from the WHO and health authorities across 10 European countries during the H1N1 pandemic. Conflict and damage were emphasised when disseminating the press releases [53]. Hall and Wolf [52] interviewed German public health experts and found that the participants attributed fear and panic in the public to sensationalist media coverage.

One qualitative study examining the perceptions of healthcare professionals, regional public health officers, epidemiologists and public health experts across eight European countries following the H1N1 pandemic found that they all experienced collaboration with the media as poor and that the professionals felt misunderstood [55]. The stakeholders emphasised the importance of establishing good relations between national health authorities and the media and highlighted that society’s trust in public health authorities must be improved long before a pandemic [55].

Overall, the evidence describes framing as an important topic in studies of mass media; however, no studies were identified regarding the effect of framing and public responses. Furthermore, no studies reported on outcomes related to health authorities’ communication with the public through radio.

Government and health authorities’ websites

Six of the included studies on government websites assessed readability using multiple validated indexes, accessibility, and timeliness of the updates on the websites [57,58,59,60,61,62]. Cross-sectional Google search studies of the readability of websites found that websites of the UK Health Service government [57] and online educational articles on COVID-19 on websites related to governments, hospitals and health organisations (e.g., WHO) [58] provided information related to COVID-19 that was too difficult for the general population to read. A prospective study of the websites of the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention during the first months of the H1N1 pandemic classified the documents according to their intended audience and their reading level. The study analysed the material for readability using instruments to assess the suitability of materials, which take into account criteria such as layout, typography, graphics, and surrounding context and its accessibility to an intended audience. The authors concluded that while the webpages were adequately adapted to the reading level of the intended audience, the format and layout (i.e., text-heavy and densely formatted) made the material difficult to comprehend [59]. A study of the accessibility of information on the WHO website found that the information was not accessible for the elderly because the website lacked non-text alternative content for people with vision problems [60]. The lack of timely updated information on webpages has been reported as particularly problematic at the local governmental level in China during the COVID-19 pandemic [62] and during the H1N1 pandemic in the U.S. [61].

The evidence suggests that adaptations for the reading level of the audience alone do not determine information accessibility. The readability, layout, format, accessibility and timeliness of the updates are additional variables that have been studied in assessing the quality of health authorities’ and governmental websites.

Social media platforms

Eight of the included studies on the social media platform Twitter and the Chinese microblogging website Sina Weibo reported on message content (i.e., framing), engagement (i.e., message retransmission and number of likes) and misinformation [15, 16, 63,64,65,66,67,68]. Sutton et al. [63] conducted an analysis of officials’ communication on Twitter and found that retweeting (i.e., reposting or forwarding) of messages is influenced by message content, message features, the organisational type of the account, the number of followers of an account and the time and day the message was sent. Messages were most likely to be retweeted if they contained content related to surveillance, technical information, self-efficacy, or collective efficacy or symptoms. Messages that included video were retransmitted 63% more often than those without video [63]. In a textual analysis of Twitter messages from U.S. public health agencies’ accounts during the first 60 days of the COVID-19 pandemic, Sutton et al. [15] found that the content changed from focusing on instructive messages in the first phase to motivational messages to sustain action in the long term, which focused on the need to protect vulnerable groups.

Five studies explored factors affecting lack of viewer engagement with health authorities’ social media posts related to COVID-19. Kamiński et al. [64] conducted an infodemiology study (an area of science concerning contributed content on the internet) of Twitter messages published by health authorities across the globe. They found that health authorities often used negative framing in Twitter messages on pandemic information compared to celebrities and politicians, who posted optimistic messages that were associated with higher viewer engagement. However, while more positive posts from health authorities may help to enhance engagement, it may also undermine the perceived seriousness of the message. The authors argued for collaboration between health authorities and opinion leaders to succeed in pandemic risk communication [64]. An information dissemination study of U.S. health authorities’ Twitter messages identified inconsistencies and incongruences in messages over time related to critical topics in the communication of COVID-19 information. However, the study did not report on the effect of message inconsistency and people’s self-protective behaviour [65]. Studies of posts on the Sina Weibo platform found low engagement with COVID-19 posts from the Chinese government [16, 66, 67]. The studies suggested that the lack of engagement was related to a failure to use media richness properly [66] and the use of nonpersonal and nonnarrative content [16, 67].

The evidence suggests that health authorities are faced with low engagement with their posts related to pandemic health risk information on social media. Messages containing narratives and a degree of self-efficacy are among the topics that affect engagement on social media platforms Twitter and Sina Weibo. No studies on health authorities’ use of social media in pandemic health risk communication in relation to effects on self-protective behaviour were identified.

YouTube videos

Five of the included studies explored the use of the world’s largest video-sharing platform, YouTube [69,70,71,72,73]. Complementing traditional television, online video has become increasingly important. However, videos from credible governmental sources and the WHO are highly underrepresented on YouTube, leaving YouTube vulnerable to the spread of misinformation [69,70,71]. Li et al. [69] found that 25% of the most popular videos related to COVID-19, which elicited more than 62 million views worldwide, contained misleading content. While governmental videos contained factual information and more accurate COVID-19-specific information, they accounted for only 11% of the videos and 10% of the views [69]. COVID-19-related videos with misleading content were found to have a higher percentage of views on YouTube than those from credible sources [70]. Across nations, the few videos from government and health agencies on COVID-19-related information on YouTube were often found to be credible, but they received a low number of likes and comments [72] and represented a low share of their videos [70]. Two issues that may explain the lack of social media “reach” by health authorities on YouTube are YouTube’s heavy reliance on the English language (videos) and the fact that public agencies tend to have low numbers of subscribers to their YouTube channels [70].

The evidence based on these studies [69,70,71,72] suggests that YouTube is a source of misinformation for COVID-19, and health authorities’ videos on YouTube are underrepresented and have low engagement. No studies on health authorities’ use of YouTube videos in pandemic health risk communication that also reported on their effect on self-protective behaviour were identified.

Source credibility

Source credibility describes key topics related to perceived trust in formal information sources (health authorities, governments, and public health care professionals) related to pandemic health risk information and the impact on self-protective behaviour across media types. Evidence related to governmental approaches to create trust and tailor pandemic health risk communication with ethnic minority groups is described.

Trust in formal sources

Ten of the included studies reported data on trust in formal sources, i.e., government sources and health care professionals [37, 41, 46,47,48, 50, 51, 56, 68, 75]. Trust in formal governmental sources has been associated with more accurate pandemic risk knowledge and self-protective behaviour [46, 47]. In a survey study of Hong Kong adults, trust in formal sources from the government about influenza was associated with greater reported understanding of the causes of H1N1 and more self-reported hand hygiene among males than among those who trusted informal and interpersonal information sources [46]. In a survey study of the US population, trust in governmental sources was positively associated with accurate disease knowledge related to COVID-19. Younger people expressed higher trust in informal sources, such as the CNN news channel and social networks (e.g., Twitter), than US governmental sources, while the older population expressed higher trust in US governmental sources. The authors argue that younger people’s high level of trust in informal sources makes them vulnerable to misinformation and false news [47].

However, credible sources may differ from the sources that are used most often by people. Jardine et al. [37] found that Canadian people used mass media and friends as information sources about the H1N1 pandemic but found public healthcare professionals to be the most credible sources [37]. King et al. [48] found that despite its lack of trustworthiness, the Australian mass media was the most important source of health risk information for parents during the H1N1 pandemic. Medical doctors, authoritative hospitals and government websites, in contrast, were perceived as most trustworthy.

A survey study of a sample of Slovenian adults by Lep et al. [41] found that medical professionals and scientists were perceived as most credible but that news portals, television news and social media were the most used information sources related to COVID-19.

Two qualitative studies reported on failures in governmental risk communication in China [51, 68]. According to Zhang et al. [51], Chinese authorities failed to disclose their uncertainties during the early Wuhan COVID-19 outbreak, which the authors argue undermined their institutional trust. In another qualitative study, Zhang et al. [68] explored the spread of misinformation related to the effect of a Chinese herb on COVID-19 prevention. A message was delivered by one official health expert through the Chinese authoritative media, which affected the perception of the credibility of the message. Social media shared the message using titles inconsistent with the facts, which eventually led to rumours and hoarding behaviour [68].

The evidence describes both positive and negative behavioural outcomes of health risk information from formal sources. However, while the evidence indicates that trust in formal sources is associated with increased knowledge and self-protective behaviour, the studies lacked controls for education level, which may moderate the relationship between gender and age differences and knowledge outcomes. The evidence suggests a disconnect between the information sources people use and find credible and those they report using for pandemic health risk information.

Trust in formal spokespersons

Corresponding with the literature review by the WHO on risk communication in public health emergencies [12], five of the included studies found that messages related to pandemic health risk were perceived as most trustworthy when they came from a healthcare professional [37, 41, 48, 56, 75]. An RCT assigned Japanese participants to receive intervention messages from different sources. This study found that health care professionals’ written persuasive messages encouraging people to stay home were more effective than messages from the governor, patients or a public health expert or a control message [75]. Luth et al. [56] conducted a content analysis of television news clips and found that one of the reasons why the Canadian government lost control of its message in mass media was related to the choice of governmental spokespersons in press conferences. When Canadian government officials appeared on television in press conferences with a visual identity as bureaucrats and politicians, they decreased the credibility of their message [56].

Two studies suggest that the credibility of governmental spokespersons and governmental trust are highly related to the evaluation of crisis management [50, 51]. A survey study of public perception of the Israeli government’s emergency management regarding COVID-19 found that the higher the public’s trust and evaluation of crisis management were, the greater the public’s compliance with guidelines. Those who perceived that the prime minister was the most credible spokesperson had a high evaluation of crisis management, whereas those with low evaluation of crisis management trusted other sources of information, such as infectious disease specialists, ministry of health websites and scientific articles [50]. Governmental trust was found to be reduced in China due to the perception that officials had concealed information about the COVID-19 outbreak in Wuhan. This led to reduced government credibility and increased spread of information from unofficial sources, conspiracy theories, and rumours [51]. The evidence suggests that while health care professionals are often perceived as a credible source, trust in governmental sources may not be stable over time. However, no longitudinal studies were identified that studied the variability in governmental trust and media types over time. Although the evidence indicates that trust in healthcare professionals is high, independent of media type, no studies were identified that examined the impact of different media types on people’s trust in formal sources.

Tailored communication with ethnic minority groups

Health authorities are advised to collaborate with communities to ensure that their concerns and information needs are understood and to tailor advice and messages to address the target groups [12, 13, 18, 82]. Only three of the included studies reported data on modes of communication and ethnic minority groups [78,79,80]. One qualitative study with the Latino community in the U.S. found that all respondents wanted more personalised COVID-19 information from researchers or health professionals via communication channels such as personal email, text messaging or a group communication method (e.g., video group call, Facebook group) [80]. A Swedish policy analysis described how the government and decision makers relied on migrant community leaders, representatives of migrant associations, religious leaders, and other influencers to reach out with culturally sensitive information [78]. In a qualitative focus group study of communication priorities for vaccination to indigenous people in Canada, Driedger et al. found [79] that risk messages were transmitted in different dialects and by different formats (radio, television, print, online, community sessions). An additional issue was the need to tailor not only the communication mode but also the message for ethnic minority groups and migrant populations. In a study of indigenous Canadian people, the authors concluded that the communication failed to engender behaviour change because the target group did not understand why they were prioritised for vaccines, instead believing that the government was using them as “guinea pigs” [79].

The evidence from the included studies suggests that ethnic minority groups and migrant populations need personalised information and trusted spokespersons. The review did not identify studies of effective modes of communication with ethnic minority groups.

How the message is communicated

How the message is communicated reflects the outcomes related to how key topics, including the use of narratives and jargon, emotional valence, and multimodal information, affect health authorities’ pandemic health risk messages.

Narratives

According to Slovic [83], people tend to consider their feelings to guide their decision making and judgements, and the use of narratives has been explored in the risk communication literature [14]. Six of the included studies explored how the narrative tone of communications plays a role in the way health authorities’ messages are interpreted and acted upon during pandemics [16, 49, 66, 67, 73, 76]. The choice of narratives has been found to affect uncertainty and anxiety in the public [49], pandemic knowledge [73], engagement [16, 66, 67] and self-protective behaviour (stay home messages) [76]. Liu et al. [49] found that messages negatively affected people’s anxiety when hospitals reported a need for monetary donations, as this reflected a shortage of beds in hospitals. People experienced less anxiety when the government reported opening schools in China, as this provided them with hope [49]. An experimental study found that people reported greater knowledge and greater pandemic influenza prevention measures when they viewed non-narrative health videos about pandemic influenza compared to those who viewed narrative videos [73]. Two infodemiology studies of the Chinese social media platform Sina Weibo found that people were more engaged (more comments and likes) with narrative posts than non-narrative posts, both from the government and from personal sources [16, 67]. Posts related to new evidence and a nonnarrative style were strong negative predictors of the number of shares [16].

A focus group study of English residents found that factual, evidence-based messages related to pandemic influenza from health authorities were most convincing, particularly those that included cost-benefit comparisons of H1N1 vaccination safety. Messages that aimed to elicit feelings of anticipated regret for not getting vaccinated were generally perceived as patronising and unprofessional [76]. Another content analysis of the social media platform Sina Weibo found that posts with high media richness (both text and video) and positive emotions increased citizens’ engagement regarding government social media, while posts high in media richness and negative emotions attenuated citizens’ engagement. Consequently, the authors concluded that plain text should be used when sharing posts that elicit negative emotions, while video should be attached to text when reporting content eliciting positive emotions [66].

The evidence indicates that the use of narratives and messages eliciting emotions in health authorities’ pandemic health risk communication leads to engagement with the public, while people learn more from non-narrative messages than narrative messages. The evidence also suggests that negative narratives may negatively affect people’s emotions.

Use of jargon

Four of the included studies reported on outcomes related to the use of jargon in health authorities’ health risk communication [57, 59, 68, 77]. In terms of messages and wording in pandemic health risk communication, the WHO recommends avoiding the use of jargon [12]. However, an experimental study on health messages from health agencies in the context of COVID-19 found no negative effect of jargon on how much difficulty people experience in understanding a message (processing fluency), which may relate to people’s greater motivation to process information that includes jargon when urgent and risky concerns are communicated [77]. Two studies of readability emphasised the importance of adjusting written material to the intended audience. Khan et al. [57] found that the use of jargon and long sentences were contributing factors to the purportedly low readability of UK health authority websites. In a prospective study of the U.S. health authorities’ CDC guidance documents, Legasse` et al. [59] found that while the use of scientific jargon was adjusted to the reading level of different audiences, the documents were still difficult to comprehend due to suboptimal layout (i.e., text-heavy and densely formatted) [59]. The use of jargon in a news report from the Chinese government has also been explored as a contributor to the misinformation and development of rumours in a qualitative study [68].

The sparse evidence based on these studies [57, 59, 68, 77] emphasises that the use of jargon in pandemic health risk messages is multifaceted. Studies report on variables related to the reading level of the audience, misunderstandings, and the recipient’s motivation to process the information.

Graphic and audio-visual means

Whether a message is communicated visually through graphics, graphs, moving images, colours, symbols or by the use of text or numbers can have a significant impact on its reach and how the message is received. According to WHO guidelines, engaging and effective risk communication is multimodal and includes visual information [12]. Three included studies explored the impact of graphic and audio-visual means [17, 56, 74]. The understanding of exponential growth, for example, which is a key concept during COVID-19, is interpreted differently when communicated through numbers as opposed to visually. A quasi-experimental study with 1980 participants from 43 countries found that people tend to linearise exponential functions when assessing them intuitively by looking at graphs and underestimate future values based on the current value. The study found that showing people prior data in raw numbers before showing them graphs causally reduced exponential-growth prediction bias, which affected risk perception and was associated with improved safety compliance with the WHO’s COVID-19 recommendations [74].

According to Luth et al. [56], public health agencies also need to be aware of the emotional valence and the effect of colours and symbolism in visual media, as they can be used to encourage positive health behaviours or elicit panic. In a content analysis of Canadian television news of the H1N1 pandemic, Luth et al. [56] found the use of static information screens in conjunction with auditive messages that used colours (red) and symbols (technical, alien objects), which may contribute to negative valence [56]. Luth et al. [56] also emphasised the importance of matching news footage with the public health message delivered. Luth et al. [56] found a mismatch between the visual content and the audio content in the presentation of official priority groups for vaccination. Visual footage showed seemingly healthy nonpriority individuals in line to be vaccinated, but the audio targeted vulnerable groups [56].

The perception of colour may also vary depending on the audience and context. A study of an information leaflet used by the Slovak Ministry of Health in communication about the H1N1 pandemic found that the use of white and red types on a dark blue background was preferred by most primary school students. This combination of colours, however, was perceived negatively by more than half of healthcare professionals, presumably because of differences in ageing and different colour perceptions and preferences in children and adults [17]. The authors argue that reading literacy and age are important factors to take into account when tailoring information in health information leaflets [17]. The limited evidence related to graphic and audio-visual means [17, 56, 74] suggests that different modalities affect the message in a multifaceted way. Visual and multimedia messages are interpreted differently than text- or number-based messages. Symbolism, colour perception and preferences change according to audiences and contexts. While the effect and importance of creative and audio-visual production and means is an important area for pandemic health risk communication, no studies of creative communication choices or evaluations/comparisons of various visualisation techniques relating to graphic and audio-visual communication were identified.

Discussion

Key topics identified and implications for research and practice

Our review provides novel insights into topics regarding different modes of communication used by health authorities in health risk communication with the public during a pandemic. We found that key topics identified in the health risk communication literature, such as trust, dissemination through multiple channels, framing, narratives, self-efficacy, the use of jargon, health literacy, misinformation, tailored risk communication, multimodal information, and emotional valence, remain relevant to health authorities’ health risk communication during pandemics [12,13,14, 29].

Communication channels

Consistent with other reviews, this review found many studies reporting that people receive information from multiple channels [12, 84]. The studies suggest that people are not passive receivers of health authorities’ information, a wide spectrum of communication channels is used, and meaning making is influenced by both informal and formal sources, as highlighted in the literature [85]. Hence, health authorities are advised to disseminate information through multiple channels, including social media [12, 84]. However, health authorities’ rapid message dissemination on social media suggests inconsistency, which could be a source of confusion for the public [37, 65]. According to Jardine et al. [37], receiving information from multiple sources is only better if the additional information sources contribute to improved informed decision making, less confusion and strengthened credibility. The review findings also emphasise that misinformation is a significant problem across social media sites related to pandemic risk communication. This sometimes results in confusion and even panic for the public [23, 26, 27]. A major challenge for health authorities during pandemics has been to combat the infodemic, the overabundance of information, by identifying and addressing misinformation, rumours, and the contradictory information the public receives from multiple informal sources on social media sites [12, 23, 27, 86]. At the same time, there is a lack of consistency in their own messages that are disseminated rapidly and in parallel on multiple communication channels. As highlighted by Ratzan et al. [20], global health communication demands a communication strategy, particularly on social media, that ensures consistent and congruent messages to the public. Challenges for health authorities across nations are to create engagement in social media, provide the public with webpages that are accessible, updated in a timely way, and tailored towards varying reading levels and with a readable layout to ensure or avoid risk amplification of their messages delivered through mass media.

Source credibility

Social trust is “the willingness to rely on those who have the responsibility for making decisions and taking actions related to the management of technology, the environment, medicine, or other realms of public health and safety” [[87], p. 354]. Social trust in health authorities is value-based and relies on judgements of similarities in intentions and values [88]. Corresponding to the existing literature, our review findings suggest that people tend to have social trust in health care professionals as spokespersons and information sources in public health emergencies [12]. Furthermore, the current review indicates that trust in health authorities as an information source is not a static phenomenon; rather, it is highly dynamic and related to the public perception of health authorities’ crisis management [50, 51]. The evidence in the present review cannot conclude why trust in governmental sources varies. In a critical review of the literature, Siegrist [88] found that social trust varies by hazard and respondent groups, and little is known about the heuristics people rely on when evaluating hazards [88]. Factors related to why people choose informal sources of information may relate both to the respondents (e.g., attitudes, education) and to the government’s crisis management or trust in a particular spokesperson. According to Siegrist [88], there is a need to separate trust and confidence. The type of trust described in the present review is related to the concept of “confidence”. Confidence is affected by past experiences of emerging evidence, indicating that events will occur as expected. While the level of confidence in crisis management may affect risk perception and behavioural outcomes, a total loss of social trust is rarely found in response to governmental risk communication (or the lack of such) [88].

The evidence of the current review also corresponds with the literature indicating that health authorities may need to collaborate with trusted spokespersons and tailor communication methods to reach out to immigrant and ethnic populations [12, 13]. Finally, this study corresponds to the knowledge gained from research conducted on epidemics (notably Ebola, Zika and yellow fever), suggesting that efficient modes of communication vary by location and population. What works best for one population might work poorly for another [28]. Hence, health authorities’ health risk communication needs to be adaptive to the pandemic situation and the multiple receivers in the field, be responsive to its development over time, and choose credible spokespersons in media to maintain public confidence in health authorities as a source of health risk information.

The content of the message

This review identified topics related to the content of the message, such as the use of narratives, emotional valence, framing and jargon, multimodal messages and providing people with messages that support self-efficacy. Our findings correspond with previous literature findings that health authorities should use balanced and factual information based on science and evidence and should motivate self-efficacy and promote specific actions people can realistically take to protect their health [12, 82, 89]. However, while the WHO guidelines propose that messages should be jargon free and not explained in technical terms to reach the general public [12], we identified one study [77] implying that this may not apply to the COVID-19 context. Existing reviews of the literature have emphasised the impact of education and health literacy on protective behaviour during pandemic and infectious disease outbreaks [3,4,5] and concluded that individual differences in both attitudes and knowledge about risks suggest that there may be no “one size fits all” approach to risk communication [90]. The use of jargon may be a question of what works for whom.

The evidence to support the choice of message attributes (e.g., narratives, non-narratives, emotional components) in the current review is inconclusive. Although the findings in the present review suggest that people learn more from non-narrative messages, Downs [91] argues that the use of narratives in science communication may help to communicate complex information because it can provide the receiver with a context, capture attention and improve the understanding and processing of the information. According to Balog-Way et al. [14], there is no single dominant formula for risk communication. In a review of the risk communication literature, Balog-Way and colleagues conclude that the effect of single message attributes is typically contingent, indirect and cumulative, and effective risk communication requires a multifaceted approach. Furthermore, it is a matter of what works for whom. The evidence related to jargon and narratives in the current review reflects the complexity health authorities face when designing effective messages for various audiences.

Research gaps

The research gaps identified in this review are related to health authorities’ pandemic health risk communication and modes of communication. This review was limited to empirical studies of pandemics, which pertained to mainly the H1N1 pandemic and the response phase of COVID-19 (Jan through Oct 2020).

A research gap identified in this review is the lack of high-quality RCT studies to study the effect of modes of pandemic health risk communication on behavioural outcomes. Only four studies with experimental designs were identified in this review. The study by Bekalu et al. [73] has a high risk of bias, as subject flow with attrition was not assessed, and it was not reported whether the analyses were per protocol or intention to treat. The study by Banarjee et al. [74] also lacks a description of recruitment and attrition after starting the experiment; hence, the sample cannot be considered randomised. Furthermore, the quasi-experimental design has a high risk of bias. The study by Okuhara [75] lacks a proper description of the intervention given (persuasive message to stay home) and suffers from a lack of detail on recruitment or attrition and a lack of description of the analytic strategy (intention to treat vs. per protocol). The Shulman and Bullock [77] sample was gathered from “Mechanical Turk”, making recruitment selection very difficult to assess, and these authors did not discuss attrition after randomisation, intention to treat or per protocol analyses. Hence, all the experimental studies had a high risk of bias.

The lack of effectiveness studies and high-quality trials for risk communication is notable in risk and disaster communication research [92]. In a review of the effectiveness of disaster communication, Bradley et al. [92] concluded that it has been difficult to conduct RCTs in risk communication in recent years because people in the intervention and control groups are likely to share information, with a resultant significant likelihood of “contamination”. Additionally, the differences between the studies make it difficult to conclude that one method of risk communication is superior to others [92].

As the rapidly evolving knowledge related to pandemic health risk and the multiple channels used by the public impose challenges for research on health risk communication during pandemics, methodological approaches need to embrace this complexity. Studies with theory-driven approaches to risk management have analysed COVID-19 from a complexity perspective [93,94,95]. Complex adaptive systems and resilient responses in healthcare have been studied with mixed-method and multimethod case study methodologies, which are relevant to risk communication [96, 97]. Only two case studies of the crisis management/healthcare system were identified [48, 78]. Case studies are needed to gain insight into 1) health authorities’ capacities to respond to the various and complex challenges faced in risk communication through the pandemic and 2) their strategies in using different modes of communication.

Furthermore, three areas of research gaps need to be highlighted. First, we found that most studies focused on the general population or on health agencies, and only a small number of studies focused on ethnic minority groups, in accordance with existing reviews [12, 13]. Currently, there is a lack of knowledge on the modes of communication used to take into account the diversity of receivers in the field.

Second, no studies were identified that documented the effect on people’s self-protective behaviour related to health authorities’ use of social media platforms (e.g., Twitter, Facebook, Sina Weibo) and videos shared on YouTube. We identified studies on health authorities’ health risk communication on social media and outcomes pertaining to viewer engagement on YouTube and message retransmission on Twitter. However, previous studies have found that viewer engagement (number of views) is a poor predictor of usefulness, whereas the upload source and target audiences are good predictors of usefulness [98]. Future studies might benefit from studying the effectiveness of health authorities’ health risk communication on self-protective behaviour and include measures related to diverse upload sources, source credibility, and the effect of using different media types.

Third, the current review identified a limited number of studies of health authorities’ use of graphic and audio-visual means. The interdependence between graphic and audio visual means implies that modes presented together need to be interpreted with respect to one another [99]. More studies are needed on the effect of multimodal health risk communication in formal health risk communication. Health authorities’ videos were found to be credible with high quality, but they were not popular; they did not receive likes and comments, and they failed to engage the public. More studies are needed on how health authorities may include creative means in health risk communication with the public (e.g., evaluation of different types of narratives as well as creative choices in pandemic videos, evaluation of visual techniques for communicating data and numbers) without hampering their confidence and role as knowledge translators.

Limitations

The present review is based on evidence retrieved from only two search databases. The search terms used were mainly Emtree terms and MeSH terms designed to capture articles in three areas: health and risk communication, media types and modes of communication, and pandemics. This may have resulted in the omission of relevant papers using different terms. This rapid scoping review explored the evidence and highlighted the key topics, research gaps and outcomes reported but did not compare studies or draw conclusions on causal relationships across studies and does not represent a concept analysis of the identified topics.

Conclusion

The aim of this rapid scoping review was to synthesise the evidence regarding the different modes of communication used by health authorities in health risk communication with the public during a pandemic. This rapid scoping review identified studies in the interdisciplinary field of health risk communication mainly during the H1N1 pandemic and COVID-19 pandemic across three categories: communication channels, source credibility and how the message is communicated. A research gap identified in this review is the lack of high-quality RCT studies to study the effect of modes of pandemic health risk communication on behavioural outcomes. Content and engagement have been the major outcomes evaluated in social media, while effect studies on health authorities’ engagement in social media platforms (e.g., Twitter, YouTube) in pandemic health risk communication and the effect on self-protective behaviour are lacking. The evidence suggests that ethnic minority groups and migrant populations need personalised information and trusted spokespersons. However, there is a lack of studies on the modes of communication used to take into account the diversity of receivers in the field in pandemic health risk communication. More studies are needed across the fields of health risk communication and media studies (including visual communication, web design, video, and digital marketing) at a time when online digital communication is central to reaching the public.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

“WHO”:

World health organization

“COVID-19”:

Coronavirus disease of 2019

“RCT”:

Randomised controlled trial

References

  1. Porta M. A dictionary of epidemiology: USA: Oxford university press; 2014.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Baekkeskov E, Rubin O. Why pandemic response is unique: powerful experts and hands-off political leaders. Disaster Prev Manag. 2014;23(1):81–93. https://doi.org/10.1108/DPM-05-2012-0060.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Tooher R, Collins JE, Street JM, Braunack-Mayer A, Marshall H. Community knowledge, behaviours and attitudes about the 2009 H1N1 influenza pandemic: a systematic review. Influenza Other Respir Viruses. 2013;7(6):1316–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/irv.12103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Webster RK, et al. How to improve adherence with quarantine: rapid review of the evidence. Public Health. 2020;163-9.

  5. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pandemic: a review. Br J Health Psychol. 2010;15(4):797–824. https://doi.org/10.1348/135910710X485826.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Kelly H. The classical definition of a pandemic is not elusive. Bull World Health Organ. 2011;89(7):540–1. https://doi.org/10.2471/BLT.11.088815.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Abraham T. Lessons from the pandemic: the need for new tools for risk and outbreak communication. Emerg Health Threats J. 2011;4(1):7160. https://doi.org/10.3402/ehtj.v4i0.7160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Nicola M, Alsafi Z, Sohrabi C, Kerwan A, al-Jabir A, Iosifidis C, et al. The socio-economic implications of the coronavirus and COVID-19 pandemic: a review. Int J Surg. 2020;78:185–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2020.04.018.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. del Rio C, Collins LF, Malani P. Long-term health consequences of COVID-19. JAMA. 2020;324(17):1723–4. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2020.19719.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Xiong J, Lipsitz O, Nasri F, Lui LMW, Gill H, Phan L, et al. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2020;277(1):55–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. de Lima CVC, Cândido EL, da Silva JA, Albuquerque LV, Soares LM, do Nascimento MM, et al. Effects of quarantine on mental health of populations affected by Covid-19. J Affect Disord. 2020;275:253–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.063.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Organization, W.H. Communicating risk in public health emergencies: a WHO guideline for emergency risk communication (ERC) policy and practice. Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2017.

  13. Vaughan E, Tinker T. Effective health risk communication about pandemic influenza for vulnerable populations. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(S2):S324–32. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.162537.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Balog-Way D, McComas K, Besley J. The evolving field of risk communication. Risk Anal. 2020;40(S1):2240–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13615.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Sutton J, Renshaw SL, Butts CT. The first 60 days: American public health Agencies' social media strategies in the emerging COVID-19 pandemic. Health security. 2020;18(6):454–60. https://doi.org/10.1089/hs.2020.0105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ngai CSB, Singh RG, Lu W, Koon AC. Grappling with the COVID-19 health crisis: content analysis of communication strategies and their effects on public engagement on social media. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(8):e21360. https://doi.org/10.2196/21360.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Krajcovic A, et al. Effectiveness of the information leaflet personal measures during pandemic flu a (H1N1) issued by the Ministry of Health of the Slovak Republic during the 2009/2010 flu pandemic. Acta Medica Martiniana. 2015;5(1):36–44.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Barrelet C, Bourrier M, Burton-Jeangros C, Schindler M. Unresolved issues in risk communication research: the case of the H1N1 pandemic (2009–2011). Influenza Other Respir Viruses. 2013;7(2):114–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/irv.12090.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Schiavo R. Health communication: From theory to practice, vol. 217. USA: Wiley; 2013.

  20. Ratzan SC, Sommarivac S, Rauh L. Enhancing global health communication during a crisis: lessons from the COVID-19 pandemic. Public Health Res Pract. 2020;30(2). https://doi.org/10.17061/phrp3022010.

  21. Elleström L. The Modalities of Media II: An Expanded Model for Understanding Intermedial Relations, in Beyond Media Borders, vol. 1. Cham: Palgrave Macmillan; 2020. p. 3–91.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Gamhewage G. An introduction to risk communication: World Health Organization; 2014. p. 1–6.

  23. Tang L, Bie B, Park SE, Zhi D. Social media and outbreaks of emerging infectious diseases: a systematic review of literature. Am J Infect Control. 2018;46(9):962–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2018.02.010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Klemm C, Das E, Hartmann T. Swine flu and hype: a systematic review of media dramatization of the H1N1 influenza pandemic. J Risk Res. 2016;19(1):1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2014.923029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Budd J, Miller BS, Manning EM, Lampos V, Zhuang M, Edelstein M, et al. Digital technologies in the public-health response to COVID-19. Nat Med. 2020;26(8):1183–92. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-020-1011-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kullar R, et al. To tweet or not to tweet—a review of the viral power of twitter for infectious diseases. Curr Infect Dis Rep. 2020;22:1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Srivastava KC, et al. Facade of media and social media during covid-19: A review. Int J Res Pharm Sci. 2020;11(Special Issue 1).

  28. Toppenberg-Pejcic D, Noyes J, Allen T, Alexander N, Vanderford M, Gamhewage G. Emergency risk communication: lessons learned from a rapid review of recent gray literature on Ebola, Zika, and yellow fever. Health Commun. 2019;34(4):437–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2017.1405488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Gesser-Edelsburg A, Stolero N, Mordini E, Billingsley M, James JJ, Green MS. Emerging infectious disease (EID) communication during the 2009 H1N1 influenza outbreak: literature review (2009-2013) of the methodology used for EID communication analysis. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2015;9(2):199–206. https://doi.org/10.1017/dmp.2014.126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. González-Padilla DA, Tortolero-Blanco L. Social media influence in the COVID-19 pandemic. Int Braz J Urol. 2020;46(suppl 1):120–4. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1677-5538.ibju.2020.s121.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Anwar A, Malik M, Raees V, Anwar A. Role of mass media and public health communications in the COVID-19 pandemic. Cureus. 2020;12(9):e10453. https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.10453.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O'Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169(7):467–73. https://doi.org/10.7326/M18-0850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Peters MD, et al. Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2015;13(3):141–6. https://doi.org/10.1097/XEB.0000000000000050.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Tricco AC, et al. Rapid reviews to strengthen health policy and systems: a practical guide. Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2017.

  35. Mayring P. Qualitative Content Analysis: Theoretical Background and Procedures. In: Bikner-Ahsbahs A, Knipping C, Presmeg N, editors. Approaches to Qualitative Research in Mathematics Education. Advances in Mathematics Education. Dordrecht: Springer; 2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9181-6_13.

  36. Lin L, et al. Media use and communication inequalities in a public health emergency: a case study of 2009–2010 pandemic influenza A virus subtype H1N1. Public Health Rep. 2014;129(6_suppl4):49–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Jardine CG, Boerner FU, Boyd AD, Driedger SM. The more the better? A comparison of the information sources used by the public during two infectious disease outbreaks. PLoS One. 2015;10(10):e0140028. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140028.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Al-Hasan A, Yim D, Khuntia J. Citizens' adherence to COVID-19 mitigation recommendations by the government: a three-country comparative evaluation using online cross-sectional survey data. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(8):e20634. https://doi.org/10.2196/20634.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Alanezi F, et al. Implications of public understanding of COVID-19 in Saudi Arabia for fostering effective communication through awareness framework. Front Public Health. 2020;8(494).

  40. Ali SH, Foreman J, Tozan Y, Capasso A, Jones AM, DiClemente RJ. Trends and predictors of COVID-19 information sources and their relationship with knowledge and beliefs related to the pandemic: Nationwide cross-sectional study. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2020;6(4):e21071. https://doi.org/10.2196/21071.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Lep Ž, Babnik K, Hacin Beyazoglu K. Emotional responses and self-protective behavior within days of the COVID-19 outbreak: the promoting role of information credibility. Front Psychol. 2020;11(1846).

  42. Meier K, Glatz T, Guijt MC, Piccininni M, van der Meulen M, Atmar K, et al. Public perspectives on protective measures during the COVID-19 pandemic in the Netherlands, Germany and Italy: a survey study. PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0236917. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236917.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Parsons Leigh J, Fiest K, Brundin-Mather R, Plotnikoff K, Soo A, Sypes EE, et al. A national cross-sectional survey of public perceptions of the COVID-19 pandemic: self-reported beliefs, knowledge, and behaviors. PLoS One. 2020;15(10):e0241259. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241259.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Reddy SP, et al. South Africans’ understanding of and response to the COVID-19 outbreak: an online survey. S Afr Med J. 2020;110(9):894–902.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Riiser K, Helseth S, Haraldstad K, Torbjørnsen A, Richardsen KR. Adolescents’ health literacy, health protective measures, and health-related quality of life during the Covid-19 pandemic. PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0238161. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238161.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Liao Q, Cowling B, Lam WT, Ng MW, Fielding R. Situational awareness and health protective Responses to pandemic influenza a (H1N1) in Hong Kong: a cross-sectional study. PLoS One. 2010;5(10):e13350. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013350.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Fridman I, Lucas N, Henke D, Zigler CK. Association between public knowledge about COVID-19, trust in information sources, and adherence to social distancing: cross-sectional survey. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2020;6(3):e22060. https://doi.org/10.2196/22060.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. King CL, Chow MYK, Wiley KE, Leask J. Much ado about flu: a mixed methods study of parental perceptions, trust and information seeking in a pandemic. Influenza Other Respir Viruses. 2018;12(4):514–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/irv.12547.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Liu M, Zhang H, Huang H. Media exposure to COVID-19 information, risk perception, social and geographical proximity, and self-rated anxiety in China. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Gesser-Edelsburg A, Cohen R, Hijazi R, Abed Elhadi Shahbari N. Analysis of public perception of the Israeli Government’s early emergency instructions regarding COVID-19: online survey study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(5):e19370. https://doi.org/10.2196/19370.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Zhang L, Li H, Chen K. Effective risk communication for public health emergency: reflection on the COVID-19 (2019-nCoV) outbreak in Wuhan, China. Healthcare. 2020;8(1):64. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare8010064.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Hall K, Wolf M. Whose crisis? Pandemic flu, ‘communication disasters’ and the struggle for hegemony. Health. 25(3):322–38.

  53. Rossmann C, Meyer L, Schulz PJ. The mediated amplification of a crisis: communicating the a/H1N1 pandemic in press releases and press coverage in Europe. Risk Anal. 2018;38(2):357–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12841.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Basnyat I, Lee ST. Framing of influenza a (H1N1) pandemic in a Singaporean newspaper. Health Promot Int. 2015;30(4):942–53. https://doi.org/10.1093/heapro/dau028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Cloes R, Ahmad A, Reintjes R. Risk communication during the 2009 influenza a (H1N1) pandemic: stakeholder experiences from eight European countries. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2015;9(2):127–33. https://doi.org/10.1017/dmp.2014.124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Luth W, Jardine C, Bubela T. When pictures waste a thousand words: analysis of the 2009 H1N1 pandemic on television news. PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e64070. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064070.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Khan S, Asif A, Jaffery AE. Language in a time of COVID-19: literacy Bias ethnic minorities face during COVID-19 from online information in the UK. J Racial Ethnic Health Disparities. 2020;1-7. 

  58. Szmuda T, Özdemir C, Ali S, Singh A, Syed MT, Słoniewski P. Readability of online patient education material for the novel coronavirus disease (COVID-19): a cross-sectional health literacy study. Public Health. 2020;185:21–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2020.05.041.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Lagassé LP, Rimal RN, Smith KC, Storey JD, Rhoades E, Barnett DJ, et al. How accessible was information about H1N1 flu? Literacy assessments of CDC guidance documents for different audiences. PLoS One. 2011;6(10):e23583. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023583.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Fernández-Díaz E, Iglesias-Sánchez PP, Jambrino-Maldonado C. Exploring WHO communication during the COVID 19 pandemic through the WHO website based on W3C guidelines: accessible for all? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(16):5663. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17165663.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Ringel JS, Trentacost E, Lurie N. How well did health departments communicate about risk at the start of the swine flu epidemic in 2009? Health Aff. 2009;28(Supplement 1):w743–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Hu G, Li P, Yuan C, Tao C, Wen H, Liu Q, et al. Information disclosure during the COVID-19 epidemic in China: City-level observational study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(8):e19572. https://doi.org/10.2196/19572.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Sutton J, Renshaw SL, Butts CT. COVID-19: retransmission of official communications in an emerging pandemic. PLoS One. 2020;15(9):e0238491. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238491.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Kamiński M, Szymańska C, Nowak JK. Whose tweets on COVID-19 gain the Most attention: celebrities, political, or scientific authorities? Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2021;24(2):123–8. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2020.0336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Wang Y, Hao H, Platt LS. Examining risk and crisis communications of government agencies and stakeholders during early-stages of COVID-19 on twitter. Comput Hum Behav. 2020;114:106568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Chen Q, Min C, Zhang W, Wang G, Ma X, Evans R. Unpacking the black box: How to promote citizen engagement through government social media during the COVID-19 crisis. Comput Hum Behav. 2020;110:106380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Liao Q, Yuan J, Dong M, Yang L, Fielding R, Lam WWT. Public engagement and government responsiveness in the communications about COVID-19 during the early epidemic stage in China: Infodemiology study on social media data. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(5):e18796. https://doi.org/10.2196/18796.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Zhang L, Chen K, Jiang H, Zhao J. How the health rumor misleads People’s perception in a public health emergency: lessons from a purchase craze during the COVID-19 outbreak in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(19):7213. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17197213.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Li HO-Y, Bailey A, Huynh D, Chan J. YouTube as a source of information on COVID-19: a pandemic of misinformation? BMJ Glob Health. 2020;5(5):e002604. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjgh-2020-002604.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Dutta A, Beriwal N, van Breugel L, Sachdeva S, Barman B, Saikia H, et al. YouTube as a source of medical and epidemiological information during COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional study of content across six languages around the globe. Cureus. 2020;12(6):e8622. https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.8622.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. D’Souza RS, D’Souza S, Strand N, Anderson A, Vogt MNP, Olatoye O. YouTube as a source of medical information on the novel coronavirus 2019 disease (COVID-19) pandemic. Glob Public Health. 2020;15(7):935–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/17441692.2020.1761426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Moon H, Lee GH. Evaluation of Korean-language COVID-19–related medical information on YouTube: cross-sectional Infodemiology study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(8):e20775. https://doi.org/10.2196/20775.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Bekalu MA, Bigman CA, McCloud RF, Lin LK, Viswanath K. The relative persuasiveness of narrative versus non-narrative health messages in public health emergency communication: evidence from a field experiment. Prev Med. 2018;111:284–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.11.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Banerjee R, Bhattacharya J, Majumdar P. Exponential-growth prediction bias and compliance with safety measures related to COVID-19. Soc Sci Med. 2021;268:113473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Okuhara T, Okada H, Kiuchi T. Examining persuasive message type to encourage staying at home during the COVID-19 pandemic and social lockdown: a randomized controlled study in Japan. Patient Educ Couns. 2020;103(12):2588–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2020.08.016.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Mowbray F, Marcu A, Godinho CA, Michie S, Yardley L. Communicating to increase public uptake of pandemic flu vaccination in the UK: which messages work? Vaccine. 2016;34(28):3268–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2016.05.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Shulman HC, Bullock OM. Don’t dumb it down: the effects of jargon in COVID-19 crisis communication. PLoS One. 2020;15(10):e0239524. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239524.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Kavaliunas A, Ocaya P, Mumper J, Lindfeldt I, Kyhlstedt M. Swedish policy analysis for Covid-19. Health Policy Technol. 2020;9(4):598–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hlpt.2020.08.009.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Driedger SM, Cooper E, Jardine C, Furgal C, Bartlett J. Communicating risk to Aboriginal peoples: first nations and Metis Responses to H1N1 risk messages. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e71106. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Moyce S, Velazquez M, Claudio D, Thompson S, Metcalf M, Aghbashian E, et al. Exploring a rural Latino community’s perception of the COVID-19 pandemic. Ethn Health. 2021;26(1):126–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/13557858.2020.1838456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Nisbet MC. Framing, the media, and risk communication in policy debates. Newbury Park: Sage handbook of risk communication; 2014: p. 216–27.

  82. Tangcharoensathien V, Calleja N, Nguyen T, Purnat T, D’Agostino M, Garcia-Saiso S, et al. Framework for managing the COVID-19 infodemic: methods and results of an online, crowdsourced WHO technical consultation. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(6):e19659. https://doi.org/10.2196/19659.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Slovic P. The feeling of risk: New perspectives on risk perception. London/New York: Routledge; 2010.

  84. Fitzpatrick-Lewis D, Yost J, Ciliska D, Krishnaratne S. Communication about environmental health risks: a systematic review. Environ Health. 2010;9(1):67. https://doi.org/10.1186/1476-069X-9-67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Neeley L. In: Árvai J, Rivers L, editors. Risk communication in social media, in effective risk communication: Canada/USA: Routledge Abingdon; 2014.

  86. Eysenbach G. How to fight an infodemic: the four pillars of infodemic management. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(6):e21820. https://doi.org/10.2196/21820.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Siegrist M, Cvetkovich G, Roth C. Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk/benefit perception. Risk Anal. 2000;20(3):353–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/0272-4332.203034.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Siegrist M. Trust and risk perception: a critical review of the literature. Risk Anal. 2021;41(3):480–90. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Pidgeon N. Engaging publics about environmental and technology risks: frames, values and deliberation. J Risk Res. 2021;24(1):28–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1749118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Bier VM. On the state of the art: risk communication to the public. Reliability Eng Syst Saf. 2001;71(2):139–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0951-8320(00)00090-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Downs JS. Prescriptive scientific narratives for communicating usable science. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2014;111(Supplement 4):13627–33.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  92. Bradley DT, McFarland M, Clarke M. "The effectiveness of disaster risk communication: a systematic review of intervention studies." Effective Communication During Disasters. 2016;81-120.

  93. Braithwaite J, et al. Int J Qual Health Care. 2021;33(1):–The 40 health systems, COVID-19 (40HS, C-19) study. mzaa113

  94. Clay-Williams R, Rapport F, Braithwaite J. The Australian health system response to COVID-19 from a resilient health care perspective: what have we learned? Public Health Res Pract. 2020;30(4):e3042025.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Saurin TA. A complexity thinking account of the COVID-19 pandemic: implications for systems-oriented safety management. Saf Sci. 2012;134:105087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Iflaifel M, et al. Resilient health care: a systematic review of conceptualisations, study methods and factors that develop resilience. BMC Health Serv Res. 2020;20:1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Berg SH, Akerjordet K, Ekstedt M, Aase K. Methodological strategies in resilient health care studies: an integrative review. Saf Sci. 2018;110:300–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2018.08.025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Kumar N, Pandey A, Venkatraman A, Garg N. Are video sharing web sites a useful source of information on hypertension? J Am Soc Hypertens. 2014;8(7):481–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jash.2014.05.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Kress G, Van Leeuwen T. Multimodal discourse. The Modes and Media of Contemporary Communication. London: Bloomsbury Acad; 2010.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We want to acknowledge the Trond Mohn Foundation for providing funding for this study and the COVCOM user panel for providing feedback on our work in progress.

Funding

The COVID communication: Fighting a pandemic through translating science (COVCOM) project has received funding from the Trond Mohn Foundation under grant agreement number TMS2020TMT10 and the University of Stavanger. JKO is supported by the National Institute for Health Research Yorkshire and Humber Patient Safety Translational Research Centre (NIHR Yorkshire and Humber PSTRC). The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JR, SW, KKB and HT had the main idea for the study. All authors devised the search strategy and eligibility criteria. SHB conducted the database searches, the exclusion of studies, and the eligibility assessment of the full-text articles, extracted data and drafted the synthesis of results. SHB, JKOH, SW and MTS analysed the data. KKB validated the Table 2 content. MTS designed Fig. 2. SHB drafted the initial manuscript, and all authors provided critical revision of intellectual content. All authors approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

This study was conducted by an interdisciplinary team of researchers across health and risk communication (JKOH, SW, SHB), science communication (JR), media studies (HT, JR, MT) and experimental research (DAL, KB, JR).

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Siv Hilde Berg.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Search strategy.

Additional file 2.

Screening questions, eligibility criteria and data charting.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Berg, S.H., O’Hara, J.K., Shortt, M.T. et al. Health authorities’ health risk communication with the public during pandemics: a rapid scoping review. BMC Public Health 21, 1401 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11468-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11468-3

Keywords