Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Effects of financial incentives on motivating physical activity among older adults: results from a discrete choice experiment

Abstract

Background

There is extensive evidence that regular physical activity confers numerous health benefits. Despite this, high rates of physical inactivity prevail among older adults. This study aimed to ascertain if incentives could be effective in motivating physical activity through improving uptake of walking programs, either with or without an enrolment fee to cover corresponding costs.

Methods

A discrete-choice conjoint survey was fielded to a national sample of older adults in Singapore. Each respondent was given ten pairs of hypothetical walking programs and asked to choose the option they preferred. Each option varied along several dimensions, including the level and type (cash, voucher, or health savings credit) of incentive and an enrolment fee. For each option, they were asked how likely they would be to join their preferred program. A random utility model (RUM) was used to analyze the responses.

Results

Results suggest that a free 6-month program with a $500 cash incentive would generate enrolment rates of 58.5%; charging $50 to enroll lowers this to 55.7%. In terms of incentive type, cash payments were the most preferred incentive but not significantly different from supermarket vouchers. Both were preferred to health savings credits and sporting goods vouchers. Concerns of adverse selection were minimal because those who were inactive represented at least 72% of new participants for any offered program(s) and were the majority.

Conclusions

Study results demonstrate the potential for even modest incentives to increase program uptake among inactive older adults. Moreover, although cash was the most preferred option, supermarket vouchers, which could potentially be purchased at a discount, were a close alternative. Results also suggest that an enrolment fee is a viable option to offset the costs of incentives as it has only minimal impact on participation.

Peer Review reports

Background

There is extensive evidence that physical activity exerts a strong protective effect on the health of older adults. This includes reducing the risk and progression of non-communicable diseases and other disabling conditions [14], improving cardiorespiratory and muscle fitness [5], health-related quality of life [6, 7], psychological well-being [1] and longevity. It also has the potential to contain rising medical costs [8, 9]. Despite these benefits, current data reveal low levels of physical activity levels among older adults [10, 11] in nearly every developed country.

Walking is an accessible form of physical activity with proven health benefits [12] that can be conducted across a variety of settings without specialized equipment. It is also the most common form of physical activity among older adults who engage in regular activity [13]. It is for such reasons that walking programs have been promoted as a means to increase activity levels among sedentary older adults. Yet, in many communities, participation in structured programs targeting older adults remains low [14, 15].

By increasing the benefits of participation, economic theory suggests that the use of incentives could increase participation rates in structured walking programs. Incentives have been shown to be effective in positively shaping various health behaviors and health outcomes [1624] although there is limited data on its suitability and efficacy among older adults [25]. Moreover, even if effective, incentives come with a cost. One way to offset the cost is to include an enrolment fee. It is possible that the fee may deter some potential participants, but the net effect of incentives and an enrolment fee in affecting uptake by older adults in structured programs remains an empirical question. Therefore, the goal of this study was to conduct a conjoint analysis survey aimed at identifying the extent to which stated uptake of walking programs could be increased via the use of incentives, either with or without a corresponding enrolment fee.

In this stated preference conjoint study we estimate the net effect of several types/magnitudes of incentives and enrolment fees for candidate walking programs among a sample of older, multi-ethnic adults in Singapore. We hypothesize that incentives will increase the stated likelihood of participation in the programs regardless of the presence of an enrolment fee. Conjoint analysis is well suited for this task because it allows for generating predictions of program effects in a low-cost and efficient manner for several candidate programs, the most promising of which can then be tested in a randomized controlled trial to quantify the effects on both uptake and outcomes.

Methods

Participants and setting

The conjoint survey was administered in 2011 by a local survey firm to a national sample of 1,000 Singaporeans aged 50 and over. Trained interviewers took written consent and administered the paper and pencil survey in respondents’ homes. The interviews were conducted in four languages: English, Mandarin, Malay and Tamil.

Survey structure

The survey contained questions concerning socio-demographic characteristics and health status. Data on past and present physical activity levels were also collected as were attitudes towards walking programs. The core of the survey was the discrete choice conjoint experiment. Each respondent was given ten pairs of hypothetical programs and asked to choose the option they preferred. They were then asked whether or not they would join their preferred program if given the option. If they responded “Very likely” or “Somewhat likely”, they were assumed to join their preferred program. Prior to fielding the conjoint questions, participants were introduced to the various program features and were told that the programs lasted for 6 months, were to be conducted in a group setting, and that each session comprised of 45 minutes of walking and 15 minutes of stretching, and that a non-refundable enrolment fee was to be paid in advance but that if they met program goals a financial incentive would be provided at the conclusion of the program. Other aspects of the programs varied along six dimensions (six attributes) as shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Attributes and levels

All attributes and their levels were finalized through a series of focus groups and cognitive interviews. Travel time and related costs were included as travel considerations were likely to affect program participation. There were 4 levels for travel time ranging from 15 minutes to 45 minutes and 3 for travel cost: $0, $2 and $5, which approximate the cost of public transportation as well as the effect of a potential travel subsidy that offsets travel costs. A key component of the program was the inclusion of financial incentives. As is common in programs of this sort, various types of financial incentives were offered (cash, supermarket vouchers, Medisave (health savings account) credits and sporting goods vouchers) with the amount of incentive ranging from $150 to $450 over the 6-month period. Finally, a one-time enrolment fee which varied from $0 (a free program), $20 and $50 was also included to quantify the effect of these fees on program participation.

For each of the ten pairs of hypothetical walking programs, participants were asked to choose their preferred program and then indicate how likely they would be to join the program if it were offered to them. The response options consisted of four choices: “Very likely”, “Somewhat likely”, “Somewhat unlikely” and “Very unlikely”. An example choice task is reproduced in Figure 1. The full survey is available upon request.

Figure 1
figure 1

Sample conjoint task.

Experimental design

Since our survey contained 6 attributes (each with either 3 or 4 levels), it was not feasible to show respondents every possible combination of attributes and levels. Therefore, following common practice in DCEs [26], we produced a statistically efficient fractional design that was orthogonal, had minimal level overlap and ensured level balance (each level appears approximately the same number of times) [27]. The design consisted of four choice sets with ten tasks each that were allocated randomly to respondents. Each task consisted of two hypothetical walking programs. The survey also included a “rationality” test to determine whether respondents chose the correct alternative in a dominated task (where one choice is the same or better on all attribute levels). Those who chose the dominated alternative were excluded from the analysis (19.8% of the sample) out of concerns that their data may be invalid. This left a sample of 802 respondents.

Ethic statement

The study was approved by the National University of Singapore IRB, and was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Statistical analysis

A random utility model (RUM) was used to analyze responses to the conjoint survey tasks. This model assumes that the utility associated with each program is a function of its attribute levels:

u jt i = v i x jt , c jt + e jt i

In this specification, u jt i represents a respondent i’s utility for scenario j in t tasks. v i represents the observable components of the utility function and is composed of a vector of all program attribute levels excluding the incentive payment (x jt ) and the incentive payment (Cjt). A model which specified incentive payment levels as categorical (indicator variables for $150, $200, $300 and $450) was found to produce estimates that were not significantly different from those generated from a linear specification. For ease of exposition, the incentive payment was coded as a linear term. All other attributes were effects-coded. Effects coding allows for easy comparisons of the relative importance of the levels as the sum of the preference weights within an attribute will be zero. Linear coding yields a single coefficient for an attribute. In this case, the coefficient on the incentive payment term is assumed to be linear within the range of incentives offered, thus allowing the coefficient to be interpreted as the marginal utility of an additional dollar of incentives. e jt i is a random error term representing the component of utility which is unobservable. As per utility theory, each respondent is expected to choose the scenario that provides the highest level of utility in each task. This model was estimated using a hierarchical Bayes estimation procedure [28] (implemented in Sawtooth Software CBC Hierarchical Bayes Module version 5.2.8.), which assumes heterogeneous preferences and allows for the estimation of the mean and variance of individual-level preference weights (the Beta coefficients). To derive the monetary amount required for an individual to be indifferent between two attribute levels, we divided the difference in preference weights between the two levels by the marginal utility of the incentive payment.

The inclusion of a follow-up question indicating likelihood of joining a program enabled us to estimate program uptake. To estimate uptake for select programs, we quantified the percentage of individuals whose predicted utility for a given program was greater than their predicted utility when not joining (responding Very Unlikely or Somewhat Unlikely to join). If greater, they were assumed to join the program. We present results for a free program and for one with a $50 enrolment fee. In addition to the above, we also present the proportion of those enrolling who were already meeting activity guidelines, as incentives for these individuals merely represents a transfer payment and are not expected to increase activity levels.

Results

Table 2 shows the demographic characteristics of our sample. The median age was 59.5 years. Over half the respondents were female and had less than primary school education. Consistent with the population in Singapore, the vast majority were Chinese. Slightly under half were employed yet nearly 80% did not meet activity guidelines.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics

Regression results from the conjoint analysis are presented in Additional file 1: Table S1. Figure 2 graphically presents these results after centering the preference weights to 5 and rescaling to between 0 and 10 for ease of exposition. Attribute levels with higher preference weights were preferred to those with lower weights. The most preferred walking program was a convenient, free (in terms of travel cost and enrolment fee) program with one 45-minute session per week and a $450 cash incentive. When statistically significant, the attribute levels were in the expected direction. None of the levels for travel cost were significantly different from one another nor was there a difference between the $20 and $50 enrolment fee. In terms of incentive type, cash payments were the most preferred incentive but not significantly different from NTUC supermarket vouchers. Both were preferred to Medisave (health savings) credits and sporting goods vouchers were least preferred.

Figure 2
figure 2

Preferences across attributes and levels.

The relative value of various types of incentives can be seen in Table 3. One dollar in cash is roughly equivalent to $1.09 (95% CI: $1.01 to $1.18) in supermarket vouchers, $1.73 ($1.52 to $1.98) in Medisave vouchers, and $3.89 ($2.99 to $5.54) in sporting good vouchers. All of the differences are statistically significant.

Table 3 Incentive amount needed by payment type

Figure 3 shows estimated uptake for the sample program shown in Table 4 with 1) no enrolment fee and 2) a $50 enrolment fee. As expected, the free program results in higher predicted uptake at all incentive levels, and greater incentives generate greater stated levels of enrolment. However, including a $50 enrolment fee only decreases enrolment by an average of 2%. Based on these results, a free program with $500 worth of incentives would generate enrolment rates of 58.5%; charging $50 to enroll only lowers this figure to 55.7%.

Figure 3
figure 3

Uptake rate by enrolment fee for varying incentive amounts.

Table 4 Hypothetical walking program used to estimate uptakes

Figure 4 shows how uptake varies between those who state they are meeting physical activity guidelines (”Active”) and those who state they are not (“Inactive”). Almost half of Actives claim they would participate in the sample program even if they were not offered any incentives. The percentage of actives increases to as much as 69% for the highest incentive amount. In contrast, only 38% of Inactives claim they would join a free program. However, when incentives are provided, their stated participation increases at a faster rate than for Actives. At $500, over 55% of Inactives state they would join the program. Because they are the majority of the population, regardless of the incentive amount, Inactives represent at least 72% of participants, and this number increases with higher incentive levels.

Figure 4
figure 4

Uptake rate by physical activity status for varying incentive amounts.

Discussion

The objective of this study was to identify a promising walking program for older, multi-ethnic adults in Singapore, and then to test the effects on stated enrolment when incentives and/or an enrolment fee are offered. Not surprisingly, results showed that respondents most prefer the least onerous program with the greatest benefits. This is one involving minimal travel time and cost, comprising the least number of weekly activity sessions (1 session per week), offering a 450SGD cash incentive and without any enrolment fee. Although minimal, one brisk walking session per week would account for 30% of recommended weekly activity if participants walk at the recommend pace. Moreover, this level of activity is still associated with some health benefits, especially among sedentary adults [2931]. Therefore, it may be worth offering such a program, even with incentives, if it could encourage sustained increases in activity levels.

A key finding of this study is that financial incentives increase predicted uptake for all programs offered and more so among the least active older adults. For the hypothetical six month program offered, providing SGD$150 for meeting participation goals (SGD$25 ≈ USD20 monthly over 6 months assuming 1SGD = 1.25USD) increases predicted program uptake by 8.5% (assuming no enrolment fee). Raising this to SGD$450 (SGD$75 ≈ USD 60 monthly over 6 months) increases uptake by 13%. These results demonstrate the potential for even modest incentives to increase uptake. Moreover, although cash was clearly the most preferred option, supermarket vouchers, which could potentially be purchased in bulk at a discount, were a close alternative. Health savings account credits and sporting goods vouchers/equipment are not recommended as respondents place a lower value on these rewards and thus they are less of a motivator.

Although we show that incentives increase program uptake and have been shown to be successful in improving outcomes in prior activity studies [25, 32], they are not without cost. One way to offset those costs is via an enrolment fee. These results suggest that an enrolment fee is a viable option as it has a minimal impact on participation. Moreover, the largest impact occurs when going from a free program to a program with a nominal fee. Increases beyond this level can be set so as to recover the cost of the incentives, and will likely have only a modest additional loss in participation; less than 2% based on our estimates when fees are within the $SGD20 to $SGD50 range. Moreover, if the enrolment fee is seen as a deposit whose return is dependent upon program completion, it is likely to have the added benefit of reducing attrition and increasing program effectiveness [21].

When incentives are provided, there is always the concern of adverse selection. In this case the concern is that the incentives go to those who would have been doing the required amounts of activity even without the incentives. Although it is unclear whether these results would generalize to other older adult populations, we found that incentives have a greater stated effect on enrolment for inactive participants. This result may not be surprising given that active participants may have their routines well established and may be less interested in these types of programs. A financial incentive may also be less of a motivation to them, as they have already made the decision to be active. Nevertheless, active participants may benefit through accruing more health benefits to the extent that incentives induce additional physical activity [3335]. Monetary incentives are likely to have the greatest effect on participation and outcomes for those who are least active.

Although the first of its kind in a multi-ethnic sample of older adults, this study adds to growing evidence of the potential for financial incentives to motivate health behavior change [24, 25, 36, 37], specifically in promoting activity among older adults in the US and other countries. These results are corroborated by Finkelstein et al. [25] who observed significant, large effects in an incentive-based physical activity trial among older adults aged 50+ where participants assigned to the intervention arm logged an average of 4.1 hours of activity a week relative to their counterparts in the control arm who logged only 2.3 hours over 4 weeks. However, several limitations must be noted. The primary limitation is that the results are based on a survey, and not a real world experiment. The extent to which these surveys serve as a good basis for successful program implementation remains unknown. This should be an area of future research. As such, the results should be interpreted with caution. Yet this is both a limitation and a strength. This study used a low cost survey elicitation technique to generate hypotheses about what might happen were these programs to be offered; future studies should test these predictions in the real world. Moreover, as with any study, even if accurate for Singapore, the generalizability of these results beyond older adults here is unknown. Future studies will need to be targeted to the population of interest. Besides, field studies involving older adults would also need to account for the unique cognitive and psychosocial characteristics of this sub-population [38]. Also, real world cost-effectiveness studies would be a necessary next step to evaluate the long-term feasibility of such programs in specific settings. Finally, incentives, even if effective at increasing uptake, may not be sustainable as they may not translate into sustained behavior change. However, in at least one study, increased activity persisted even after the removal of incentives [39, 40].

The use of incentives has raised concerns about a “crowding out effect”. This would result if the incentives undermine intrinsic motivation. Yet, although theory suggests this could be the case, there is no empirical evidence to support it [41]. Several studies show that incentives increase activity levels of both children and adults. As a result, this appears not to be a legitimate concern [25, 32, 40]. Also, providing incentives may not be affordable in the long term. Although we show that an enrolment fee is one strategy to overcome this concern, other strategies will be required to engage sedentary individuals for whom incentives are ineffective. Identifying these strategies should be a topic of future research. Regardless, these results suggest that incentives may be part of a comprehensive solution aimed at increasing activity levels of older adults.

Conclusions

Study findings add to the growing literature that incentives are a feasible and potentially effective strategy to increase program uptake among older adults. Although cash is the most preferred incentive type, supermarket vouchers are a close alternative. To offset associated costs, enrolment fees are a viable option. Future research should assess the affordability of incentive use and sustainability of model programs in addition to identifying other strategies to encourage sustained activity of older adults.

Abbreviations

ACSM:

America college of sports medicine

AHF:

American heart foundation

NCD:

Non-communicable diseases

HPB:

Health promotion board

RUM:

Random utility model.

References

  1. Chodzko-Zajko WJ, Proctor DN, Fiatarone Singh MA, Minson CT, Nigg CR, Salem GJ, Skinner JS: American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Exercise and physical activity for older adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009, 41 (7): 1510-1530. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181a0c95c.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. American College of Sports Medicine Position Stand: Exercise and physical activity for older adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1998, 30 (6): 992-1008. 10.1097/00005768-199806000-00033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Butler RN, Davis R, Lewis CB, Nelson ME, Strauss E: Physical fitness: benefits of exercise for the older patient. 2. Geriatrics. 1998, 53 (10): 49-52.

    Google Scholar 

  4. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Physical Activity and Health: A Report of the Surgeon General. 1996, Atlanta, GA; U.S: Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion

    Google Scholar 

  5. Franco OH, de Laet C, Peeters A, Jonker J, Mackenbach J, Nusselder W: Effects of physical activity on life expectancy with cardiovascular disease. Arch Intern Med. 2005, 165 (20): 2355-2360. 10.1001/archinte.165.20.2355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fiatarone Singh MA: Benefits of exercise and dietary measures to optimize shifts in body composition with age. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2002, 11: S642-S652. 10.1046/j.1440-6047.11.supp3.17.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Church TS, Earnest CP, Skinner JS, Blair SN: Effects of different doses of physical activity on cardiorespiratory fitness among sedentary, overweight or obese postmenopausal women with elevated blood pressure: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2007, 297 (19): 2081-2091. 10.1001/jama.297.19.2081.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ackermann RT, Williams B, Nguyen HQ, Berke EM, Maciejewski ML, LoGerfo JP: Healthcare cost differences with participation in a community-based group physical activity benefit for medicare managed care health plan members. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008, 56 (8): 1459-1465. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01804.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Anderson LH, Martinson BC, Crain AL, Pronk NP, Whitebird RR, O'Connor PJ, Fine LJ: Health care charges associated with physical inactivity, overweight, and obesity. Prev Chronic Dis. 2005, 2 (4): 15-

    Google Scholar 

  10. Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Masse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M: Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008, 40 (1): 181-188. 10.1249/mss.0b013e31815a51b3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Cook M, Greer E, Murray K, Mcgeown P, McVicker D, Ruddy M: Promotiong Physical Activity with Older People. http://www.sportni.net/NR/rdonlyres/45D1A19E-735C-4F0A-A3D7-11F563BA38D9/0/PromotingPhysicalActivityOlderPeople.pdf,

  12. Wong CH, Wong SF, Pang WS, Azizah MY, Dass MJ: Habitual walking and its correlation to better physical function: implications for prevention of physical disability in older persons. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2003, 58 (6): 555-560. 10.1093/gerona/58.6.M555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Simpson ME, Serdula M, Galuska DA, Gillespie C, Donehoo R, Macera C, Mack K: Walking trends among U.S. Adults: the behavioral risk factor surveillance system, 1987–2000. Am J Prev Med. 2003, 25 (2): 95-100. 10.1016/S0749-3797(03)00112-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. 10, 000 Steps. http://www.shapeup.org/resources/10ksteps.html,

  15. 10, 000 Steps. http://www.10000steps.org.au/,

  16. Jeffery RW, Hellerstedt WL, Schmid TL: Correspondence programs for smoking cessation and weight control: a comparison of two strategies in the Minnesota Heart Health Program. Health Psychol. 1990, 9 (5): 585-598.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Jeffery RW, Bjornson-Benson WM, Rosenthal BS, Kurth CL, Dunn MM: Effectiveness of monetary contracts with two repayment schedules on weight reduction in men and women from self-referred and population samples. Behav Ther. 1984, 15 (3): 273-279. 10.1016/S0005-7894(84)80029-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Jeffery RW, Thompson PD, Wing RR: Effects on weight reduction of strong monetary contracts for calorie restriction or weight loss. Behav Res Ther. 1978, 16 (5): 363-369. 10.1016/0005-7967(78)90005-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Jeffery RW, Gerber WM, Rosenthal BS, Lindquist RA: Monetary contracts in weight control: effectiveness of group and individual contracts of varying size. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1983, 51 (2): 242-248.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Finkelstein EA, Linnan LA, Tate DF, Birken BE: A pilot study testing the effect of different levels of financial incentives on weight loss among overweight employees. J Occup Environ Med. 2007, 49 (9): 981-989. 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31813c6dcb. 910.1097/JOM.1090b1013e31813c31816dcb

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Volpp KG, John LK, Troxel AB, Norton L, Fassbender J, Loewenstein G: Financial incentive-based approaches for weight loss: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008, 300 (22): 2631-2637. 10.1001/jama.2008.804.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Volpp KG, Gurmankin Levy A, Asch DA, Berlin JA, Murphy JJ, Gomez A, Sox H, Zhu J, Lerman C: A randomized controlled trial of financial incentives for smoking cessation. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2006, 15 (1): 12-18. 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-05-0314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Giné X, Karlan D, Zinman J: Put your money where your butt is: a commitment contract for smoking cessation. Am Econ J Appl Econ. 2010, 4 (2): 213-235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Sutherland K, Christianson JB, Leatherman S: Impact of targeted financial incentives on personal health behavior: a review of the literature. Med Care Res Rev. 2008, 65 (6 Suppl): 36S-78S. 10.1177/1077558708324235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Finkelstein EA, Brown DS, Brown DR, Buchner DM: A randomized study of financial incentives to increase physical activity among sedentary older adults. Prev Med. 2008, 47 (2): 182-187. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.05.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Reed Johnson F, Lancsar E, Marshall D, Kilambi V, Mühlbacher A, Regier DA, Bresnahan BW, Kanninen B, Bridges JFP: Constructing experimental designs for discrete-choice experiments: report of the ISPOR conjoint analysis experimental design good research practices task force. Value Health. 2013, 16 (1): 3-13. 10.1016/j.jval.2012.08.2223.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Zwerina K, Huber J, Kuhfeld WF: A general method for constructing efficient choice designs. 1996, Durham, NC: Fuqua School of Business, Duke University

    Google Scholar 

  28. Huber J, Train K: On the similarity of classical and Bayesian estimates of individual mean partworths. Market Lett. 2001, 12 (3): 259-269. 10.1023/A:1011120928698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kesaniemi YK, Danforth E, Jensen MD, Kopelman PG, Lefebvre P, Reeder BA: Dose-response issues concerning physical activity and health: an evidence-based symposium. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2001, 33 (6 Suppbibul): S351-358.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Manson JE, Greenland P, LaCroix AZ, Stefanick ML, Mouton CP, Oberman A, Perri MG, Sheps DS, Pettinger MB, Siscovick DS: Walking compared with vigorous exercise for the prevention of cardiovascular events in women. N Engl J Med. 2002, 347 (10): 716-725. 10.1056/NEJMoa021067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Oguma Y, Shinoda-Tagawa T: Physical activity decreases cardiovascular disease risk in women: review and meta-analysis. Am J Prev Med. 2004, 26 (5): 407-418. 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.02.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Finkelstein EA, Tan YT, Malhotra R, Lee CF, Goh SS, Saw SM: A Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial of an Incentive-Based Outdoor Physical Activity Program. The Journal of pediatrics. 2013, 163 (1): 167-172. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.01.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lee I: Dose-response relation between physical activity and fitness: Even a little is good; more is better. JAMA. 2007, 297 (19): 2137-2139. 10.1001/jama.297.19.2137.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Sattelmair J, Pertman J, Ding EL, Kohl HW, Haskell W, Lee I-M: Dose Response Between Physical Activity and Risk of Coronary Heart Disease: A Meta-Analysis. Circulation. 2011, 124 (7): 789-795. 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.110.010710.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Hamer M, Stamatakis E, Steptoe A: Dose response relationship between physical activity and mental health: The Scottish Health Survey. Br J Sports Med. 2008, 43 (14): 1111-1114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Hennrikus DJ, Jeffery RW, Lando HA, Murray DM, Brelje K, Davidann B, Baxter JS, Thai D, Vessey J, Liu J: The SUCCESS project: the effect of program format and incentives on participation and cessation in worksite smoking cessation programs. Am J Public Health. 2002, 92 (2): 274-279. 10.2105/AJPH.92.2.274.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Gingerich SB, Anderson DR, Koland H: Impact of financial incentives on behavior change program participation and risk reduction in worksite health promotion. Am J Health Promot. 2012, 27 (2): 119-122. 10.4278/ajhp.110726-ARB-295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Klein E, Karlawish J: Challenges and Opportunities for Developing and Implementing Incentives to Improve Health‒Related Behaviors in Older Adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010, 58 (9): 1758-1763. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03030.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Acland D, Levy M: Habit Formation and Naiveté in Gym Attendance: Evidence from a Field Experiment. http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/46827/,

  40. Charness G, Gneezy U: Incentives to Exercise. Econometrica. 2009, 77 (3): 909-931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Promberger M, Marteau TM: When do financial incentives reduce intrinsic motivation? comparing behaviors studied in psychological and economic literatures. Health Psychol. 2013, 32 (9): 950-957.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Vicky Drury for her help with the focus groups, Ms. Patsy Lam for help with the early manuscript draft, and Dr. Robert Alan Sloan from the Health Promotion Board (HPB) for his advice on physical activity analyses. This work was supported by funding from the NUS (National University of Singapore) Virtual Institute for the Study of Ageing (DPRT/AGE/2010/20).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eric A Finkelstein.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

EAF, MAF and YTT contributed to the conception and design of the project. MAF performed all analyses and MB assisted with interpretation. MAF and YTT completed the first draft while EAF provided critical revision. All authors edited and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Farooqui, M.A., Tan, YT., Bilger, M. et al. Effects of financial incentives on motivating physical activity among older adults: results from a discrete choice experiment. BMC Public Health 14, 141 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-141

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-141

Keywords