- Research article
- Open access
- Published:
To bite or not to bite! A questionnaire-based survey assessing why some people are bitten more than others by midges
BMC Public Health volume 10, Article number: 275 (2010)
Abstract
Background
The Scottish biting midge, Culicoides impunctatus, responsible for more than 90% of biting attacks on human beings in Scotland, is known to demonstrate a preference for certain human hosts over others.
Methods
In this study we used a questionnaire-based survey to assess the association between people's perception of how badly they get bitten by midges and their demographic, lifestyle and health related characteristics.
Results
Most people (85.8%) reported being bitten sometimes, often or always with only 14.2% reporting never being bitten by midges when in Scotland. There was no association between level of bites received and age, smoking, diet, exercise, medication, eating strongly flavoured foods or alcohol consumption. However, there was a strong association between the probability of being bitten and increasing height (in men) and BMI (in women). A large proportion of participants (33.8%) reported experiencing a bad/severe reaction to midge bites while 53.1% reported a minor reaction and 13.1% no reaction at all. Also, women tend to react more than men to midge bites. Additionally, the results indicated that the susceptibility to being bitten by midges is hereditary.
Conclusions
This study suggests that midges prefer to bite men that are tall and women that have a large BMI, and that the tendency for a child to be bitten or not could be inherited from their parent. The study is questionnaire-based; therefore, the interpretation of the results may be limited by the subjectivity of the answers given by the respondents. Although the results are relevant only to the Scottish biting midge, the approach used here could be useful for investigating human-insect interactions for other insects, particularly those which transmit pathogens that cause disease.
Background
The Scottish biting midge, Culicoides impunctatus, is responsible for more than 90% of biting attacks on human beings in Scotland. The painful reactions caused by the bites discourage tourists and disrupt outdoor industries including agriculture and forestry, causing significant losses to the British economy [1]. An estimated 20% loss in working hours each year has been reported in the forestry industry due to midge bites alone [2]. In extreme circumstances, a rate of more than 40 000 midges landing on one forearm every hour has been recorded [3].
Biting midges are known to demonstrate a preference for certain human hosts over others. Anecdotal evidence relating to this phenomenon is now supported by recent scientific evidence. We have shown that host preference is controlled by differences in body odour profiles of different individuals, with non-attractive people producing natural "repellents" [4]. Although the chemical basis for differential attraction to midges is now known, we do not know whether there are particular characteristics or lifestyles that make someone more or less susceptible to attack by midges.
The body odours of many vertebrates (including human beings) are known to provide information about individual identity, sex, reproductive status and health to other members of the same species [5]. Therefore, odours that are specifically associated with such factors could influence biting insects during the selection of an appropriate human host. This has never been investigated for biting midges, but some evidence exists in studies with mosquitoes. For example, some studies have shown that species of Anopheles (malaria mosquitoes) preferentially feed on adults rather than children [6–12]. Similarly, Michael et al. [13] demonstrated that the biting rates of Culex quinquefasciatus (the Southern house mosquito) had a positive association with age in children. It has also been shown that men are bitten more readily than women [10, 14] and that larger people are also bitten more often [11]. Additionally, it has been shown that women are more likely to be bitten when pregnant [15]. Other conflicting studies have indicated that certain mosquito species will feed randomly on individuals regardless of age, sex or size [11, 14, 16]. There is also evidence that the level of attractiveness to mosquitoes could be hereditary. For example, Kirk et al. [17] performed a survey on adolescent twins and demonstrated that there was a strong genetic influence on frequency of being bitten by mosquitoes in children between the ages of 12 and 14.
Although there is anecdotal evidence for the association between certain lifestyle, health and demographic characteristics and level of attractiveness to midges, this has never been examined in detail. The aim of this study was to use, for the first time, a questionnaire-based survey to investigate whether the above factors have any association with attraction to midges and reaction to midge bites.
Methods
The survey was completed by volunteers who were participants or spectators at a duathlon event (First Monster Challenge) held in Inverness, Scotland in September 2008. The cohort involved was thus fairly uniform with the majority of respondents being young, fit and healthy adults. All questionnaires were either distributed the evening before the event and collected the following day or handed out and collected on the day of the event.
A structured self administered questionnaire survey, using both closed and open-ended questions, was developed to investigate details about the level of attractiveness to midges in Scotland and how this relates to other demographic, lifestyle and genetic factors. The questionnaire consisted of 28 questions in five sections: (1) demographics (including age, sex, height, weight and place of residence); (2) lifestyle/health; (3) perceived level of attractiveness to midges; (4) perceived level of attractiveness of their offspring (expressed as first child) to midges; (5) reproductive state (women only). For perceived level of attractiveness to midges, volunteers were asked "when in Scotland, in an area with midges, how often do you get bitten, when on your own?" and were given answer options of "never", "sometimes", "often" or "always". All other factors relating to demographics, lifestyle, etc were cross referenced to this question. Volunteers were also asked "when in Scotland, in an area with midges, how often do you get bitten, when you are with others?". The answers to this question were highly correlated with the answers to the question referring to when the volunteers were on their own, therefore, we have not included these results.
Participants were also asked "when midges bite how do you react?" and were given four options "no reaction" (no mark on skin), "minor" (raised red mark that disappears quickly), "bad" (raised red itchy mark that lasts for a few days) and "very bad" (large red mark with blisters giving pain and itching). Again, factors relating to demographics and lifestyle, etc were cross referenced to this question. The study was approved by the North of Scotland Research Ethics Committee (Ref 07/S0801/51).
Statistical analysis
Data for level of attractiveness (i.e. "never", "sometimes", "often" and "always") were combined to give two categories "never bitten" (category "never" only) and "bitten" (a combination of categories "sometimes", "often" and "always"). Data for reaction to bites ("no reaction", "minor", "bad" and "very bad") were combined to give three categories "no reaction", "minor" and "bad" (a combination of "bad" and "very bad"). Descriptive statistics were then performed on the data using GenStat (version 12.1.0.3412). Two-way cross tabulations were used to analyse associations between level of attractiveness and the following variables: age, sex, height, weight, BMI, place of residence, smoking habit, diet, allergies, exercise, medication intake (including oral contraceptives), alcohol consumption and menstrual cycle stage (women only). For this purpose, the continuous variables age, height, weight and BMI were grouped to form categorical variables. In these cases, category boundaries were chosen to be both easily interpretable and to give a reasonable spread of participants across categories. To determine whether susceptibility to being bitten by midges can be inherited, a cross tabulation was also done to investigate associations between level of attractiveness of parents and their children. A chi-square test of independence was done on all two-way tables and, due to small sample sizes for certain categories, p-values were generated from a permutation test using the method of Roff & Bentzen [18]. When the chi-square test indicated association between the variables, further models were fitted. Where one of the variables could be construed as a binary response, logistic regression was used to identify the categories where the response differed. Where all variables had more than two categories, multinomial models were fitted, and residuals from a model of conditional independence were used to identify categories contributing to the association.
For the variables height, weight and BMI, where the distribution differed markedly between men and women, three-way tabulations were formed for the level of attractiveness ("never bitten" vs. "bitten") against both sexes and height, weight or BMI. Logistic regression was used to analyse these data, using height, weight or BMI as a continuous variable.
Results
Participant summary
A total of 325 participants took part in this survey. The majority (66.2%) were male and most were within the 'under 31' or '31-40' age categories (30% and 33% respectively) and there was an equal age distribution across both sexes (p = 0.46). The majority of participants were fit and healthy with most people (76.9%) doing more than 3 hours of exercise per week and only 6.6% doing less than 1 hour per week. Forty five percent of males and 53% of females had a normal BMI (between 21 and 30). Most people lived in Scotland (71%) with the majority of these living in the west of the country (Table 1).
Level of being bitten
When the participants were asked the question "when in Scotland alone in an area with midges, how often are you bitten?" the majority (85.8%, n = 310) said they were bitten sometimes, often or always with only 14.2% claiming to never be bitten. When the participants were asked the question "when in Scotland in an area with midges with others, how often are you bitten?" the majority (88.4%, n = 303) said they were bitten sometimes, often or always with only 11.6% claiming to never be bitten. There was a very high association (p < 0.001) between the two questions, with only 9.2% (28 out of 303) participants giving different answers to the two questions.
The data for other factors were then cross-referenced with the answers to the question "when in Scotland alone in an area with midges, how often are you bitten?". Because of possible variation in people's ability to accurately recall, we compared this with their actual perceived level of being bitten on the day of the survey when there were midges present and people were being bitten. There was a strong positive association between the level of being bitten usually compared with level of being bitten on that day (p < 0.001).
There was a strong association between where people live and how much they report they are usually bitten (p = 0.004). The probability of being bitten was significantly greater for people living in west Scotland and significantly less for those living in England (Table 2).
Logistic regression relating the probability of (reporting) being bitten to sex and height showed strong evidence that the probability of being bitten increased with height for men (p = 0.006, n = 305; Figure 1) but not for women. The predicted probability of being bitten for men at the 10th (170.7 cm) and 90th (188 cm) sample percentiles of height distribution are 0.7 (SE 0.05) and 0.92 (SE 0.26). Weight showed no relationship to the probability of being bitten (p = 0.79, n = 300). However, there was evidence that the probability of being bitten increased with BMI for women (p = 0.050, n = 296) but not for men. The predicted probability of usually being bitten in Scotland for females at 10th (19.42) and 90th (27.90) sample percentiles of BMI distribution are 0.77 (SE 0.072) and 0.96 (SE 0.02) respectively (Figure 1).
There was a strong interaction between the frequency of bites the first child receives and how often the parent is usually bitten (p < 0.001, n = 146). Logistic regression showed that the probability of the first child not being bitten by midges was much higher if the parent also claimed to be usually not bitten (p < 0.001). There was no evidence that the sex of the parent had an effect on the rate of biting on the child (p = 0.11) (Figure 2).
There was insufficient variation in the responses to reliably assess association between ethnic origin, general diet, medication or illness and whether people are bitten or not bitten by midges. There was no significant association between age, sex, smoking, exercise, eating strongly flavoured foods (e.g. garlic, chilli and onions), frequency and amount of alcohol consumption and whether people are bitten or not bitten by midges (p > 0.05). In women there was no evidence for an interaction between level of biting and the reproductive status (i.e. premenopausal, menopausal or postmenopausal) (p = 0.99, n = 62), whether the respondents were taking oral contraceptives (p = 0.44, n = 91), or the stage of the menstrual cycle (p = 1.00, n = 51). There was some evidence of a significant association with allergy to insect bites and being bitten (p = 0.05, n = 310), due to a high proportion of people with insect allergy reporting that they are "always bitten" when in Scotland. This association disappears (p = 0.99) for the binary (not bitten vs. bitten) response.
Level of reaction to bites
For participants' reported reactions to midge bites, data in the "bad" and "very bad" categories were combined in the analysis because there were only 11 responses in the "very bad" category (3.6%, n = 305). The majority of people (53.1%) reported a minor reaction and 33.8% reported a bad reaction. Only 13.1% claimed that they have no reaction to midge bites.
There was a strong association between the frequency of being bitten and the reaction to bites with most people who report reacting badly to bites also receiving high numbers of bites and with those who do not react badly claiming they never get bitten (p < 0.001, Figure 3).
There was no correlation between age and reaction to bites (p = 0.46, n = 305). However, there was a significant association of reaction to bites in relation to sex (p = 0.005), which was caused by women having a significantly higher probability of reporting a bad reaction to midge bites (p = 0.01) (Figure 4).
There was no association between residence (p = 0.37, n = 292), eating strongly flavoured foods (e.g. chilli, garlic, onions, p = 0.14, n = 300) or the frequency (p = 0.70, n = 301) and amount (p = 0.36, n = 303) of alcohol ingestion, reproductive status (p = 0.18, n = 61), contraceptive usage (p = 0.86, n = 90) or menstrual stage (p = 0.94, n = 52) and reaction to midge-bites. Neither smoking (p = 0.27, n = 304) or allergies (including allergies to animals, antibiotics, food, hay fever and pollen) had any effect on reaction to bites either amalgamated (p = 0.98, n = 304) or individually. However, there was a significant association between people reporting an allergy to insects and their reaction to bites, with people with an insect allergy reacting more badly to bites than those without (p = 0.03, n = 305), as would be expected.
There was also a significant association between exercise and reaction to midge bites (p = 0.04, n = 303). A multinomial model indicated that people doing the least exercise (less than one hour per week) and most exercise (more than 4 hours) were more likely to have no reaction to midge bites than people who do between 1 and 4 hours of exercise per week. Logistic regression showed no significant interaction between level of reaction and height (p = 0.59, n = 300), weight (p = 0.74, n = 296) or BMI (p = 0.18, n = 292) (when differences due to sex had been accounted for).
Discussion
Despite the vast amount of anecdotal evidence surrounding why some people get bitten more than others by blood-feeding insects, few studies have investigated the demographic, lifestyle and health factors involved. This is the largest investigation of its type for any biting insect. Our finding that only 14.2% of people claim to never be bitten is the first measurement of differential attraction to biting insects using a large sample size. To our knowledge this is the first reported statistic of the prevalence of the trait for being "unattractive" to biting insects and supports our previous work on mosquitoes and midges on much smaller sample sizes when directly measuring attractiveness and odour profiles of individuals [4, 19].
It is well known that the west coast of Scotland is an area of high midge populations and, therefore, it was expected to find that people who claim they are bitten at home the most indeed lived in the west of Scotland. This geographic region provides a unique environment which allows the Scottish biting midge to thrive successfully [3].
Our study is the first to show an association between body size and attractiveness to biting midges. The preference for biting people of a greater height could be associated with midge behaviour and flight patterns. For example, Venter et al. [20] demonstrated, using light traps, that most midges are caught at a height of 2.8 metres above ground level. Midges are also known to rest in trees after they have emerged from pupae and are found in greater numbers with increasing height (in particular from 1-4 metres) [21]. It is, therefore, possible that midges searching for a suitable host would be descending from above and would encounter taller people, within in a group, first. Additionally, larger people would provide a more substantial visual target for host-seeking midges as well as greater amounts of heat, moisture and attractant semiochemicals (behaviour modifying chemicals), such as carbon dioxide, which are all involved in the location of a suitable blood meal. These findings are also supported by similar studies with mosquitoes. For example, Port et al. [11] demonstrated that larger people are bitten more often by certain mosquito species. Similarly, men have been shown to be bitten more readily than women in previous studies, however, whether this is due to true differences between the sexes (a pattern not found here) or due to differences in body size is unknown [10, 14, 22].
Some investigations have provided evidence that certain mosquito species will preferentially feed on adults rather than children [6–12]. Similarly, Michael et al. [13] demonstrated that biting rates of Culex species mosquitoes had a positive association with age in children. In this study, the participants were adults so we could not assess whether there is a difference between children and adults. However, we did not find any association of age with the level of being bitten by midges. There is a reported link between alcohol ingestion and attractiveness to biting insects suggesting that people who drink alcohol are more attractive to mosquitoes [23, 24]. Anecdotal evidence for this certainly exists in relation to midges, however, in our study we found no such association. There was also no association between being bitten and the consumption of strongly flavoured foods (e.g. garlic, chilli and onions). This is contrary to popular belief as people commonly believe that garlic, in particular, makes you less attractive to biting insects. In support of our findings, Rajan et al. [25] found no effect of garlic consumption on how many mosquito bites were received in laboratory trials. There is also much speculation that other factors such as smoking, exercise, diet and reproductive stage can be related to whether people are bitten or not bitten by midges [26]. However, no such associations were found in this study. This suggests that these factors do not influence the cues used by midges during their search for a potential blood meal.
The interaction between the frequency of bites the first child receives and how often the parent is usually bitten suggests that the factor that makes someone attractive or unattractive to midges may be hereditary and could be controlled genetically. Kirk et al. [17] performed a survey on adolescent twins and also suggested that there was a strong genetic influence on frequency of being bitten by mosquitoes of children of certain ages. Other studies have shown that odours of twins are similar and thus may indicate the importance of genetic makeup on body odour [27]. Logan et al. [4, 19] demonstrated that the reason some people are bitten less by mosquitoes and midges is due to the production of repellent chemicals in their body odour. Therefore, it is plausible that the biosynthesis or release of such natural repellents is under genetic control and is hereditary [26]. Although differences in parental awareness of bites received by their children may also play a role here, there appears to be growing evidence that the level of attractiveness to biting insects is under genetic control. We are currently investigating further the possible genetic control of attractiveness to biting insects in molecular studies in our laboratories.
For the first time we also report the proportion of people who claim to react differently to bites from midges and our results suggest that, despite the immense annoyance factor caused by these insects, the majority of people sustain a minor reaction whilst 33% report having a bad reaction. Those that report having no reaction to midge bites are in the minority with a strong association between the frequency of being bitten and the reaction to bites. This may be a direct association with number of bites received (e.g. the more bites received, the higher the level of perceived reaction) or it may indicate that there is an indirect link between level of attractiveness and level of reaction, that does not relate to the number of bites received. Indeed the severity of cutaneous reactions to insect bites is known to differ between individuals and, therefore, there could be an association [28, 29]. Additionally, other studies have shown that people's perception of how attractive they are (in a questionnaire) matches well with their accurately measured level of attractiveness to mosquitoes in the laboratory [4, 19, 26]. However, no study has ever investigated this link and we cannot make definitive conclusions from our own study. Nevertheless, it does suggest that people's perception of how attractive they are to mosquitoes could be influenced by their reaction to the bites they receive rather than the number of bites. In support of this, Read et al. and Peng et al. [30, 31] also demonstrated a lower than expected correlation between reported bites of volunteers and the number of mosquitoes caught in traps using their body odours in a public perception of mosquito annoyance survey.
In our study we found a significant association of reaction to midge bites in relation to sex, which was seen in women having a significantly higher probability of a bad reaction to midge bites than men. Women may experience a greater reaction to midge bites or may have a greater awareness of bites received. Further sensitivity studies would need to be done to confirm this result, although other studies have also suggested, by measuring the level of mosquito saliva-specific antibodies in infants, that there may be similar differences between the sexes in terms of their reaction to bites from insects [32].
The study is not without limitations as our respondents were participants, spectators and organisers of a duathlon event and consequently the majority of respondents were young, fit and healthy. Additionally, it relied on subjective answers from volunteers rather than experimental measurement of level of attractiveness of the participants. However, measurement of every individual would have been logistically impossible. The data have been analysed using the answers to the questions "how often are you usually bitten when in Scotland?". Because of possible variation in people's ability to accurately remember, we compared this with their perceived level of being bitten on the day of the survey when there when people were being bitten and we found a strong positive association. We also have evidence from previous investigations that the perceived level of being bitten by biting insects is directly correlated to their actual level of "attractiveness" when measured accurately using a laboratory-based behavioural experiment with mosquitoes. Additionally, we have previously demonstrated that the level of attractiveness to midges is reflected in the response of mosquitoes to the same people, indicating that the results of this study could be extrapolated to instances where people are bitten by mosquitoes, including those that vector pathogens [4, 19].
Conclusions
In summary, we found that 14% of people do not attract midges and only a small proportion report no reaction to midge bites. This study suggests that midges prefer to bite men that are tall and women that have a large BMI, and that the tendency for a child to be bitten or not could be inherited from their parent. Also, women tend to react more than men to midge bites. Contrary to common belief diet, health and other lifestyle factors do not play a role in making people more or less attractive to midges. The study is questionnaire-based; therefore, the interpretation of the results may be limited by the subjectivity of the answers given by the respondents. Although the results are relevant only to the Scottish biting midge, the approach used here could be useful for investigating human-insect interactions for other insects, particularly those which transmit pathogens.
References
Blackwell A, Wadhams LJ, Mordue W: Electrophysiological and behavioural studies of the biting midge, Culicoides impunctatus Goetghebuer (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae): Interactions between some plant-derived repellent compounds and a host-odour attractant, 1-octen-3-ol. Physiological Entomology. 1997, 22 (2): 102-108. 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1997.tb01146.x.
Hendry G, Godwin G: Biting midges in Scottish forestry: a costly irritant or a trivial nuisance?. Scottish Forestry. 1988, 42: 113-119.
Mordue (Luntz) AJ: Biting midge chemical ecology. Biologist. 2003, 50 (4): 159-162.
Logan JG, Seal NJ, Cook JI, Stanczyk NM, Birkett MA, Clark SJ, Gezan SA, Wadhams LJ, Pickett JA, Mordue J: Identification of Human-Derived Volatile Chemicals That Interfere With Attraction of the Scottish Biting Midge and Their Potential Use as Repellents. J Med Entomol. 2009, 46 (2): 208-219. 10.1603/033.046.0205.
Brown RE: What is the role of the immune system in determining individually distinct body odours?. International Journal of Immunopharmacology. 1995, 17: 655-661. 10.1016/0192-0561(95)00052-4.
Muirhead-Thomson RC: Mosquito behaviour in relation to malaria transmission and control in the tropics. 1951, London: Edward Arnold & Co
Thomas TC: Biting activity of Anopheles gambiae. Brit Med J. 1951, 4744-1402.
Boreham PFL, Chandler JA, Jolly J: Incidence of Mosquitoes Feeding on Mothers and Babies at Kisumu, Kenya. J Trop Med Hyg. 1978, 81 (4): 63-67.
Bryan JH, Smalley ME: Use of ABO Blood-Groups as Markers for Mosquito Biting Studies. Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene. 1978, 72 (4): 357-360. 10.1016/0035-9203(78)90127-X.
Carnevale P, Frezil JL, Bosseno MF, Lepont F, Lancien J: Study of Agressivity of Anopheles gambiae a in Relation to Age and Sex of Human Subjects. B World Health Organ. 1978, 56 (1): 147-154.
Port GR, Boreham PFL, Bryan JH: The Relationship of Host Size to Feeding by Mosquitoes of the Anopheles gambiae Giles Complex (Diptera, Culicidae). B Entomol Res. 1980, 70 (1): 133-144. 10.1017/S0007485300009834.
Curtis CF, Lines JD, Ijumba J, Callaghan A, Hill N, Karimzad MA: The Relative Efficacy of Repellents against Mosquito Vectors of Disease. Med Vet Entomol. 1987, 1 (2): 109-119. 10.1111/j.1365-2915.1987.tb00331.x.
Michael E, Ramaiah KD, Hoti SL, Barker G, Paul MR, Yuvaraj J, Das PK, Grenfell BT, Bundy DA: Quantifying mosquito biting patterns on humans by DNA fingerprinting of bloodmeals. American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene. 2001, 65 (6): 722-728.
Clyde DF, Shute GT: Selective Feeding Habits of Anophelines Amongst Africans of Different Ages. American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene. 1958, 7 (5): 543-545.
Lindsay S, Ansell J, Selman C, Cox V, Hamilton K, Walraven G: Effect of pregnancy on exposure to malaria mosquitoes. Lancet. 2000, 355 (9219): 1972-1972. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02334-5.
Burkot TR: Non-Random Host Selection by Anopheline Mosquitoes. Parasitol Today. 1988, 4 (6): 156-162. 10.1016/0169-4758(88)90151-2.
Kirk KM, Eaves LJ, Meyer JM, Saul A, Martin NG: Twin study of adolescent genetic susceptibility to mosquito bites using ordinal and comparative rating data. Genet Epidemiol. 2000, 19 (2): 178-190. 10.1002/1098-2272(200009)19:2<178::AID-GEPI5>3.0.CO;2-3.
Roff DA, Bentzen P: The Statistical-Analysis of Mitochondrial-DNA Polymorphisms - Chi-2 and the Problem of Small Samples. Molecular Biology and Evolution. 1989, 6 (5): 539-545.
Logan JG, Birkett MA, Clark SJ, Powers S, Seal NJ, Wadhams LJ, Mordue AJ, Pickett JA: Identification of human-derived volatile chemicals that interfere with attraction of Aedes aegypti mosquitoes. J Chem Ecol. 2008, 34 (3): 308-322. 10.1007/s10886-008-9436-0.
Venter GJ, Hermanides KG, Boikanyo SN, Majatladi DM, Morey L: The effect of light trap height on the numbers of Culicoides midges collected under field conditions in South Africa. Vet Parasitol. 2009, 166 (3-4): 343-345. 10.1016/j.vetpar.2009.09.003.
Carpenter S, Mordue W, Mordue J: Selection of resting areas by emerging Culicoides impunctatus (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae) on downy birch (Betula pubescens). International Journal of Pest Management. 2008, 54 (1): 39-42. 10.1080/09670870701469138.
Rahm U: Zum Problem der Attraktion von Stechmücken durch den Menschen. Acta Trop. 1956, 13: 319-344.
Shirai Y, Tsuda T, Kitagawa S, Naitoh K, Seki T, Kamimura K, Morohashi M: Alcohol ingestion stimulates mosquito attraction. J Am Mosquito Contr. 2002, 18 (2): 91-96.
Bernier UR, Kline DL, Posey KH: Human emanations and related natural compounds that inhibit mosquito host-finding abilities. 2007, Boca Raton: CRC Press, Florida
Rajan TV, Hein M, Porte P, Wikel S: A double-blinded, placebo-controlled trial of garlic as a mosquito repellant: a preliminary study. Med Vet Entomol. 2005, 19 (1): 84-89. 10.1111/j.0269-283X.2005.00544.x.
Logan JG: Why do mosquitoes "choose" to bite some people more than others?. Outlooks on Pest Management. 2008, 19 (6): 280-283. 10.1564/19dec14.
Roberts SC, Gosling ML, Spector Tim, Miller Paul, Penn Dustin, Marion Petrie: Body Odor Similarity in Noncohabiting Twins. Chem Senses. 2005, 30: 651-656. 10.1093/chemse/bji058.
Mellanby K: Mans Reaction to Mosquito Bites. Nature. 1946, 158 (4021): 750-751. 10.1038/158751a0.
Mckiel JA, West AS: Effects of Repeated Exposures of Hypersensitive Humans and Laboratory Rabbits to Mosquito Antigens. Can J Zoolog. 1961, 39 (5): 597-&. 10.1139/z61-064.
Read NR, Rooker JR, Gathman JP: Public Perception of Mosquito Annoyance Measured by a Survey and Simultaneous Mosquito Sampling. J Am Mosquito Contr. 1994, 10 (1): 79-87.
Peng ZK, Beckett AN, Engler RJ, Hoffman DR, Ott NL, Simons FER: Immune responses to mosquito saliva in 14 individuals with acute systemic allergic reactions to mosquito bites. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology. 2004, 114 (5): 1189-1194. 10.1016/j.jaci.2004.08.014.
Peng ZK, Ho MK, Li CH, Simons FER: Evidence for natural desensitization to mosquito salivary allergens: mosquito saliva specific IgE and IgG levels in children. Annals of Allergy Asthma & Immunology. 2004, 93 (6): 553-556.
Pre-publication history
The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2458/10/275/prepub
Acknowledgements
First Group PLC are thanked for hosting the survey during their annual charitable 'First Monster Challenge' duathlon event on the shores of Loch Ness. We thank Lesley Smart and Barry Pye for their assistance in data collection and all the volunteers who participated in this study. Dr Kirsty England is thanked for her help with the manuscript and Dr JS Lemon and SNAP software in the design of the questionnaire. Rothamsted Research receives grant-aided support from the BBSRC.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
JGL, AJM, NMS, ENIW and JIC designed the research question. JGL, SJW and ENIW conducted the analysis and drafted the manuscript. JGL wrote the final manuscript. AJM, NMS, ENIW, JIC and SJW participated in writing the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.
Authors’ original submitted files for images
Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Rights and permissions
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
About this article
Cite this article
Logan, J.G., Cook, J.I., Stanczyk, N.M. et al. To bite or not to bite! A questionnaire-based survey assessing why some people are bitten more than others by midges. BMC Public Health 10, 275 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-10-275
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-10-275