Skip to main content

Impact on sales of adding a smaller serving size of beer and cider in licensed premises: an A-B-A reversal design

Abstract

Background

Smaller serving sizes of alcoholic drinks could reduce alcohol consumption across populations thereby lowering the risk of many diseases. The effect of modifying the available range of serving sizes of beer and cider in a real-world setting has yet to be studied. The current study assessed the impact on beer and cider sales of adding a serving size of draught beer and cider (2/3 pint) that was between the current smallest (1/2 pint) and largest (1 pint) standard serving sizes.

Methods

Twenty-two licensed premises in England consented to taking part in the study. The study used an ABA reversal design, set over three 4-weekly periods, with A representing the non-intervention periods, during which standard serving sizes were served and B the intervention period when a 2/3 pint serving size of draught beer and cider was added to the existing range, along with smaller 1/2 pint and larger 1 pint serving sizes. The primary outcome was the daily volume of beer and cider sold, extracted from sales data.

Results

Fourteen premises started the study, of which thirteen completed it. Twelve of those did so per protocol and were included in the primary analysis. After adjusting for pre-specified covariates, the intervention did not have a significant effect on the volume of beer and cider sold per day (3.14 ml; 95%CIs -2.29 to 8.58; p = 0.257).

Conclusions

In licensed premises, there was no evidence that adding a smaller serving size for draught beer and cider (2/3 pint) when the smallest (1/2 pint) and largest (1 pint) sizes were still available, affected the volume of beer and cider sold. Studies are warranted to assess the impact of removing the largest serving size.

Trial registration

ISRCTN: https://doi.org/10.1186/ISRCTN33169631 (08/09/2021), OSF: https://osf.io/xkgdb/ (08/09/2021).

Peer Review reports

Background

Excess alcohol consumption contributes to premature mortality and preventable morbidity [1], causing approximately three million global deaths per year and accounting for 5.1% of the global burden of disease [2]. Reducing alcohol consumption at the population level has been declared a global public health priority [3]. This is reflected in WHO Europe’s recent decision to commit all member states to a comprehensive plan for accelerating action on reducing alcohol consumption across the continent [4, 5]. Although recent years have seen a shift in the setting in which most alcohol is consumed, with more being consumed in homes rather than bars, pubs and restaurants [6], a large proportion of alcohol is still being consumed in licensed premises. For example, in 2019, almost a third of alcohol sold in Great Britain was bought and drunk in pubs, clubs, bars and restaurants [7], making licensed premises an important target for intervention.

Alcohol consumption in populations is influenced by a variety of factors, both at the individual, and environmental or contextual level [2, 8]. Individual factors include gender [9], family circumstances [10,11,12], social support [13] and socio-economic status [14]. Environmental factors include advertising, marketing, and product labelling, as well as opportunities for alcohol purchasing and consumption [15, 16] . For example, studies have shown that exposure to alcohol advertising can increase alcohol consumption [17,18,19], while restricting advertising can have a small impact on reducing consumption [20, 21]. Additional labelling of alcohol products, including health warning and calorie labels, may also have the potential to reduce alcohol use [22,23,24]. Furthermore, the physical availability of alcohol can influence consumption: the more readily alcohol is available, the more likely it is to be consumed [25]. In support of this, studies have shown that individuals living in neighbourhoods with more licensed premises, such as bars, tend to drink more [26, 27]. Decreasing the opportunity to buy alcohol by reducing its availability or its affordability can also reduce its consumption. For example, within retail settings, decreasing the proportion of alcoholic drinks for sale and increasing the proportion of non-alcoholic drinks—from 25% to 50% or 75%—can reduce the amount of alcohol selected and purchased [28]. Finally, there is a lot of evidence showing that increasing the affordability of alcohol by making it cheaper, increases its consumption [29, 30]. Conversely, decreasing its affordability decreases its consumption [31].

Interventions that involve changing the size of portions and containers of products that can harm health, including alcohol, also show promise. This follows from the well documented “portion size effect” for food, i.e. that people consume less when presented with smaller portions, packages, or tableware [32,33,34]. Until recently this effect had been neglected as a focus of study in relation to alcoholic drinks and the size of servings and containers in which these are served.  There is now evidence showing that larger wine glasses increase the volume of wine sold, and therefore consumed, in restaurants [35]. Smaller wine glasses might also reduce the amount of wine drunk in homes, although the evidence for this is very limited [36]. Drinking wine at home from smaller 50cl bottles, compared with standard 75cl bottles, may also reduce consumption [37], but the impact of 37.5cl bottles is less certain [36].

Interventions that target the sizes of servings for reducing alcohol consumption can be classified broadly into three groups [38]:

  1. i.

    removing the largest serving size from existing options;

  2. ii.

    reducing the smallest serving size (either by adding a new smaller size or reducing the existing smallest size);

  3. iii.

    adding a size smaller than the largest serving size to existing options

Thus far, only the first of these has been studied in relation to alcoholic drinks. In the first study to be conducted in real-world settings, removing the largest serving size of wine by the glass (most often 250ml) for four weeks decreased wine sales – a proxy for consumption – across 21 licenced premises by 7.6% [39]. This reflects findings from two prior studies in semi-naturalistic settings, in which the largest servings of wine and beer were removed and replaced with smaller servings. In both studies, the intervention reduced the volume of alcohol consumed on a single occasion [40].

Although potentially effective, interventions that involve restricting options, such as removing the largest serving sizes, are likely to evoke opposition both from the alcohol industry, given their potential to reduce sales of targeted drinks [41], and from the public, who tend to support such interventions less than information-based interventions [42]. Such opposition is arguably less likely with interventions that involve increasing existing options by adding new smaller serving sizes, with larger sizes remaining available. Such interventions do not restrict options and are therefore more likely to be perceived as acceptable to the public [42]. A small number of studies have evaluated these types of interventions in the context of food consumption. Three of these [43,44,45] found that adding smaller servings of hot meals and entrees to the menus of worksite cafeterias and restaurants resulted in a small but potentially meaningful proportion of customers choosing the smaller servings (between 5%–13% in worksite cafeterias and 19%–31% in restaurants). Although none of these studies assessed the amounts of food or energy purchased, one of them showed – by measurement of leftovers – that the intervention reduced overall energy consumed [45]. Finally, a more recent study found that adding a medium and a smaller serving size of packaged sausages to the default larger size offered in a supermarket reduced the amount of meat purchased [46]. No studies, however, have assessed the impact on alcohol consumption or sales of adding smaller serving sizes of drinks to existing options. In theory, the addition of a smaller serving size to a range of options could reduce alcohol consumption by better reflecting people's existing preferences for an ideal serving size [47, 48]. This would be the case when the largest serving sizes are considered too large but the smaller ones too small. It could also shape norms about what is an appropriate size [49].

The aim of the current study was to assess the impact on beer and cider sales of adding a serving size of draught beer and cider (2/3 pint) to the range of options available in licensed premises that was between the smallest (1/2 pint) and largest (1 pint) serving sizes. We hypothesised that adding a 2/3 pint serving size would reduce the volume of beer and cider sold.

Methods

The study protocol and statistical analysis plan were pre-registered (ISRCTN: ISRCTN33169631 https://doi.org/10.1186/ISRCTN33169631 (registration date 08/09/2021) Open Science Framework: registration https://osf.io/xkgdb/ (registration date 08/09/2021) protocol: https://osf.io/sxe9t; statistical analysis plan: https://osf.io/9sr5j).

Study design

The study used an ABA treatment reversal design consisting of three consecutive four-week periods, in which A represented the non-intervention periods during which the usual range of serving sizes was available, and B represented the intervention period.

Setting and context

The study was conducted in pubs, bars and restaurants in England.

Participants

Participants were 22 licensed premises in England. Their location and other characteristics are shown in Table 1. Approximately half (54%) were pubs, and were located in London (59%).

Table 1 Characteristics of recruited licensed premises

To be eligible to take part in the study, licensed premises had to meet the following criteria:

  1. i.

    sell a minimum of 100 pints of beer and cider on average per week

  2. ii.

    be willing to introduce 2/3 pints for all beers and ciders sold on tap

  3. iii.

    have an electronic point of sale (EPOS) till system to record daily sales of all drinks and their served sizes

  4. iv.

    be primarily indoor, permanent establishments in a fixed location; i.e. not purposefully temporary or time-limited (e.g. pop-up), or mobile venues (e.g. vans)

Sample size calculation

Power simulations [50] suggested that 14 sites using an ABA reversal design with each period lasting four weeks would be needed in order to have 80% power to detect a predicted effect of 8.4% reduction in log beer and cider volume. Recruitment was conducted in two waves. Due to a large level of attrition during the first wave, we overrecruited in the second wave. The final recruited sample consisted of 22 sites.

Intervention

Licensed premises added a new serving size of 2/3 pints to their existing range of draught beers and ciders. The 2/3 pint was offered in addition to the existing larger serving size of one pint and smaller serving size of half-pint, with proportionate pricing, i.e. with a price which was linear-by-volume between the pint and half-pint sizes, as confirmed during fidelity checks. The researchers provided the 2/3 pint glasses. Premises adopted a range of strategies to promote the new serving size, including signs, posters, advertising on blackboards and adding it to menus. These were not controlled by the researcher team.

Within the TIPPME intervention typology for changing environments to change behaviour [51], the type of intervention used in the current study was ‘Size’, and focused on the ‘Product’ itself, i.e. the alcoholic drink. The TIPPME intervention typology provides a framework to reliably classify and describe interventions which alter small-scale (proximal) physical micro-environments to change selection, purchase and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco products. In the typology, interventions are described according to two dimensions: type of intervention (changing the Availability, Position, Functionality, Presentation, Size, Information) as well as the spatial focus of the intervention (whether a Product, a Related Object or the Wider Environment is targeted) [51].

Procedure

Data were collected during two phases: between September 2021 and December 2021 and between February 2022 and May 2022. There were no COVID restrictions in place during the study. Potentially eligible premises were identified through a publicly available database (http://whatpub.com). Those based in one of eight geographical locations where it was feasible for the research team to conduct fidelity checks were sent email invitations to participate in the study. Premises replying with an interest in taking part were sent additional information about the study and were assessed for eligibility over the telephone. Eligible premises agreeing to participate provided written informed consent for doing so.

Recruited premises changed their available serving sizes for draught beer and cider on two occasions over a period of 12 weeks, first to add a 2/3 pint serving size during the intervention period (B), and second to remove it during the second non-intervention period (A). Till systems, menus and signs were updated as appropriate to reflect the available serving sizes. Premises managers were reminded via email one day before each required change.

Premises were paid £250 (plus 20% VAT) for participating in the study and providing all requested data. They were also allowed to keep the 2/3 pint glasses and were reimbursed for the costs of any necessary changes to menus and signs.

Measures

Primary outcome

Daily volume (in milliliters (ml)) of all beer and cider sold (draught as well as bottled), extracted from sale records.

Secondary outcomes

The following outcomes were extracted from sales records:

  1. i

    Daily volume (in ml) of beer and cider sold by each serving size:

    • 1/3 pint (189ml)

    • 1/2 pint (284ml)

    • 330ml bottle

    • 440ml can

    • 500ml bottle

    • pint (568ml)

    • 600 ml

    • 5 L (5000 ml)

  2. ii

    Daily volume (in ml) of wine sold

  3. iii

    Daily revenue from food, alcoholic and non-alcoholic drinks

Covariates

Given that daily temperature, day of the week, season and holidays can influence alcohol sales [52, 53], the following covariates were considered:

  1. i.

    Maximum daily local temperature

  2. ii.

    Dummy variables for special events (e.g. Bank Holidays, other holidays, major sporting events, etc.)

  3. iii.

    Total revenue (as a proxy for premises busyness)

  4. iv.

    Dummy variables for day of the week

  5. v.

    Study day from start of a period (number from 1 to 84)

  6. vi.

    Season at start of study: autumn or winter

Data analysis

Unadjusted summaries of the volume of beer and cider sold during the non-intervention and intervention periods were calculated both overall and for each serving size. Outliers in the daily data were identified using range checks, scatter plots, median absolute deviation values and histograms. The potential outliers identified were all deemed genuine values and it was assumed that the two model covariates (total revenue – a proxy for site busyness – and special events) could handle these to ensure no outliers in the model residual diagnostics.

Primary analysis

A generalized linear mixed model (being generalized additive models which can accommodate heterogeneity) was used to predict daily volume of beer and cider sales according to study period (A vs B). Premises were treated as a random factor and heterogeneity between premises was modelled. The analysis included pre-specified covariates for day of the week, study day and total revenue. An overall effect was estimated from this model. The mean difference and associated 95% confidence intervals (CI) and p-value, as well as a Cohen’s d effect size and its 95% CI were calculated. All regression model diagnostics (residual plots, worm plots) were checked and were satisfactory once a variance stabilising transformation was used (square root).

Only premises that completed the study per protocol and provided all primary outcome data were included in the primary analysis.

Sensitivity analyses

Four sets of sensitivity analyses were conducted to check the robustness of the primary analysis:

  1. 1.

    Generalized linear regression analysis, repeating the primary analysis but taking into account three additional covariates: i) total number of special events in each period; ii) season at the start of the study; iii) maximum daily local temperature.

  2. 2.

    Generalized linear regression analysis, repeating the primary analysis but adding daily-level data from all premises, including those that violated the protocol for intervention implementation (intention to treat analysis).

  3. 3.

    Generalized linear regression analysis, repeating the primary analysis but including the two non-intervention periods as separate factor levels (i.e. using A1, B & A2 levels for the periods). This was conducted to assess whether there were differences in the two A periods and whether aggregating the data from these two periods in the primary analysis was justified.

  4. 4.

    As data might be less variable when aggregated at the period level, a generalized linear regression analysis using period-level data to compare mean daily sales during period A (aggregate value for 2 four-week A period) and mean daily sales during period B (aggregate value for 1 four-week B period).

Secondary analyses

For the secondary outcomes, generalized linear mixed models were used, with the distribution of the data assessed by model diagnostics dictating which model was most appropriate (e.g. Poisson regression).

The following secondary analyses were conducted:

  1. 1.

    Regression analyses to predict the number of beer and cider drinks sold in each serving size according to the study Period (A vs B).

  2. 2.

    A regression analysis to predict the daily volume of wine sold according to the study Period (A vs B). The analysis included covariates for day of the week, study day and total revenue.

  3. 3.

    A regression analysis to predict total revenue from all food and drink according to the study Period (A vs B). The analysis included covariates for day of the week and study day.

Results

The flow of premises through the study is shown in Fig. 1. Twenty-two licensed premises were recruited from 2594 that were contacted, a recruitment rate of 0.8%. Thirteen completed the study and provided all primary outcome data, a retention rate of 59%. One premises was excluded from the primary analysis for violating the protocol and continuing to sell 2/3 pints during the second non-intervention period, as identified by inspection of their sales data.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow of premises through study

Primary outcome: volume of beer and cider sales

The unadjusted mean daily volume of beer and cider sold per premises during the non-intervention periods (A) was 83,179.32ml (sd = 92,549.14) and 81,296.28ml (sd = 94,259.47) during the intervention period (B). After accounting for pre-specified covariates, the difference in the volume of beer and cider sold per day during the intervention period (B) compared to the volume sold during the two non-intervention periods (A) was not significantly different (square root of volume = 3.14ml, 95%CIs -2.29 to 8.58; p = 0.257) (Table 2). There was heterogeneity between premises (sigma coefficients were statistically significant at p < 5%; Supplement, Table S1). Figure 2 shows the effect of the intervention on beer and cider sales overall and for each premises (Supplement, Table S1).

Table 2 Mixed effects regression results (95% CI) predicting the square root of volume (ml) of beer and cider per day (n = 12)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Percentage change in sales of beer and cider from adding a smaller (2/3 pint) serving size

Sensitivity analyses

Results were unchanged when performing an intention‐to‐treat analysis (n = 13) that included the one premises that had violated the protocol (Table S2, supplement), and when including additional covariates in the model (Table S3). Using period-level data rather than daily-level data, also did not show a significant difference in the volume of beer and cider sold during the intervention compared to the non-intervention periods (-265.3ml; 95% CIS -6756.68 to 6226.01; p = 0.93).

Daily sales of beer and cider did not significantly differ during the two non-intervention periods (A) (Table S4, supplement), justifying the modelling choice of combining data from both non-intervention periods for the primary analysis.

Secondary outcomes

Beer and cider sales by serving size

The unadjusted mean daily volume of beer and cider sold in different serving sizes is show in Table 3.

Table 3 Unadjusted mean (sd) volume of beer and cider (ml) sold per day, overall and according to serving size (n = 13) and results of regression analyses assessing differences in the number of beers and ciders sold in each size according to study period

A Poisson regression (for skewed data) showed that the number of 1/3 pints sold increased during the intervention compared to the non-intervention periods (IRR = with 1.84; 0.611 95% CIs 0.149 to 1.07; p < 0.01). However, very few 1/3 pints were sold overall during the study. Additional analysis revealed no evidence that the proportion of days during which any 1/3pints were sold varied according to study period (-0.22; 95%CIs -1.16 to 0.64; p = 0.62).

Negative binomial regressions (for skewed data) found no evidence that the number of beers and ciders sold in the following serving sizes differed according to study period: 1/2 pints (-0.018 95%CIs -0.088 to 0.053; p = 0.62); 440ml (0.042 95%CIs -0.136 to 0,220; p = 0.64); 600ml (-0.087 95% CIs -0.549 to 0.373; p = 0.708).

Mixed effect regressions found no evidence that the number of beers and ciders sold in the following serving sizes differed according to study period: 330ml (-0.010; 95% CIs -0.150 to 0.130; p = 0.887); 500ml (0.121 95% CIs -0.0003 to 0.2442; p = 0.051); pint (0.092 95% CIs -0.118 to 0.302; p = 0.39); 5L (0.004 95% CIs -0.116 to 0.124; p = 0.949).

A regression estimating the daily volume of 2/3 pints sold during the intervention period only showed that this was significantly different from zero (square root of volume = 1.30 95% CI 1.22 to 1.38 p < 0.001).

Volume of wine sold

The unadjusted mean daily volume of wine sold during the non-intervention periods (A) was 4273.2ml (sd = 5291.9) and 3979.6ml (sd = 4579.9) during the intervention period (B). There was no evidence of a difference in the volume of wine sold per day between the intervention and non-intervention periods (square root of volume = -1.00, 95%CIs -3.48 to 1.46; p = 0.424).

Revenue

The unadjusted mean daily revenue during the non-intervention periods (A) was £2487.6 (sd = £2504.2) and £2336.0 (sd = £2370.6) during the intervention period (B). There was no evidence of a difference in total daily revenue (£) between the intervention and non-intervention periods (square root of revenue = -0.133, 95% CI -1.126 to 0.860; p = 0.793).

Discussion

There was no evidence in licensed premises that adding a smaller serving size (2/3 pint) that was between the smallest and largest sizes to the existing options for draught beer and cider affected the volume of beer and cider sold. There was also no evidence that wine sales or daily revenues were impacted by the intervention.

These findings did not support the study hypothesis that adding 2/3 pints to existing options of beer and ciders reduces sales. They also appear to differ from findings of the few studies assessing the impact of adding smaller servings on food consumption [43,44,45,46]. Three of these studies [43,44,45] had different outcome measures to the current study –proportion of people selecting smaller servings and/or total energy consumed – which could explain the different conclusions. The one study that used a similar outcome measure to the current study, i.e. amount of food purchased, was conducted in a supermarket where customers had physical access to all available serving sizes [46], in contrast to the current study, in which people had to order a serving size in order to see it.

There are two likely explanations for the lack of an evident effect of smaller serving sizes in the current study. First, the current study was underpowered to detect anything other than a large effect of 2/3 pints on beer and cider sales. Studies conducted in real-world settings with greater power to detect smaller but still meaningful effects are therefore warranted.

Second, the findings reflect a true null effect, operating through at least two routes. While some might have shifted from a pint to a 2/3 pint, a similar number might have shifted from a 1/2 pint to a 2/3 pint. Alternatively, the new size was not selected enough to have an effect. Reflecting this, although the volume of beer and cider sold in 2/3 pints during the intervention period differed from zero, it was only 1.5% of the total volume of beer and cider sold. Furthermore, 2/3 pints might not have been selected more often because customers were unaware of the new size, or despite being aware they did not prefer it. Premises adopted a range of strategies to advertise the new serving size, including signs, posters and adding it to menus, which were not evaluated. It is also not known how the new size was promoted by staff. Invitations to downsize serving sizes at the point of sale can shift customers to smaller sizes [54]. Given that premises in the study were likely motivated to sell larger sizes to increase revenue, such strategies might have not been used.

The role of norms is a final factor that may explain the lack of impact of the intervention. People hold social and personal norms for what constitutes an appropriate serving size to consume [49, 55]. The addition of a new serving can shape these social norms but it is possible that in the present study there was not enough time for this to occur. The customary serving size for draught beer and cider in the UK is a pint [56] and has been for centuries [56]. Shifting this well-established norm could require considerably more time than the four weeks the new size was on offer in this study.

Implications for research and policy

If the intervention assessed in this study is found to have an effect in subsequent, adequately powered studies, changing alcohol licensing regulations to make 2/3 pints for draught beer and cider available mandatorily in licensed premises, in the same way as 125 ml serving sizes of wine are, might be worth consideration. This is especially important given that interventions that involve adding a smaller serving size to existing options are likely to be better accepted by businesses and the public than those that involve removing or restricting options [57,58,59].

A potentially more effective intervention is to remove the largest serving size from existing options. Removing the largest serving of wine by the glass from sale in licensed premises reduces wine sales by an estimated 6.5%, without affecting total revenues [39]. In the case of draught beer and cider in the UK, the largest and most popular serving size is the pint (568ml) [56] Future research should assess the impact on the volume of beer and cider consumed of removing the offer of pints in licensed premises.

Strengths and limitations

The strength of the present study is that it is the first to estimate the impact of increasing the available options for alcoholic drinks in licensed premises by adding a smaller serving size, as a method for reducing excess alcohol consumption. A further strength is the use of objective measures of impact. The study, however, has several limitations. First, the generalisability of the findings is limited by the majority of premises studied being in London, and constituting a very small proportion of those approached. Second, the study relied on an A-B-A design, which has a higher risk of bias than experimental designs, although the analyses attempted to account for potential confounding variables. Third, although all premises promoted the introduction of 2/3 pints using posters, advertising on blackboards and making changes to menus, the study neither controlled for nor evaluated the strategies premises adopted. As such, it is not known whether variations in how 2/3pints were advertised could have influenced the findings. Finally, outcomes concerned sales rather than actual consumption, although sales are a valid, practicable, commonly used [60,61,62] proxy for consumption [60,61,62,63,64].

Conclusion

In conclusion, increasing the available serving sizes for draught beer and cider in licensed premises by adding a smaller serving size (2/3 pint) when the largest size (1 pint) was still available, did not provide evidence of an effect on the volume of beer and cider sold. Additional studies are warranted with more power to detect smaller effect sizes for the intervention, as well as to assess the impact of removing the largest serving size.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset generated during the current study is available on the Open Science Framework repository (https://osf.io/evm6u).

References

  1. Gakidou EAA, Abajobir AA, Abate KH, Abbafati C, Abbas KM, et al. Global, regional, and national comparative risk assessment of 84 behavioural, environmental and occupational, and metabolic risks or clusters of risks, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2016. The Lancet. 2017;390(10100):1345–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. World Health Organization. Alcohol 2022 [Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/alcohol.

  3. Rehm JSK. Alcohol and mortality: global alcohol-attributable deaths from cancer, liver cirrhosis, and injury in 2010. Alcohol Res. 2014;35(2):174.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. WHO. Seventy-second Regional Committee for Europe: Tel Aviv, 12–14 September 2022: European framework for action on alcohol 2022–2025, 2022.

  5. Movendi International. WHO Europe Regional Committee Meeting Adopts Historic Framework for Action on Alcohol 2022 [Available from: https://movendi.ngo/media-release/who-europe-regional-committee-meeting-adopts-historic-framework-for-action-on-alcohol/.

  6. Holloway SL, Jayne M, Valentine G. ‘Sainsbury’s is my local’: English alcohol policy, domestic drinking practices and the meaning of home. Trans Inst Br Geogr. 2008;33(4):532–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Beer B, Association P. Statistical Handbook. London: Brewing Publications Limited; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Burton R, Henn C, Lavoie D, et al. A rapid evidence review of the effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of alcohol control policies: an English perspective. The Lancet. 2017;389(10078):1558–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Hughes TL, Wilsnack SC, Kantor LW. The influence of gender and sexual orientation on alcohol use and alcohol-related problems: toward a global perspective. Alcohol Res. 2016;38(1):121.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Cruz JE, Emery RE, Turkheimer E. Peer network drinking predicts increased alcohol use from adolescence to early adulthood after controlling for genetic and shared environmental selection. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(5):1390.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Dawson DA. The link between family history and early onset alcoholism: earlier initiation of drinking or more rapid development of dependence? J Stud Alcohol. 2000;61(5):637–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Mares SH, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Burk WJ, et al. Parental alcohol-specific rules and alcohol use from early adolescence to young adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(7):798–805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Ramirez R, Hinman A, Sterling S, et al. Peer influences on adolescent alcohol and other drug use outcomes. J Nurs Scholarsh. 2012;44(1):36–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Huckle T, You RQ, Casswell S. Socio-economic status predicts drinking patterns but not alcohol-related consequences independently. Addiction. 2010;105(7):1192–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Hughes K, Quigg Z, Eckley L, et al. Environmental factors in drinking venues and alcohol-related harm: the evidence base for European intervention. Addiction. 2011;106:37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Marteau TM, Hollands GJ, Nambiar D, et al. Changing behaviour at scale to prevent NCDs. In: Banatvala N, Bovet P, eds. Noncommunicable Diseases: A Compendium: Routledge 2023:347–54.

  17. Grenard JL, Dent CW, Stacy AW. Exposure to alcohol advertisements and teenage alcohol-related problems. Pediatrics. 2013;131(2):e369–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Koordeman R, Anschutz DJ, Engels RC. The effect of alcohol advertising on immediate alcohol consumption in college students an experimental study. Alcoholism. 2012;36(5):874–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Roberts B, Murphy A, Chikovani I, et al. Individual and community level risk-factors for alcohol use disorder among conflict-affected persons in Georgia. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(5):e98299.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Brown KG, Stautz K, Hollands GJ, et al. The cognitive and behavioural impact of alcohol promoting and alcohol warning advertisements: an experimental study. Alcohol Alcohol. 2016;51(3):354–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Stautz K, Brown KG, King SE, et al. Immediate effects of alcohol marketing communications and media portrayals on consumption and cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Zhao J, Stockwell T, Vallance K, et al. The effects of alcohol warning labels on population alcohol consumption: an interrupted time series analysis of alcohol sales in Yukon, Canada. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2020;81(2):225–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Clarke N, Pechey E, Mantzari E, et al. Impact of health warning labels communicating the risk of cancer on alcohol selection: an online experimental study. Addiction. 2021;116(1):41–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Clarke N, Ferrar J, Pechey E, et al. Impact of health warnings and calorie labels on selection and purchasing of alcoholic and non-alcoholic drinks: a randomised controlled trial. medRχiv (preprint). https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.07.22.22277929.

  25. Stockwell T, Gruenewald PJ. Controls on the physical availability of alcohol. In: Heather N, Stockwell T, eds. The essential handbook of treatment and prevention of alcohol problems: Wiley 2004:213–33.

  26. Foster S, Trapp G, Hooper P, et al. Liquor landscapes: does access to alcohol outlets influence alcohol consumption in young adults? Health Place. 2017;45:17–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Freisthler B, Wernekinck U. Examining how the geographic availability of alcohol within residential neighborhoods, activity spaces, and destination nodes is related to alcohol use by parents of young children. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2022;233:109352.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Clarke N, Blackwell AK, Ferrar J, et al. Impact on alcohol selection and online purchasing of changing the proportion of available non-alcoholic versus alcoholic drinks: a randomised controlled trial. PLoS Med. 2023;20(3):e1004193.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Sharma A, Sinha K, Vandenberg B. Pricing as a means of controlling alcohol consumption. British Med Bull. 2017;123:1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Xu X, Chaloupka FJ. The effects of prices on alcohol use and its consequences. Alcohol Res Health. 2011;34(2):236.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Wyper GM, Mackay DF, Fraser C, et al. Evaluating the impact of alcohol minimum unit pricing on deaths and hospitalisations in Scotland: a controlled interrupted time series study. The Lancet. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(23)00497-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Marteau TM, et al. Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015(9).

  33. Clarke N, Pechey E, Pechey R, et al. Size and shape of plates and size of wine glasses and bottles: impact on self-serving of food and alcohol. BMC Psychol. 2021;9(1):1–12.

  34. Zlatevska N DC, Holden SS. Sizing up the Effect of Portion Size on Consumption: A Meta-Analytic Review. J Mark. 2014;78(3):140–54.

  35. Pilling M, Clarke N, Pechey R, et al. The effect of wine glass size on volume of wine sold: a mega-analysis of studies in bars and restaurants. Addiction. 2020;115(9):1660–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Mantzari E, Ventsel M, Ferrar J, et al. Impact of wine bottle and glass sizes on wine consumption at home: a within and between households randomised controlled trial. Addiction. 2022;117(12):3037–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.16005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Codling S, Mantzari E, Sexton O, et al. Impact of bottle size on in-home consumption of wine: a randomized controlled cross-over trial. Addiction. 2020;115(12):2280.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Mantzari E, Marteau TM. Impact of sizes of servings, glasses and bottles on alcohol consumption: a narrative review. Nutrients. 2022;14(20):4244. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14204244.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Mantzari E, Ventsel M, Pechey E, et al. Impact on wine sales of removing the largest serving size by the glass: a treatment reversal trial in 21 pubs, bars and restaurants. PLOS Medicine under revision

  40. Kersbergen I, Oldham M, Jones A, et al. Reducing the standard serving size of alcoholic beverages prompts reductions in alcohol consumption. Addiction. 2018;113(9):1598–608.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Maani N, Van Schalkwyk M, Filippidis F, et al. Manufacturing doubt: Assessing the effects of independent vs industry-sponsored messaging about the harms of fossil fuels, smoking, alcohol, and sugar sweetened beverages. SSM-Population Health. 2022;17:101009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Diepeveen S, Ling T, Suhrcke M, et al. Public acceptability of government intervention to change health-related behaviours: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Vermeer WM, Leeuwis FH, Koprulu S, et al. The process evaluation of two interventions aimed at portion size in worksite cafeterias. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2012;25(2):180–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-277X.2011.01219.x[publishedOnlineFirst:20111203].

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Vermeer WM, Steenhuis IH, Poelman MP. Small, medium, large or supersize? The development and evaluation of interventions targeted at portion size. Int J Obes. 2014;38(Suppl 1):S13–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2014.84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Berkowitz S, Marquart L, Mykerezi E, et al. Reduced-portion entrées in a worksite and restaurant setting: impact on food consumption and waste. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(16):3048–54. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1368980016001348.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Vandenbroele J, Slabbinck H, Van Kerckhove A, et al. Curbing portion size effects by adding smaller portions at the point of purchase. Food Qual Prefer. 2018;64:82–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.10.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Marteau TM, Hollands GJ, Pechey R, et al. Changing the assortment of available food and drink for leaner, greener diets. BMJ. 2022;377:e069848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Pechey R, Hollands GJ, Marteau TM. Explaining the effect on food selection of altering availability: two experimental studies on the role of relative preferences. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Robinson E, Kersbergen I. Portion size and later food intake: evidence on the “normalizing” effect of reducing food portion sizes. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018;107(4):640–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Green P, MacLeod CJ, Nakagawa S. SIMR: an R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7(4):493–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210x.12504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Hollands GJ, Bignardi G, Johnston M, et al. The TIPPME intervention typology for changing environments to change behaviour. Nat Human Behav. 2017;1(8):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Hirche M, Haensch J, Lockshin L. Comparing the day temperature and holiday effects on retail sales of alcoholic beverages–a time-series analysis. Int J Wine Bus Res. 2021;33:432–55.

  53. de Vocht F, Brown J, Beard E, et al. Temporal patterns of alcohol consumption and attempts to reduce alcohol intake in England. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Schwartz J, Riis J, Elbel B, et al. Inviting consumers to downsize fast-food portions significantly reduces calorie consumption. Health Aff. 2012;31(2):399–407. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2011.0224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Robinson E, Henderson J, Keenan GS, et al. When a portion becomes a norm: Exposure to a smaller vs. larger portion of food affects later food intake. Food Qual Pref. 2019;75:113–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. de Moor D. The ultimate beer measures table 2017 [Available from: https://desdemoor.co.uk/the-ultimate-beer-measures-table/.

  57. Cullerton K, Donnet T, Lee A, et al. Playing the policy game: a review of the barriers to and enablers of nutrition policy change. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(14):2643–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Freudenberg N. Lethal but legal: corporations, consumption, and protecting public health: Oxford University Press 2014.

  59. Riis J. Opportunities and barriers for smaller portions in food service: lessons from marketing and behavioral economics. Int J Obes. 2014;38(Suppl 1):S19-24. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2014.85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Clarke N, Pechey R, Pilling M, et al. Wine glass size and wine sales: four replication studies in one restaurant and two bars. BMC Res Notes. 2019;12(1):1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Pechey R, Couturier D-L, Hollands GJ, et al. Does wine glass size influence sales for on-site consumption? A multiple treatment reversal design. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Pechey R, Couturier D-L, Hollands GJ, et al. Wine glass size and wine sales: a replication study in two bars. BMC Res Notes. 2017;10(1):1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Hinton ECBJ, Fay SH, Wilkinson LL, Ferriday D, Rogers PJ, et al. Using photography in ‘the restaurant of the future’. A useful way to assess portion selection and plate cleaning? Appetite. 2013;1(63):31–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Vermote MVV, Stok M, Mullie P, D’Hondt E, Deforche B, et al. The effect of a portion size intervention on French fries consumption, plate waste, satiety and compensatory caloric intake: an on-campus restaurant experiment. Nutr J. 2018;17(1):43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None

Transparency declaration

The lead author (the manuscript's guarantor) affirms that the manuscript is an honest, accurate, and transparent account of the study being reported; that no important aspects of the study have been omitted; and that any discrepancies from the study as originally planned (and, if relevant, registered) have been explained.

Funding

Collaborative Award in Science from Wellcome (Behaviour Change by Design: 206853/Z/17/Z) awarded to Theresa Marteau, Paul Fletcher, Gareth Hollands, Marcus Munafò.

The funder had no involvement in any part of the study, including in the writing of the manuscript and the decision to submit it for publication. The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of Wellcome.

For the purpose of Open Access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

TMM, IL, GJH conceptualized and designed the study with contributions from EM. EM coordinated the study and led on recruitment, data collection and cleaning, along with MV & EP. MP led on the statistical analysis. EM, GJH, and TMM drafted the manuscript with all authors providing critical revisions. TMM is guarantor.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Theresa M. Marteau.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study, including all experimental protocols, was approved by the University of Cambridge Psychology Research Ethics Committee (reference no: PRE.2019.035). All methods were carried out in accordance with the committee’s relevant guidelines and regulations. Managers or owners of premises agreeing to participate provided written informed consent for doing so. As only licensed premises were recruited, not individual customers, consent to participate in the study was obtained only from the managers or owners of participating premises and not their customers.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

All authors have completed the Unified Competing Interest form (available on request from the corresponding author) and declare: no support from any organisation for the submitted work; no financial relationships with any organisations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous three years; and no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mantzari, E., Ventsel, M., Pechey, E. et al. Impact on sales of adding a smaller serving size of beer and cider in licensed premises: an A-B-A reversal design. BMC Public Health 23, 1239 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-16163-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-023-16163-z

Keywords