Skip to main content

Table 4 The relationship between physical activity and cognitive development

From: Systematic review of the relationships between physical activity and health indicators in the early years (0-4 years)

# of studies

Design

Quality assessment

# of participants

Absolute effect

Quality

Risk of bias

Inconsistency

Indirectness

Imprecision

Other

Mean baseline age ranged from 18.3 weeks-4.94 years; where mean age was not reported, baseline age ranged from 12 months-5 years. Data were collected by RCT, clustered RCT, non-randomized intervention, cross-over trial, and cross-sectional study designs. Cognitive development was assessed by psychomotor skills (objectively measured), time on task (direct observation), early literacy and language skills (objectively measured), creativity (direct observation; Thinking Creatively in Action and Movement test), attention (direct observation), attention span (proxy-report interview; proxy-report Child Temperament Questionnaire), literacy skills (self-report; Woodcock Johnson, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test), math skills (self-report; Woodcock Johnson Applied Problems subscale), language development (objectively measured; Gessell Developmental Schedules – Development Quotient), free and cued word recall (objectively measured), cognitive function (objectively measured; Herbst Test), and sustained attention and response inhibition (objectively measured; Picture Deletion Task for Preschoolers).

2

RCTa

Serious risk of biasb

No serious inconsistency

No serious indirectness

No serious imprecision

None

454

The PA interventions (planned passive cycling or structured/organized PA) were favourably associated with improved cognitive development in 2 studies [90, 91].

MODERATEc

1

Clustered RCTd

No serious risk of bias

No serious inconsistency

No serious indirectness

No serious imprecision

None

125

The PA intervention (physical exercises to enact meanings of words) was favourably associated with improved cognitive development [110].

The PA intervention (physical exercises unrelated to words) was favourably associated with improved cognitive development (cued recall of words but not free recall of words) [110].

HIGH

4

Non-randomized interventione

Serious risk of biasf

No serious inconsistency

No serious indirectness

No serious imprecision

None

460

The PA interventions (structured/organized PA, free play and structured PA, or academic MVPA lessons) were favourably associated with improved cognitive development (only in intervention sites that actively participated in the intervention in 1 study; for alliteration in 2/2 studies; and for rhyming and picture naming in 1/2 studies) in 4 studies [93, 111,112,113].

VERY LOWg

3

Cross-over trialh

Serious risk of biasi

No serious inconsistency

No serious indirectness

No serious imprecision

None

182

The PA condition (structured/organized PA or MVPA breaks) was favourably associated with improved cognitive development (sustained attention but not response inhibition in 1 study) in 2 studies [114, 115].

Recess conditions were favourably associated with cognitive development in 1 study [116].

VERY LOWj

3

Cross-sectionalk

Serious risk of biasl

No serious inconsistency

No serious indirectness

No serious imprecision

None

3138

TPA was unfavourably associated with cognitive development in 1 study [101] and not associated with cognitive development in 1 study [109].

MVPA was not associated with cognitive development in 1 study [109].

Outdoor PA (at child care) was not associated with cognitive development in 1 study [58].

VERY LOWm

  1. MVPA: moderate- to vigorous-intensity physical activity; PA: physical activity; RCT: randomized controlled trial; TPA: total physical activity
  2. aIncludes 2 RCTs [90, 91]
  3. bNo intention-to-treat analysis; parent-child dyads were excluded if they did not carry out the management plan or if they became sick during the study and the physical activity program was interrupted in 1 study [90]. Physical activity was not measured, so it is unknown if the intervention significantly changed physical activity in 2 studies [90, 91]
  4. cQuality of evidence was downgraded from “high” to “moderate” because of serious risk of bias
  5. dIncludes 1 clustered RCT [110]
  6. eIncludes 4 non-randomized interventions [93, 111,112,113]
  7. fPhysical activity was not measured, so it is unknown if the intervention significantly changed physical activity in in 2 studies [93, 113]
  8. gQuality of evidence was downgraded from “low” to “very low” because of serious risk of bias
  9. hIncludes 3 cross-over trials [114,115,116]
  10. iCondition was not randomly assigned in 1 study [116]. Physical activity was not measured, so it is unknown if there were significant differences in physical activity between conditions in 2 studies [114, 116]. Unclear what conditions had significant differences in the outcome measure in 1 study [116]
  11. jQuality of evidence was downgraded from “low” to “very low” because of serious risk of bias
  12. kIncludes 3 cross-sectional studies [58, 101, 109]
  13. lConvenience sample was used in 1 study [101]. Physical activity was measured only during child care in 2 studies [58, 109]. No potential confounders were adjusted for in 2 adjusted studies [101, 109]. No psychometric properties reported for the subjective physical activity measure or the outcome measure in 1 study [101]
  14. mQuality of evidence was downgraded from “low” to “very low” because of serious risk of bias