Skip to main content

Table 1 Results of the included interventions

From: Information-based cues at point of choice to change selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco products: a systematic review

Author, reference and country

Design

Study setting

Product and cue category

Information-based cue intervention

Comparison

Duration

Outcome measures

Results

Result

Medium of cue

Content of cue

Location of cue

Cues concerning attention

Achabal 1987 [25] USA

Randomised controlled trial

Supermarket

Fruit and vegetables

Colour signs

Pictures of produce (broccoli, cabbage, carrots, cauliflower, kiwi, tomatoes), plus purchase selection information

Signs placed in a holder strip six feet off the floor, directly about the produce

No signs in store Additional arm: Nutrition signs, which included the same information as the intervention arm, plus calorie content and a panel of key nutrients.

4 weeks (plus 4 weeks pre-test, and 4 weeks post-test)

Weekly sales data

No variation in sales were observed by the presence of the sign, with no significant effect of the sign (F = 0.684, p = < 0.505). Additional analysis comparing matched store data, found no differences in the number of times the sales were greater in the intervention stores than the control stores.

Payne 2015 [29] USA

Pilot study

Supermarket

Fruit and vegetables

Brightly coloured placards

Graphics of popular fruit and vegetables, plus positive and social norm messages

Placards were placed on the inside front and outside front of the grocery cart

No intervention

14 days (plus 57 days matched pre-intervention period)

Weekly sales data

There was an increase in average spending per day per person on produce of 16% (< 0.01) in the intervention store and an increase of 4% (p = n.s.) in the control store.

+

Payne 2016 [30]

USA

Pilot study

Supermarket

Fruit and vegetables

Large green arrows

Green arrows included graphics of fruits and vegetables, an emoticon to facilitate social approval and text such as “follow green arrow for health”

Arrows were placed around the store perimeter, on the floor in highly visible places

No intervention

14 days (plus 14 days pre-intervention period)

Weekly sales data

There was an increase on produce spending per day per person of 8% (p = 0.01). The authors do not state change in spending in the control store.

+

Hanks 2016 [26] USA

Randomised controlled trial

School cafeterias

Fruit and vegetables

Brightly coloured vinyl banners and/or TV segments

Branded media vegetable characters with human characteristics and super human strength.

Banners were placed around the salad bar and on top of the bar. TVs were placed on small tables near the salad bar Small printed vegetables were also placed on the floor to direct children to the salad bar

No Intervention

4 weeks (plus 2 weeks pre-intervention)

Food preperation records and count data of children serving themselves

There was an increase from 60 to 185 daily vegetable servings in the schools with the combined vinyl banner and TV segments intervention. There was a reported increase in servings for the individual intervention arms, but data were not significant. Count data showed a significant increase in frequency of children taking vegetables from the salad bar; 12.6 to 24% with vinyl banners, 10.2% to 34.6% with vinyl banners and TV segments. There was no significant change with just the TV segments (13.8% to 18.9%) or in the control schools.

+/−

Morizet 2012 [36] France

Experimental design (randomised by temporal order and condition)

School cafeterias

Fruit and vegetables

A white paper easel with text information

A basic label, “new carrot/broccoli recipe” and a model label: “new carrot/broccoli recipe, special mix for superheroes”

Labels were presented at the front of the gastronorm tray where vegetables were located.

No label

Two different days (one per vegetable option)

Choice frequency of vegetables

Children selected the familiar carrot or broccoli dish significantly more often when no label was present. For the carrot dish there was a significant difference in the number of children who selected the novel dish with the basic label (p = 0.012), and the model label (p = 0.002) as compared to no label condition. The patterns were similar for broccoli, but not statistically significant.

+/−

Folta 2006 [31] USA

Randomised controlled trial

School cafeterias

Fruit and vegetables

Audio announcement

Messages promoted beans and featured a magical superhero “bean man”

Audio messages were played during the school morning announcements

The same bean dishes were introduced to the school lunch menus. The children received their normal morning announcements, without the addition of any health messages

Approximately 3 months

Choice of beans

Overall data showed no difference in frequency of bean selection between intervention and control schools.

Cues providing educational information about the product properties

Steenhuis 2004 [32] Netherlands

Randomised pre-test, post-test experimental design

Worksite cafeterias

Low fat options

Signs (plus brochures, table tents and optional self-help manuals, newsletters and badges (the additional factors were considered as part of the educational programme)).

The signs consisted of the program logo, the name of the food item, and an indication that it was a healthy choice

Signs were placed in front of targeted food products

No intervention Additional arms: Educational programme, Food supply plus education.

One month (with the option to extend to six months)

Sales proportion of low-fat products per product group A self-administered food frequency questionnaire to measure fat, fruit and vegetable intake.

At one and six months there were no significant changes in reported fat, fruit or vegetable consumption. Sales data showed a significant increase in the proportion of low fat desserts sold in the intervention sites as compared to control sites. No other differences in sales were observed.

+/−

Vyth 2011 [33] Netherlands

Cluster randomised controlled trial

Worksite cafeterias

Low fat options plus fruit

Placards with a logo. Menus explaining the logo were available.

The logo consisted of a tick, indicating a healthy choice. The logo was part of “The choices intervention” which assigns its logo to foods which meet a determined sodium, added sugar, saturated fat, trans fats, fibre and energy.

The placards were placed next to freshly prepared “choices” sandwiches and soups, and the fruit basket.

No labelling or any other communication regarding the intervention

3 weeks (plus 3 weeks pre and 3 weeks post intervention periods)

Daily sales data

There was a significant difference in fruit sales between the intervention and control sites during the intervention period. No other differences in sales of targeted products were observed.

+/−

McClain 2013 [28]USA

Cluster randomised controlled trial with repeated cross sectional assessment

University dining halls

Low fat options

Placards, posters and table tents

Colourful photographs, and messages such as “brain food”

Placards were placed at food stations and on dining tables. Posters were placed around the dining halls.

No intervention

4 weeks (plus 7 days pre and post intervention)

Harvard food frequency questionnaire data

Students in the control halls consumed significantly more junk food and high fat meat products from baseline to intervention relative to students in the intervention halls. No other differences were observed.

+/−

Lee-Kwan 2015 [27] USA

Quasi-experimental study

Carryout restaurants

Healthy entrees, sides and beverages

Menu boards and posters Paper menus were also available, which were replicates of the menu boards

Digital colour photographs of selected healthier options plus a green leaf logo to indicate the healthier options

Menu boards were placed next to healthier items. Posters were placed next to the menu boards.

No intervention

8 weeks (plus 4 week pre-intervention, and a further 16 week intervention of additional interventions)

Weekly sales data

In the intervention group the relative odds of healthy entrees were greater during the intervention period to baseline (OR 1.16, 95%CI: 1.08: 1.26). The changes were not significantly different to control sites.The relative odds of healthy sides and beverages was not significantly different over time in the intervention group. were no significant differences in units sold from baseline in the intervention group or compared to control.

Implicit cues

Stockli 2016 [35] Switzerland

One-factorial within subjects experimental design study

Vending machines in University and Public Health office buildings

Both healthy and unhealthy snacks

Posters

Study 1: A nature poster, (showing grassland, tress and a blue sky with clouds) an activity poster (showing running legs in sports shoes and asphalt in the background) and a fun fair poster (showed two carousels with a summery blue sky in the background) Study 2: A poster with the Giacometi sculptures, (elongated figures) an activity poster and a fun fair poster (as above)

Posters were placed above vending machines

No posters

4 weeks

Daily sales data

There was a significant association between poster exposure and snack choice in both studies: Study 1: The percentage of healthy snacks selected was, 34% with the nature poster, 28% with the activity poster, 22% with the control condition and 18% with the funfair poster. Study 2: The percentage of healthy snacks selected was 58% with the Giacometti poster, 44% with the activity poster, 29% with the fun fair poster and 21% with the control condition

+

Engles 2011 [34] Netherlands

Experimental design study (evenings were randomised between arms)

Bars

Alcohol

Audio: Musical playlists

A playlist of 90 songs (with a 5.5 h duration) with contextual references to alcohol. Songs contained lyrics that referred to alcohol in them, for example “red, red, wine”.

The music was played on a fixed day of the week, at a fixed starting time and were played in a random order by the bartenders

A playlist of songs created by the same artists included in the intervention playlist, but the songs did not refer to alcohol. To ensure a good match, attention was played to timeframe and period of the album, tempo and energetic content of the songs.

The three included bars collected data on 18, (bar A) 12, (bar B) and 16 evenings (bar c).

Sales of alcoholic drinks corresponding to the two hours the music was played

Turnover was significantly higher on nights when music had reference to alcohol played compared to nights when the control playlist was played (p = < 0.05)

+

  1. + Reported significant increase in healthier items (including fruit and vegetables) or decrease in unhealthier items for primary data. +/− Mixed results for reported increase in healthier items (including fruit and vegetables) or decrease in unhealthier items for primary data. – No evidence of effect on increasing healthier items (including fruit and vegetables) or decreasing unhealthier items for primary data