Skip to main content

Table 4 Regressions with objective (ratings from the masked cigarette) and expected ratings predicting ratings of experienced enjoyment, quality and harshness when the brand name was known

From: Influence of premium vs masked cigarette brand names on the experienced taste of a cigarette after tobacco plain packaging in Australia: an experimental study

 

Perceived Enjoyment of the branded cigarette (n = 70)a

 

Unadjusted Model

Adjusted ModelA, B

Predictor variables

b

[95% CI]

β

b

[95% CI]

β

Objective Enjoyment (ratings of the masked cigarette)

0.20

[0.01, 0.39]

.24*

0.23

[0.05, 0.41]

.28*

Expected Enjoyment

0.32

[0.11, 0.52]

.34**

0.31

[0.11, 0.51]

.34**

Cigarette order

   

−9.29

[−17.59, − 1.00]

−.24*

Difference in number of puffs between conditions

   

7.58

[−.19, 15.36]

.20

 

Perceived Quality of the branded cigarette (n = 71)b

 

Unadjusted Model

Adjusted Model

Predictor variables

b

[95% CI]

β

b

[95% CI]

β

Objective Quality (ratings of the masked cigarette)

0.06

[−0.17, 0.28]

.06

0.08

[−0.13, 0.30]

.09

Expected Quality

0.40

[0.13, 0.66]

.35**

0.46

[0.21, 0.72]

.42**

Cigarette order

   

−7.64

[−16.40, 1.12]

−.19

Difference in number of puffs between conditions

   

10.82

[2.43, 19.20]

.28*

 

Perceived Harshness of the branded cigarette (n = 71)c

 

Unadjusted Model

Adjusted ModelC, D, E, F

Predictor variables

b

[95% CI]

β

b

[95% CI]

β

Objective Harshness (ratings of the masked cigarette)

0.08

[−0.18, 0.33]

.07

0.11

[−0.14, 0.36]

.10

Expected Harshness

0.18

[−0.05, 0.40]

.19

0.19

[−0.02, 0.41]

.21†

Cigarette order

   

12.68

[.80, 24.55]

.25*

Difference in number of puffs between conditions

   

−1.62

[− 12.60, 9.37]

−.03

  1. Note: There was no indication of multicollinearity in any model, with correlations between objective measures being low: r = .24 (p = .043) for enjoyment, r = .35 (p = .003) for quality, and r = .09 (p = .454) for harshness. VIF values from the adjusted regression models ranged from 1.02 to 1.18, further suggesting that multicollinearity was not a concern
  2. aUnadjusted model: R2 = .22, F(2,67) = 9.19, p < .001); Adj. model: R2 = .31, F(4,65) = 7.17, p < .001)
  3. bUnadjusted model: R2 = .14, F(2,68) = 5.70, p = .005); Adj. model: R2 = .24, F(4,66) = 5.32, p = .001)
  4. cUnadjusted model: R2 = .04, F(2,68) = 1.54, p = .223); Adj. model: R2 = .11, F(4,66) = 1.94, p = .115)
  5. ASensitivity 1: Objective enjoyment no longer predicted perceived enjoyment when the brand variant name was known (Adj. model: β = .18, t(52) = 1.38, p = .174)
  6. BSensitivity 4: Objective enjoyment no longer predicted perceived enjoyment when the brand variant name was known (Adj. model: β = .17, t(59) = 1.43, p = .157)
  7. CSensitivity 1: Expected harshness no longer tended to predict perceived harshness when the brand variant name was known (Adj. model: β = .19, t(53) = 1.41, p = .164)
  8. DSensitivity 2: Objective harshness tended to predict perceived harshness when the brand variant name was known (Adj. model: β = .22, t(56) = 1.73, p = .089). Expected harshness no longer tended to predict perceived harshness when the brand variant name was known (Adj. model: β = .14, t(56) = 1.11, p = .272)
  9. ESensitivity 3: Expected harshness no longer tended to predict perceived harshness when the brand variant name was known (Adj. model: β = .18, t(61) = 1.53, p = .132)
  10. FSensitivity 4: Expected harshness no longer tended to predict perceived harshness when the brand variant name was known (Adj. model: β = .19, t(60) = 1.56, p = .123)
  11. ** p < .01. * p < .05, † p < .10