Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Childhood adversity as a risk for cancer: findings from the 1958 British birth cohort study

Abstract

Background

To analyse whether Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) are associated with an increased risk of cancer.

Methods

The National child development study (NCDS) is a prospective birth cohort study with data collected over 50 years. The NCDS included all live births during one week in 1958 (n = 18558) in Great Britain. Self-reported cancer incidence was based on 444 participants reporting having had cancer at some point and 5694 reporting never having cancer. ACE was measured using reports of: 1) child in care, 2) physical neglect, 3) child’s or family’s contact with the prison service, 4) parental separation due to divorce, death or other, 5) family experience of mental illness & 6) family experience of substance abuse. The resulting variable had three categories, no ACEs/ one ACE/ 2 + ACEs and was used to test for a relationship with cancer. Information on socioeconomic characteristics, pregnancy and birth were extracted as potential confounders. Information on adult health behaviours, socioeconomic environment, psychological state and age at first pregnancy were added to the models. Multivariate models were run using multiply-imputed data to account for missing data in the cohort.

Results

The odds of having a cancer before 50 y among women increased twofold for those who had 2+ ACEs versus those with no ACEs, after adjusting for adult factors and early life confounders (OR: 2.1, 95% CI: 1.42-3.21, p < 0.001).

Conclusion

These findings suggest that cancer risk may be influenced by exposure to stressful conditions and events early on in life. This is potentially important in furthering our understanding of cancer aetiology, and consequently in redirecting scientific research and developing appropriate prevention policies.

Peer Review reports

Background

Strong evidence for socio-economic differences in cancer survival and mortality has been shown for many cancers and across populations [1]. In 2004, cancer was among the leading four causes of death in high income countries [2, 3], and its increase is typically associated with the third stage of epidemiological transition [4]. Cancer prevalence and mortality is likely to become increasingly predominant in high-income countries and by consequence so is their contribution to health inequalities.

The causes of cancer are complex and multifactorial occurring over a lengthy time. Explanations for mechanisms linking adverse events early in life and cancer have been provided from animal models [5, 6]. In humans, early life exposure to Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE), like trauma, abuse or maltreatment in childhood has been linked both ‘indirectly’, through tobacco and alcohol use or via ‘direct’ associations [7] to alterations of the brain structure and neurobiological stress-response systems which in turn have consequences for health and emotional well-being [8]. Studies have described associations between retrospectively collected ACE and health outcomes such as liver disease [9], ischaemic heart disease [10], obesity [11, 12], perceived health [13] and psychopathology [14] as well as premature mortality [15, 16]. Regarding cancer, data are sparser and inconsistent [7, 12, 17]. One of the reasons for this inconsistency is that the ACE measure used is usually self-reported by adults asked about trauma and adversity they may have experienced during childhood. Such questions are inevitably vulnerable to various forms of recall bias [18] one example being that individuals’ memories of the past may be influenced by their later health [19].

In the present study we aim to determine if exposure to ACE, determined using prospectively collected data, is linked with the onset of cancer in adulthood. The causes of cancers are indeed diverse, however our aim here is to understand the development of susceptibility to this disease at its earliest phase, at the common roots of all cancers [20]. To do this we take a lifecourse approach, considering the whole life span. We hypothesise that the physiological embedding of stress induced by adversity in childhood is linked with biological susceptibility as well as with the development of stress-reducing health behaviours favouring the development of cancers in adult life [21, 22]. The objective of this study is to examine the relationship between psychosocial adversity in childhood and cancer after controlling for the effects of material disadvantage, health behaviours and education level using a large prospective cohort study.

Methods

Data are from the 1958 National Child Development Study (NCDS) which included all live births during one week in 1958 (n = 18558) in Great Britain. Subsequent data collections (sweeps) were carried out on cohort members aged 7, 11, 16, 23, 33, 42, 46 and 50. At the last sweep, carried out in 2008, 9790 individuals participated in the self-reported questionnaire and face to face interview, representing 53% of the original sample. The NCDS has been described in detail elsewhere [23].

Ethics & data access

Written informed consent was obtained from the parents for childhood measurements and ethical approval for the adult data collection was obtained from the National Research Ethics Advisory Panel. NCDS data are open access datasets available to non-profit research organisations.

Cancer in the cohort

The outcome variable of interest in this study was self-reported cancer between the age of 33 and 50. We did not use information available on cancer at the age of 23, as we were interested in controlling for health behaviours at this age. Two main types of variables were used across the data sweeps. At ages 46 and 50 respondents were asked to report any medical conditions among which cancers can be identified. At ages 33 and 42 respondents were asked to report if they had ‘ever had cancer’. The outcome variable ‘cancer’ constructed for the purpose of this study was categorised as follows: ‘yes’ corresponds to individuals who ever reported having had cancer at ages 33, 42, 46 or 50 (n = 444); ‘no’ corresponds to individuals who explicitly reported not having a cancer at age 33 and 42, and who did not have an ICD coded cancer among their reported medical conditions at the ages of 46 and 50 (n = 5694); ‘missing’ corresponds to individuals who did not respond to any questions on cancer, and respondents who occasionally responded ‘no’ or who were missing. These are individuals who we could not exclude as having had a cancer based on their self-report (n = 11943). Based on this conservative definition respondents had to actively respond ‘yes’ to having had a cancer or report a cancer as a medical condition. Conversely they had to consistently respond ‘no’ to questions on cancer and not report cancer as a medical condition. All other cases were defined as ‘missing’. Further details on how cancer was coded in the cohort are available from the authors.

Adverse childhood experiences (ACE)

There are many ways in which adversity can be conceptualised [2427]. We have attempted to construct a theoretical framework prior to extracting any data, in order to create a measurement with a robust content validity. We have identified ACE as a set of traumatic and stressful psychosocial conditions that are out of the child’s control, that tend to co-occur and often persist over time [12, 14, 24, 27]. We have restricted ACE to intra-familial events or conditions in the child’s immediate environment causing chronic stress responses. In our definition ACE is distinguished from events or conditions linked to the socioeconomic and material environment.

Information was extracted from the study via variables collected at age 7, 11, 16 from questions posed to the child’s parent or their teacher. Sources of adversity were divided into six categories:

  1. 1.

    Child in care: child has ever been in public/ voluntary care services or foster care at age 7, 11 or 16.

  2. 2.

    Physical neglect: child appears undernourished/ dirty aged 7 or 11, information collected from the response from child’s teacher to the Bristol Social Adjustment Guide.

Household dysfunction, as described by Felitti et al. [12], is a dimension of adversity consisting of four categories each contributing to the score:

  1. 3.

    Offenders: The child lived in a household where a family member was in prison or on probation (age 11 y) or is in contact with probation service at 7 or 11 y; the child has ever been to prison or been on probation at 16 y.

  2. 4.

    Parental separation: The child has been separated from their father or mother due to death, divorce, or separation at 7, 11 or 16 y.

  3. 5.

    Mental illness: Household has contact with mental health services at 7 or 11 y; Family member has mental illness at 7 & 11 or 16 y.

  4. 6.

    Alcohol abuse: Family member has alcohol abuse problem at 7 y.

Exposure to adversity was identified by a positive response to any of the above categories. Respondents were excluded if they had missing data for all six categories. Respondents were considered as having no adversities if they answered ‘no’ all the categories or if they answered ‘no’ to one or more category and the other categories were missing. ACE was measured by counting the reports of: child in care, physical neglect, offenders, parental separation, mental illness and alcohol abuse. A three category variable was then constructed (0 adversities/1 adversity/2or + adversities).

Early life socioeconomic and biological confounders

To examine the relationship between ACE and cancer, prior confounding variables potentially associated with both ACE and cancer need to be included in the initial multivariate model. Among the variables available at baseline, collected from the cohort member’s mothers via a questionnaire at birth, we identified those most likely to be social or biological confounding factors based on the literature. Household and parental characteristics were included: mother’s age at birth, overcrowding (people per room), mother’s partner’s social class (recoded into manual vs non-manual), and if this was unavailable the mother’s father’s social class was used mother’s education level (left school before/after minimum leaving age), and maternal smoking during pregnancy (no smoking, sometimes, often, heavy). The respondent’s characteristics and birth variables were also included: sex, gestational age at birth, parity, birth weight, foetal distress, problems during pregnancy and breastfeeding (no, one month or less, more than one month). To control for health problems in childhood, a childhood pathologies variable was constructed using data collected at ages 7, 11 and 16 y. It was based both on mother’s report and medical examinations including congenital conditions, moderate/severe disabilities, chronic respiratory or circulatory conditions, sensory impairments and special schooling (childhood pathology: yes/no).

Mediators across the lifecourse

To determine whether any observed associations between ACE and cancer were due to adult mediating factors, the following were added to the models: respondent’s educational attainment at 23y (A level/ O level/ no qualification), respondent’s occupational social class at 23 y (non-manual active/ manual active/ inactive). The ‘malaise inventory’ was used to identify symptoms of depression at the age of 23. It was based on a set of 24 questions indentifying symptoms, if the respondent reported experiencing more than seven of the symptoms they were considered as having psychological malaise (no malaise/ malaise), characterized by symptoms of depression and/or anxiety. The health behaviour variables included were: alcohol consumption at 23 y (normal drinking (women: between 1–14 units in the previous week, men: between 1–21 units in the previous week)/ abstinence (reported not consuming any alcohol in the previous week)/ heavy drinking (women: >14 units in the previous week, men: >21 units in the previous week [28]), smoking status at 23 y (never smoked/ past smoker/ current smoker), and BMI (kg/m2) categorised using the WHO cut-offs age 23 y [29]. Adult life-style variables are available at other points along the lifecourse, however in our models, these adult variables at the age of 23 are a proxy for behavioural patterns in early adulthood predating reports of cancer. Controlling for them serves as a first step to understanding possible mechanisms. In the women’s model we also created a variable identifying the age at which cohort members had their first pregnancy, a known risk factor for breast cancer. This was determined based on variables at age 33 y, and 50 y. The resulting variable is in three categories (before 33 y/ after 33 y/ no information on pregnancy).

Missing data, imputation and statistical analysis

Cohort follow-up has been good over time, 84% participated at the age of 16, with a gradual dwindling in participation throughout adulthood (72% at age 23, and 65% at age 42) when participants were most likely to have moved [23]. Refusal rates have been low, with 7% at age 23 and 13% at age 42 for example [23]. In the latest data sweep (2008) 54% (n = 9790) of the original sample participated, and for some variables missing data presents a considerable challenge. The sample used for this study is described in Figure 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Flowchart showing selection of the subsample used for this study.

To control for possible bias due to missing data, we imputed data for covariates with missing data using the multiple imputation program ICE in STATA v11. Twenty imputations were conducted taking the missing at random (MAR) assumption for the covariates only (mother’s education, father’s social class, overcrowding, birthweight, gestational age, parity, smoking during pregnancy, mother’s age at birth, breastfeeding, child pathologies, educational attainment at 23, social class at 23, malaise inventory at 23, drinking at age 23 and smoking at age 23, age at 1st pregnancy for women). Neither the exposure variable of interest (ACE) nor the cancer variable were included in the multivariate imputation model.

Bivariate crosstabulations were carried out on the imputed data using logistic regression to obtain p-values adjusted for the artificially inflated sample size. As the timing of cancer events was unknown, multivariate logistic regression analyses were carried out on the data obtained from multiply imputed data. Three models were run separately by sex, entering the variables chronologically as they would occur over the lifecourse. First early life socioeconomic circumstances and perinatal variables were entered. In model 2, childhood pathologies and ACE were added to the model. Finally, model 3 additionally controlled for education, social class, psychological malaise, health behaviours at 23 y, and for women, age at first pregnancy.

Results

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1 for the subsample (n = 6138) who provided information on whether or not they had had a cancer in the last four data sweeps (ages 33, 43, 46 and 50). Among respondents who answered questions on cancer, 3.3% of men and 10.6% of women reported having had cancer by the age of 50. In total 444 self-reported cancers were identified in the cohort reported between the ages of 33 and 50, 80% being reported by women (n = 351). Based on the available information on cancer types reported by the cohort members (see Additional file 1), breast and cervical cancers were most often reported (19% and 15% respectively), followed by skin cancers (11%). Nevertheless, most cancers remained ‘undefined ‘(39%) The majority of men (74%) and women (75%) had had no adverse childhood experiences, based on the prospectively collected information at ages 7, 11 and 16 y. The distribution of ACEs was similar for both sexes, with 5.6% of men and 5.4% of women having two or more ACEs.

Table 1 Characteristics of the subsample* at birth, during childhood and in adulthood

Table 2 shows bivariate analyses between ACE, the covariates and cancer for men and women. In men, no relationship is apparent between ACE and cancer. The multivariate models for men (Table 3) show no association between early life socioeconomic variables or perinatal variables and cancer. ACE was not significantly associated with cancer, and neither were social variables at age 23 y, or smoking and alcohol age 23 y.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics on ACE, and the lifecourse covariates by reported cancer for men and women
Table 3 Multivariate logistic regression models using data obtained from multiple imputation: men (n = 2836)

In women (Table 2), a bivariate relationship between ACE and cancer was shown. The proportion of women reporting a cancer before 50 increased from 9.1% for those with no ACEs to 13.0% for those with one ACE (p = 0.004), and 23.0% for those with 2 + ACEs (p < 0.001). Both manual parental social class and overcrowding in the household at birth were significantly related to reporting a cancer before 50 y, and mother’s education was of borderline significance. Women who had experienced childhood pathologies were also more likely to have had a cancer than those without childhood pathologies (13.3% vs 9.9%, p = 0.016). Many of the female respondent’s adult variables were also related to having had a cancer before 50 y in the bivariate analyses. Low educational attainment, being inactive in terms of occupation, being a heavy alcohol drinker, being a smoker and having symptoms of depression/ anxiety, and having a first pregnancy after the age of 33 y, were also related to having had a cancer before 50.

The multivariate models for women (Table 4) show that. ACE was significantly associated with reporting cancer before the age of 50 after control for potential confounding variables (model 2). The graded association showed that women with one ACE had a 40% increase in the odds of reporting cancer (p = 0.016), and those with two or more ACEs had a 2.5 increase in the odds of reporting a cancer versus women with no ACEs (p < 0.001). When information on adult behaviours, social characteristics and psychological malaise was added to the model this association weakened, so that women with two or more ACEs had a 2.1 increase in the odds of reporting a cancer versus those with no ACEs (p < 0.001). Women who had their first pregnancy after the age of 33 y had a significantly increased risk of having cancer before 50 y (p < 0.001). Furthermore, women who were smokers at age 23 y had a 70% increase in the odds of having cancer before 50 versus non-smokers (p = 0.001), and women who were heavy drinkers at age 23 y also had an increased risk of having cancer before 50, however this result was of borderline statistical significance (p = 0.055). Having symptoms of depression/anxiety increased the odds of having a cancer before 50 by 35%, however this result was not significant at the 5% level (p = 0.1).

Table 4 Multivariate logistic regression models using data obtained from multiple imputation: women (n = 3302)

Discussion

The main finding from this study was that psychosocial adversity in childhood was related to cancer incidence before 50 y among women, after adjusting for prior confounding factors and potential mediators, in a large prospective cohort. An accumulation of ACE remained a strong predictor of cancer in women, after taking important potential mediating factors at age 23 y into account, including smoking and drinking. Women who experienced two or more ACE doubled their risk of having a cancer before 50 relative to women who had had no childhood adversities. There was a tendency towards a graded association between childhood adversity and adult cancer across the ACE categories. These results make a significant contribution to demonstrating and understanding links between ACE and cancer because they use an a priori definition of adversity and identify ACE with prospectively collected data. The strength of the relationship between adversity and cancer was of the same magnitude as that observed between age at first pregnancy and cancer, a well-known risk factor for breast cancer [30].

The two main weaknesses of this study are in the self-reported nature of cancer incidence, and the amount of missing data caused by attrition in the cohort study. These weaknesses are partly addressed by the conservative nature of our cancer variable, and by using estimates obtained from multiple imputations to account for the missing data. We conducted sensitivity analyses by running the model using imputed datasets, using a case–control sub-sample, and using the full cohort dataset without imputation, and found that the relationship between ACE and cancer incidence was stable. Comparisons were made between complete-case analyses and those run on estimates obtained by imputation. The models yielded similar results until the inclusion of variables at age 23 (model 3). The differences observed in the results for model 3 indicate selection bias in the complete case sample, where individuals who had experienced ACEs in childhood were more likely to have missing data at age 23 regarding health behaviours. The multiple imputation model therefore enables adjustment for this bias. We are confident, therefore, that our analyses show a ‘real’ association, the nature of which needs to be established in further more complex modelling of mediating factors.

The nature of the questions collecting information on cancer varied by data sweep, some being retrospective (have you ever had cancer) while other questions asked about suffering from an illness at the time of the survey. This means that there are information gaps, notably between the age of 33 and 42 where the cohort member could have had cancer and not report that they were suffering from it subsequently. However, this means that our cancer variable is most likely conservative. The validity of self-reported cancer is likely to vary based on the cancer type diagnosed, and tends towards an underreporting of cancers, given the high levels of specificity reported in studies comparing self-reports to registry data [31]. Indeed in studies where cancer registry data have been compared to self-reported information individuals tend to underreport rather than overreport cancer history, and variation in inaccurate cancer reporting varied considerably by cancer type [32]. It is also important to consider the biases linked with overreporting cancers. Individuals whose self-reported cancer was unconfirmed by the Finnish cancer registry were more likely to have accumulated psychosocial strain across their lifecourse [33]. The probability of misreporting cancer, both over-reporting and under-reporting, has been associated with socio-demographic characteristics: gender, age, BMI, size of household, place of birth, smoking, social participation, educational level, type of employment, alcohol consumption and poor well-being [34]. The sensitivity of self-reported cancer is also likely to be higher among respondents with a high level of education [31]. Stavrou et al. have demonstrated a good level of sensitivity and specificity for self-reported cancer diagnoses in a cohort of older women in Australia, comparing self-reports to registry data [35]. Gupta et al. also describe good concordance between self-reported cancer and medical chart information [36]. The multivariate models also show associations between well known risk factors and cancer before 50, such as age at first pregnancy, smoking and drinking, which contributes to enhancing the validity of the cancer variable.

Women were four times more likely to report having had a cancer than men which is understandable given the age of cohort participants at the time of study. Based on 2007–2009 estimates of cancer incidence in the UK, 44% of cancers diagnosed among women aged 25–49 were breast cancers [37]. Rather than indicating a sex, or gender difference, the lack of association between virtually any of the variables and cancer among men is likely be mainly due to the far lower incidence in men up to age 50 (93 cancers). The distribution of cancer types observed in the cohort will continue to evolve over time, and begin to represent a greater proportion of men due to the increased occurrence of prostate and lung cancer among men >50 years. Linkage will hopefully be made between the cohort and the cancer registries which would significantly increased the validity and reliability of the information, and allow analyses by cancer type in the future, and survival analyses, which are currently not possible due to a lack of information on the timing of events. For these analyses we limited the health behavioural variables to those collected at age 23 rather than using variables available afterwards. This enabled to limit any further problems of missing data, but may lead to an underestimate of the contribution of health behaviours in the associations.

One important strength of this study is in the prospective nature of the information on ACEs. Most studies examining the links between early adversity rely on retrospective questions, which are prone to recall bias, an important issue raised by Korpimaki et al. using data from a working age population and the cancer registry [19]. Using a retrospective questionnaire to identify ACE, and limiting their analyses to individuals whose cancer was diagnosed subsequent to answering questions on ACE to address the problem of recall bias, these authors found no association between working-age cancer and reporting ACE.

There is currently insufficient power to work on cancers at different sites using the cohort. Furthermore, we take a lifecourse approach to this study and are interested in the pre-cancerous phase where early psychosocial adversity may increase susceptibility to developing cancer earlier. The cause of cancers and their prognoses are indeed diverse, however our aim here is to understand the factors contributing to susceptibility at its earliest phase. The ACE literature indicates that there are early life factors which may increase inflammatory response [38], decrease immune system efficiency [39], increase exposure to viral infections [40], and raise the probability of damaging health behaviours along the lifecourse [41, 42]. All of these behavioural and biological processes are known to be involved in cancer development.

The association between stress and cancer development and progression has been shown in biological studies [5]. Stress-related immunological changes bring about declines in natural killer cell activity by depressing their ability to respond to tumour or virally infected cells, and causing a reduction in the body’s defences linked to the repair of damaged DNA [43]. Exposure to stressors is known to trigger responses via the central nervous system produced by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA). This activity modifies neuroendocrine pathways which, over the long term, alter the critical physiological mechanisms involved in tumourogenesis [5]. When exposed to chronic stress, “the body remains in a constant state of overdrive” with adverse consequences on the regulation of systems implicated in cancer progression [6]. ACE has also been associated with risky health behaviours such as smoking, alcoholism, early sexual activity, and having multiple sexual partners [8, 13, 3841], all of which are ‘indirect’ risk factors for cancer.

Our hypothesis that adversity in childhood may be linked with biological susceptibility involved in the development of cancers in adult life is reinforced by our findings. It could operate via two main mechanisms: A direct biological effect and an indirect effect via health behaviours (or a combination of both). The fact that ACE is still associated with cancer even after adjusting for behavioural and social mediators favours argument for a direct “biological” role of ACE.

In epidemiological studies, evidence of a direct association between exposure to stress and cancer incidence is mixed and inconclusive. This is likely to be due to the different ways in which stress was conceptualised. A Danish cohort study on 8736 men and women found no direct association between cumulative stressful life events collected retrospectively, mostly during adulthood, and cancer incidence, though they did identify a relationship between stress and unhealthy lifestyles [44]. Ollonen (2005) et al. found support for an overall association between stressful life events across the lifecourse and breast cancer risk in their Finnish case–control study [45]. A meta-analysis of studies on the association between stress and breast cancer did not support an association between stressful life events in adulthood and breast cancer risk [46]. Using linkage to the cancer registry, Fang et al. found that bereaved parents were at increased risk for cancers with an infectious aetiology, especially those linked to infection by the Human Papilloma Virus (HPV) after controlling for confounders. The authors hypothesised that the stress induced by losing a child may accelerate the cancer genesis of an established infection [47]. These studies do not consider the timing of exposure to stressful events in their analyses, an important factor considering the differential effects of physiological stress responses on various areas of the brain depending on when exposure occurs along the lifecourse [48].

Conclusion

Our findings establish an association between ACE and cancer in women using prospective data. The suggestion that cancer risk may be influenced by conditions in the first years of life is potentially important in furthering our understanding of factors contributing to cancer development, and consequently in redirecting scientific research and developing appropriate prevention policies. This nevertheless remains a first step in understanding the relationship between ACE and cancer in the 1958 birth cohort. A second step will require an in-depth exploration of both direct and indirect pathways along which biological, social and psychosocial mechanisms are likely to operate.

Abbreviations

ACE:

Adverse childhood experiences

NCDS:

National child development study

HPA:

Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis.

References

  1. Woods LM, Rachet B, Coleman MP: Origins of socio-economic inequalities in cancer survival: a review. Ann Oncol. 2006, 17 (1): 5-19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Lopez AD, Mathers CD, Ezzati M, Jamison DT, Murray CJL: Global and regional burden of disease and risk factors, 2001: systematic analysis of population health data. Lancet. 2006, 367 (9524): 1747-1757. 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68770-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. WHO: The global burden of disease: 2004 update. 2008, Geneva: World Health Organisation, 160-

    Google Scholar 

  4. Omran AR: The epidemiologic transition: A theory of the epidemiology of population change (Reprinted from The Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, vol 49, pg 509–38, 1971). Milbank Q. 2005, 83 (4): 731-757. 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2005.00398.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Antoni MH, Lutgendorf SK, Cole SW, Dhabhar FS, Sephton SE, McDonald PG, Stefanek M, Sood AK: Opinion - The influence of bio-behavioural factors on tumour biology: pathways and mechanisms. Nat Rev Cancer. 2006, 6 (3): 240-248. 10.1038/nrc1820.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Lutgendorf SK, Sood AK, Antoni MH: Host factors and cancer progression: biobehavioral signaling pathways and interventions. J Clin Oncol. 2010, 28 (26): 4094-4099. 10.1200/JCO.2009.26.9357.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Brown DW, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Edwards VJ, Malarcher AM, Croft JB, Giles WH: Adverse childhood experiences are associated with the risk of lung cancer: a prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2010, 10:

    Google Scholar 

  8. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, Dube SR, Giles WH: The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006, 256 (3): 174-186. 10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Dong M, Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Giles WH, Anda RF: Adverse childhood experiences and self-reported liver disease: new insights into the causal pathway. Arch Intern Med. 2003, 163 (16): 1949-1956. 10.1001/archinte.163.16.1949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Dong M, Giles WH, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williams JE, Chapman DP, Anda RF: Insights into causal pathways for ischemic heart disease: adverse childhood experiences study. Circulation. 2004, 110 (13): 1761-1766. 10.1161/01.CIR.0000143074.54995.7F.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Thomas C, Hypponen E, Power C: Obesity and type 2 diabetes risk in midadult life: The role of childhood adversity. Pediatrics. 2008, 121 (5): E1240-E1249. 10.1542/peds.2007-2403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS: Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. Am J Prev Med. 1998, 14 (4): 245-258. 10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Dube SR, Cook ML, Edwards VJ: Health-related outcomes of adverse childhood experiences in Texas, 2002. Prev Chronic Dis. 2010, 7 (3): A52-

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Clark C, Caldwell T, Power C, Stansfeld SA: Does the influence of childhood adversity on psychopathology persist across the lifecourse? a 45-year prospective epidemiologic study. Ann Epidemiol. 2010, 20 (5): 385-394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Brown DW, Anda RF, Tiemeier H, Felitti VJ, Edwards VJ, Croft JB, Giles WH: Adverse childhood experiences and the risk of premature mortality. Am J Prev Med. 2009, 37 (5): 389-396. 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.06.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kelly-Irving M, Lepage B, Dedieu D, Bartley M, Blane D, Grosclaude P, Lang T, Delpierre C: Adverse childhood experiences and premature all-cause mortality. Eur J Epidemiol. 2013, 1-14. 10.1007/s10654-013-9832-9.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Fuller-Thomson E, Brennenstuhl S: Making a link between childhood physical abuse and cancer results from a regional representative survey. Cancer. 2009, 115 (14): 3341-3350. 10.1002/cncr.24372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Hardt J, Rutter M: Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004, 45 (2): 260-273. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00218.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Korpimaki SK, Sumanen MPT, Sillanmaki LH, Mattila KJ: Cancer in working-age is not associated with childhood adversities. Acta Oncol. 2010, 49 (4): 436-440. 10.3109/02841860903521103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Balkwill F, Mantovani A: Inflammation and cancer: back to Virchow?. Lancet. 2001, 357 (9255): 539-545. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04046-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Delpierre C, Kelly-Irving M: To what extent are biological pathways useful when aiming to reduce social inequalities in cancer?. Eur J Public Health. 2011, 21 (4): 398-399. 10.1093/eurpub/ckr076.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kelly-Irving M, Mabile L, Grosclaude P, Lang T, Delpierre C: The embodiment of adverse childhood experiences and cancer development: potential biological mechanisms and pathways across the life course. Int J Public Health. 2013, 58 (1): 3-11. 10.1007/s00038-012-0370-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Power C, Elliott J: Cohort profile: 1958 british birth cohort (national child development study). Int J Epidemiol. 2006, 35 (1): 34-41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Anda RF, Butchart A, Felitti VJ, Brown DW: Building a framework for global surveillance of the public health implications of adverse childhood experiences. Am J Prev Med. 2010, 39 (1): 93-98. 10.1016/j.amepre.2010.03.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Benjet C, Borges G, Medina-Mora ME, Zambrano J, Cruz C, Mendez E: Descriptive epidemiology of chronic childhood adversity in Mexican adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2009, 45 (5): 483-489. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Dong M, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Loo CM, Giles WH: The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse Negl. 2004, 28 (7): 771-784. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Rosenman S, Rodgers B: Childhood adversity in an Australian population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2004, 39 (9): 695-702. 10.1007/s00127-004-0802-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. House of Commons Science and Technology Committee: Alcohol guidelines. 2012, London: The stationary office, 130-

    Google Scholar 

  29. Gallo V, Mackenbach JP, Ezzati M, Menvielle G, Kunst AE, Rohrmann S, Kaaks R, Teucher B, Boeing H, Bergmann MM, et al: Social inequalities and mortality in Europe – results from a large multi-national cohort. PLoS ONE. 2012, 7 (7): e39013-10.1371/journal.pone.0039013.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Kobayashi S, Sugiura H, Ando Y, Shiraki N, Yanagi T, Yamashita H, Toyama T: Reproductive history and breast cancer risk. Breast Cancer. 2012, 19 (4): 302-308. 10.1007/s12282-012-0384-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Navarro C, Chirlaque MD, Tormo MJ, Perez-Flores D, Rodriguez-Barranco M, Sanchez-Villegas A, Agudo A, Pera G, Amiano P, Dorronsoro M, et al: Validity of self reported diagnoses of cancer in a major Spanish prospective cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006, 60 (7): 593-599. 10.1136/jech.2005.039131.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Desai MM, Bruce ML, Desai RA, Druss BG: Validity of self-reported cancer history: a comparison of health interview data and cancer registry records. Am J Epidemiol. 2001, 153 (3): 299-306. 10.1093/aje/153.3.299.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Korpimaki S, Sumanen M, Suominen S, Mattila K: Self-reported rather than registered cancer is associated with psychosocial strain. BMC Fam Pract. 2012, 13:

    Google Scholar 

  34. Manjer J, Merlo J, Berglund GR: Validity of self-reported information on cancer: determinants of under- and over-reporting. Eur J Epidemiol. 2004, 19 (3): 239-247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Stavrou E, Vajdic CM, Loxton D, Pearson SA: The validity of self-reported cancer diagnoses and factors associated with accurate reporting in a cohort of older Australian women. Cancer Epidemiol. 2011, 35 (6): E75-E80. 10.1016/j.canep.2011.02.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Gupta V, Gu K, Chen Z, Lu W, Shu XO, Zheng Y: Concordance of self-reported and medical chart information on cancer diagnosis and treatment. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2011, 11:

    Google Scholar 

  37. The 5 Most Commonly Diagnosed Cancers in Females, Average Percentages and Numbers of New Cases, by Age, UK. 2007, http://www.cancerresearchuk.org/cancer-info/cancerstats/incidence/age/#Cancer, –2009,

  38. Tietjen GE, Khubchandani J, Herial NA, Shah K: Adverse childhood experiences are associated with migraine and vascular biomarkers. Headache: The Journal of Head and Face Pain. 2012, 52 (6): 920-929. 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2012.02165.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Dube SR, Fairweather D, Pearson WS, Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Croft JB: Cumulative childhood stress and autoimmune diseases in adults. Psychosom Med. 2009, 71 (2): 243-250. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181907888.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Shirtcliff EA, Coe CL, Pollak SD: Early childhood stress is associated with elevated antibody levels to herpes simplex virus type 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009, 106 (8): 2963-2967. 10.1073/pnas.0806660106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Anda RF, Croft JB, Felitti VJ, Nordenberg D, Giles WH, Williamson DF, Giovino GA: Adverse childhood experiences and smoking during adolescence and adulthood. JAMA. 1999, 282 (17): 1652-1658. 10.1001/jama.282.17.1652.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Anda RF, Whitfield CL, Felitti VJ, Chapman D, Edwards VJ, Dube SR, Williamson DF: Adverse childhood experiences, alcoholic parents, and later risk of alcoholism and depression. Psychiatr Serv. 2002, 53 (8): 1001-1009. 10.1176/appi.ps.53.8.1001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Glaser R: Psychoneuroimmunology and cancer: fact or fiction?. Eur J Cancer. 1999, 35 (11): 1603-1607. 10.1016/S0959-8049(99)00197-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Bergelt C, Prescott E, Gronbaek M, Koch U, Johansen C: Stressful life events and cancer risk. Br J Cancer. 2006, 95 (11): 1579-1581. 10.1038/sj.bjc.6603471.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Ollonen P, Lehtonen J, Eskelinen M: Stressful and adverse life experiences in patients with breast symptoms; a prospective case–control study in Kuopio. Finland. Anticancer Res. 2005, 25 (1B): 531-536.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Duijts SFA, Zeegers MPA, Van der Borne B: The association between stressful life events and breast cancer risk: a meta-analysis. Int J Cancer. 2003, 107 (6): 1023-1029. 10.1002/ijc.11504.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Fang F, Fall K, Sparen P, Adami HO, Valdimarsdottir HB, Lambe M, Valdimarsdottir U: Risk of infection-related cancers after the loss of a child: a follow-up study in Sweden. Cancer Res. 2011, 71 (1): 116-122. 10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-10-0470.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C: Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009, 10 (6): 434-445. 10.1038/nrn2639.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thank you to the Centre for Longitudinal Studies (CLS), Institute of Education for the use of the NCDS data and to the Economic and Social Data Service (ESDS) for making them available. However, neither CLS nor ESDS bear any responsibility for the analysis or interpretation of these data.

Funding

This work was initially supported by the French Institut national du Cancer (INCa). Project title: Inégalités sociales et cancer: approche par chaînes de causalité. Michelle Kelly-Irving was funded to carry out this work by the charitable organisation “Ligue nationale contre le cancer”.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michelle Kelly-Irving.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MKI was involved in the conception and design of the study, analysing and interpreting the data, drafted the manuscript and made modifications. PG, CD, and TL were involved in the conception and design of the study, analysing and interpreting the data, and revising the manuscript. BL and DD analysed and interpreted the analyses, and revised the manuscript. RL, NC, MB, and DB interpreted the analyses and revised the manuscript. The first author had full access to all of the data in the study and takes full responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. All authors approved the current version.

Electronic supplementary material

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kelly-Irving, M., Lepage, B., Dedieu, D. et al. Childhood adversity as a risk for cancer: findings from the 1958 British birth cohort study. BMC Public Health 13, 767 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-767

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-767

Keywords