Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Correlates of susceptibility to smoking among Mexican origin youth residing in Houston, Texas: A cross-sectional analysis

Abstract

Background

Survey data suggest that in Texas Latino youth exhibit higher rates of susceptibility to smoking than youth from other ethnic groups. In this analysis we examined the relationship between susceptibility to smoking and well-known risk factors associated with smoking initiation among a cohort of 11 to 13 year old Mexican origin youth residing in Houston, Texas.

Methods

We analyzed cross-sectional survey data from 1,187 participants who reported they had never smoked, even a puff of a cigarette. The survey assessed peer and family social influence, school and neighborhood characteristics, level of family acculturation and socioeconomic status, and attitudes toward smoking. Bivariate associations, Student's t-tests, and logistic regression analysis were used to examine predictors of susceptibility.

Results

Overall, 22.1% of the never-smokers were susceptible to smoking. Boys were more likely to be susceptible than girls (25.6% vs. 18.9%), and susceptible children were slightly older than non-susceptible children (12.1 vs. 11.8 years). In addition, multivariate analyses revealed that positive expectations about smoking exerted the strongest influence on susceptibility status (odds ratio = 4.85). Multivariate analyses further revealed that compared to non-susceptible participants, susceptibles were more likely to report peer influences supportive of smoking, lower subjective social status and more detentions at school, more temptations to try smoking and to have a mother and a brother who smokes.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that interventions that target positive expectations about smoking may be useful in this population. Furthermore, because youth encounter smoking-initiation risk factors in different social environments, our results underscore the continued need for both family- and school-based primary prevention programs to adequately combat their influence. The results also can be used to inform the development of culturally sensitive programs for Mexican origin youth.

Peer Review reports

Background

The construct of cognitive susceptibility to smoking, defined as lacking a firm commitment not to smoke in the future or if offered a cigarette by a friend, integrates behavioral intentions and expectations for future behavior [1]. Over the past decade, the construct has consistently demonstrated strong predictive abilities and has become widely accepted. In prospective studies, conducted in various geographic areas of the US among adolescents from differing ethnic backgrounds, adolescents identified as susceptible to smoking were two to three times more likely to have experimented with cigarettes than their non-susceptible peers at follow-up [2–5]. Moreover, results from a community-based tobacco prevention study indicated that susceptibility to smoking is amenable to interventions [6]. In this Texas-based study, an intervention reduced susceptibility to smoking among teens by 24.6%. Collectively, these studies suggest that identifying and characterizing susceptible adolescents who have never smoked will be critical to optimizing primary smoking prevention efforts among youth.

Mexican origin youth, aged 12 to 17 years, report higher rates of susceptibility to smoking (28.8%) compared to their non-Hispanic white (20.8%) and non-Hispanic black (23.0%) counterparts [7]. Moreover, results from two large population-based surveys of middle and high school students lend support to the predictive validity of the susceptibility construct [8, 9]. In Texas, Latino youth exhibit the highest rates of experimentation with cigarettes [8] and of current smoking [9], among all ethnic groups. In 2001 in Houston, Latino middle and high school students had the highest rate of experimenting with cigarettes (68.2%; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 64.2–72.2%) followed by whites (63.8%; 95% CI: 52.3–75.3%) and African Americans (53.4%; 95% CI: 48.4–58.4%). In addition, statewide in 2001, 14.5% (95% CI: 12.3–16.7%) of all Latino middle school students reported currently smoking compared to only 8.2% (95% CI: 6.3–10.2%) of white and 6.5% (95% CI: 4.5–8.4%) of African Americans middle school students. By high school, smoking rates among Latinos and whites were similar and significantly higher than smoking rates among African American and Asian students. Given that individuals of Mexican descent represent the most rapidly growing segment of the United States (US) population and account for almost 60% of the Hispanic population in the US [10], a close examination of risk factors associated with susceptibility to smoking among Mexican origin youth is timely and warranted.

Our goal in this analysis was to examine the associations between susceptibility to smoking and well-established sociodemographic, social, contextual, and behavioral risk factors associated with smoking initiation among a cohort of 11 to 13 year old Mexican origin youth in Houston, Texas. A better understanding of the relationship between susceptibility and these risk factors may facilitate the development of more effective primary smoking prevention programs targeted to this rapidly growing, understudied, and underserved population.

Methods

Participant recruitment

Participants included in this study were between 11 and 13 years of age and were drawn from a population-based infrastructure of Mexican American households created by the Department of Epidemiology at The University of Texas M. D. Anderson Cancer Center beginning in July 2001. Participants are self-identified Mexican Americans or Mexicans of any age and sex who reside in predominantly (at least 80% according to 2000 census) Mexican American neighborhoods in Houston. Families were recruited into the cohort through random-digit telephone dialing, block-walking, intercept (such as at health fairs), or networking through already enrolled participants. A detailed description of the recruitment methodology has been published [11].

Households with potential age-eligible participants were identified from the cohort database. The potential participants' parents or legal guardians were called to assess interest in the study. In households in which there were two or more age-eligible children, the child who had had his or her birthday most recently was invited to participate in the study. Home interviews were scheduled with all parents/legal guardians who agreed to participate. A pair of bilingual interviewers visited the home, explained the goals and scope of the study, obtained written informed consent from the child's parent/legal guardian and informed assent from the child, and enrolled the child. A total of 3,000 households were identified as eligible from the cohort database. Of these 1,425 were contacted. Just over 90% of all parents with age-eligible children who were contacted agreed to enroll their child in the study. The institutional review board at The University of Texas M. D. Anderson Cancer Center approved all aspects of this study.

Data collection and variables available for analysis from participants

After consenting to join the study, each participant completed a 5-minute personal interview during which basic demographic (gender, age, nativity status (U.S. or Mexico)) and acculturation data were collected. Acculturation was assessed using 4 items from Marin et al.'s [12] Language Use subscale. The questions ascertain language used when reading, speaking at home, speaking with friends, and thinking. The scale has excellent internal reliability among Mexican Americans (alpha = 0.92). At the end of the interview, the child's height and weight were taken. The participant was then handed a personal digital assistant (PDA) with which to complete the remainder of the survey.

All measures and constructs assessed on the PDA are described in detail in Table 1. Briefly, the primary outcome variable in this study is susceptibility to smoking, which is defined as lacking a firm commitment not to smoke in the future or if offered a cigarette by a friend, integrates behavioral intentions and expectations for future behavior [1]. The construct is assessed among never-smokers only. To be coded as "non-susceptible" participants responded "no" to "Do you think you will try a cigarette soon?" and "definitely not" to "If one of your best friends were to offer you a cigarette would you smoke it?" and "Do you think you will be smoking cigarettes 1 year from now?" Risk factors associated with smoking initiation were assessed in four broad areas: attitudes towards cigarettes, peer and family influences, school characteristics and neighborhood characteristics.

Table 1 Measures assessed on the PDA

Variables available for analysis from the participants' parent

Because our study participants are a sub-cohort of a larger cohort (the population-based infrastructure of Mexican American households maintained by the Department of Epidemiology at UTMDACC), basic descriptive data provided by the participant's parents were available for 93.5% of the participants in the current study. These included educational attainment, nativity status (U.S. or Mexico), years living in the U.S. (assessed among participants born in Mexico only), acculturation, and smoking status.

Educational attainment was divided into three categories: less than high school, high school/General Educational Development equivalency, and more than high school. Acculturation was assessed using four items that assess linguistic proficiency in English from the Bidimensional Acculturation Scale for Hispanics (BAS), a validated acculturation instrument designed for use with Mexican American adults [13]. Scores ranged from 1 to 4, with higher scores reflecting fluency in English and higher levels of acculturation. The scale has very good internal reliability (alpha = 0.88). Smoking status was divided into two categories: ever and never. Ever smokers included current and former smokers who reported smoking at least 100 cigarettes in their lifetime.

Statistical analyses

We conducted Student's t-tests and Pearson chi-square analyses, as appropriate, to examine the associations between smoking susceptibility (non-susceptible vs. susceptible) and children's demographics, their parent's demographics, attitudes toward smoking, peer influences, family influences, school characteristics, and neighborhood characteristics.

To identify the variables that independently predicted susceptibility, we performed an unconditional backward elimination stepwise logistic regression analysis. Prior to completing this multivariable analysis, we examined the correlations among the risk factors that demonstrated a significant bivariate association (p < 0.05) with susceptibility. Because the two peer influence variables ("three best friends smoke," "some friends smoke;" see Table 1) were correlated (r = 0.64; p < 0.01), we created a dichotomous summary measure ("any friends smoke") to use in the multivariate models. Participants who responded "none" on the "How many of your friends smoke?" question and "0" on the "How many of your closest friends smoke?" question were coded as 0; all other participants were coded as 1. However, the correlations among all other risk factors were low. Therefore all risk factors that demonstrated a significant bivariate association (p < 0.05) with susceptibility were entered into the model. All risk factors measured as scaled variables (e.g. attitudes toward smoking, family and peer norms, parental acculturation, and subjective social status) were entered as continuous variables, while the categorical variables were entered as dummy variables. To determine the extent of multicollinearity among the risk factor variables, we examined the variance inflation factor (VIF) for each variable retained in the model.

Finally, we created a cumulative risk score for each participant by summing the risk factors identified from the logistic regression. We dichotomized most risk factors assessed as continuous variables (positive outcome expectations, peer norms, temptations to try cigarettes, and subjective social status) at the median. However, we dichotomized detentions as none or at least one. We then assigned each risk factor the value of 1 and created a risk index by summing risks [14]. Cumulative risk scores ranged between 0 and 8; no participants obtained the highest possible score of 9. We then completed an unconditional logistic regression.

Results

A total of 1,328, participants had completed the baseline cross-sectional survey. Of these, 129 participants (9.7%) were identified as ever-smokers and were excluded from the current analysis. The sample size of never-smokers available for analysis was 1,199. However, because another 12 participants had missing data on the temptations measure the sample size available for both logistic regression models was 1,187. Therefore to be consistent throughout, descriptive data tables are also based on the 1,187 participants. All analyses were conducted using SPSS.

Table 2 summarizes the demographic characteristics of the study participants and their parents by the children's smoking susceptibility. Overall, 262 participants (22.1%) were susceptible to smoking. Boys (25.6%) were more likely to be susceptible than girls (18.9%; p < 0.01). Susceptibles were on average 5 months older than non-susceptibles. There were no differences in susceptibility based on participants' country of birth, length of residency in the US, or language spoken at home, or in their parents' educational attainment. However, while not significantly different, the parents of susceptibles had lived in US for an average of 1.2 years longer than the parents of nonsuscpetibles. They were also more acculturated than the parents of the non-susceptibles (p = 0.04), and susceptible participants were more likely to have a mother who smokes or smoked than non-susceptibles (p = 0.05). The pattern was similar for fathers, but the difference was not significant, likely because of the very small number of fathers enrolled in the study.

Table 2 Summary statistics of children's and parents' characteristics by susceptibility status at baseline

Table 3 presents summary statistics for the attitudinal and social risk factors studied, by susceptibility status. Four of the five attitude measures were associated with susceptibility in the expected direction. Compared to the non-susceptibles, susceptibles perceived fewer "cons" and more "pros" associated with smoking, had higher positive expectations about smoking, and reported more temptations to smoke.

Table 3 Attitudes toward smoking, peer influence, family influence, school characteristics, and neighborhood characteristics, by susceptibility status at baseline (N = 1,187)

All three aspects of peer influence were associated with susceptibility in the expected direction: Susceptibles were more likely to report that their peers strongly approve of their smoking than were non-susceptibles, to report that some of their friends smoke, and to report that at least one of their three best friends smoke. Similarly, the majority of the family influence variables were also associated with susceptibility. Susceptibles were more likely than non-susceptibles to believe that their parents strongly approve of their smoking and to have a father, mother, brother, or sister who currently smokes.

Among the school characteristics, only three were not associated with susceptibility: having taken a health class in which smoking was discussed, participating in extracurricular activities, and participating in school sports. Susceptibles were more likely to believe that students could smoke on campus and to endorse the idea that "smoking is cool." Susceptibles were less likely to endorse the idea that "smoking is stupid," and to believe their parents would be told if they were caught smoking at school. Susceptibles also reported lower subjective social status at school, and more detentions but reported higher grades than non-susceptibles. Three neighborhood characteristics were also associated with susceptibility: susceptibles were less likely to believe that if neighbors saw them smoking the neighbors would report their smoking to their parents, to have seen pro-messages, and a greater proportion of susceptibles considered it easy to buy cigarettes in their neighborhood or near school.

In the stepwise multivariate logistic regression analysis, nine of the risk factors maintained statistical significance (Table 4). Specifically, reporting more positive expectations about smoking (odds ratio [OR] = 4.85), having a brother who smokes (OR = 2.65), reporting that any friends smoke (OR = 2.19), having a mother who smokes (OR = 1.92), believing that peer norms strongly support smoking (OR = 1.76), being 13 years old (OR = 1.43), reporting more temptations to smoke (OR = 1.36), reporting lower subjective social status (OR = 1.13), and having more detentions (OR = 1.08) were independent risk factors for being susceptible to smoking. The highest VIF obtained was 1.19 indicating that collinearity was not present among the nine risk factors retained in the model.

Table 4 Risk factors associated with susceptibility to smoking (N = 1,187)

Finally, we present the results from the logistic regression model based on the cumulative risk score (Table 5). The risk for being susceptible to smoking increased with increasing number of risk factors (ptrend < 0.01). Compared to participants with no risk factors, participants with two risk factors (23.4% of the participants) were 3.06 times more likely to be susceptible, participants with three risk factors (20.6% of the participants) were 3.88 times more likely to be susceptible to smoking, participants with four risk factors (13.1% of the participants) were 10.89 times more likely to be susceptible to smoking, and participants with five or more risk factors (14.2% of participants) were 25.67 times more likely to be susceptible to smoking.

Table 5 Cumulative risk associated with susceptibility to smoking (N = 1,187)

Discussion

Overall, we found that 129 (9.7%) of the study participants reported that they had experimented with cigarettes. Of the remaining 1,187 never-smokers, 262 (22.1%) were susceptible to smoking. It is difficult to directly compare this percentage with those in other studies that have assessed susceptibility to smoking among Latinos because of between-study differences in the age and geographic location of the participants, as well as when and how the data were collected. For example, data collected from in-person household interviews from Mexican origin youth aged 12 to 17 in 2002 through 2004 reported a 28.8% susceptibility rate [7]. However, among a cohort of migrant Latino youth with a mean age of 13 years (standard deviation, ± 1.11), Elder et al. [15] reported a susceptibility rate of 35.6%, which is comparable to the rate of 31.6% we observed among the 13-year-old participants in our study (Table 2).

The principal aim of our study was to investigate the relationship between risk factors for smoking initiation and susceptibility to smoking. Most risk factors studied were associated with susceptibility to smoking, and all associations were in the expected direction. Therefore, first we focus on the results from the logistic regression models and second discuss associations that were not significant. The strongest independent risk factor for susceptibility to smoking in our study was holding positive expectations about smoking (OR = 4.85) (Table 4). Simmons-Morton et al. [16] too reported that youth are more likely to smoke if they think that smoking will yield socially beneficial outcomes, such as gaining more friends and gaining in popularity. Dalton et al. [17] found that both negative and positive outcome expectations are associated with susceptibility to smoking; however, in our study, negative expectations were not significantly associated with susceptibility to smoking. We used the outcome expectations measure developed by Dalton et al., but adjusted for a wider range of variables and still found the summary measure of positive expectations to be the strongest predictor of susceptibility to smoking. Our results, therefore, are consistent with previous findings [16, 17] and combined with the previous studies' results underscore the importance of variables, such as outcome expectations, as predictors of smoking initiation.

In general there is a robust association among sibling's smoking status [18]; in particular smoking by older siblings predicts smoking among younger siblings [19]. We found that having an older brother who currently smokes is associated with a two fold increased odds of being susceptibility to smoking (OR = 2.65). Recent studies demonstrated that having a parent who smokes or smoked is another strong and consistent predictor of smoking initiation [20–26], while parental expectations not to smoke are protective [27]. We examined the role of the fathers' smoking status independent from that of the mothers' smoking status. Although a higher percentage of fathers than mothers were current smokers, only the mothers' smoking status was associated with their children's susceptibility (OR = 1.92). Familismo, which refers to the belief that the needs of the family outweigh the needs of the individual, plays a central role in how Mexican and Mexican American families operate. One component of familismo is the obligation to provide material and emotional support to family members [28]. In many families, this is realized by the father working long hours while the mother stays at home to raise the children and take care of the home. As a result, children spend many more hours with their mothers than fathers and form a stronger emotional bond with their mothers. It is possible, therefore, that the mothers' behaviors, including smoking behaviors, exert a stronger influence on children than the fathers' behaviors. The findings that both mother and brother smoking are associated with susceptibility underscores the need to focus on the family context when developing primary prevention messages targeted to Mexican origin youth.

The roles of both peer smoking and perceived peer norms have been examined extensively in studies of adolescent smoking. Research has typically found a strong association between participants' smoking status and close friends' smoking status [29–31]. We also found that friends' smoking was a risk factor for susceptibility to smoking (OR = 2.19). Because people tend to choose their friends based on shared characteristics [32], one of which could be smoking status, having close friends who smoke does not mean that it was the friends who caused the participant to smoke. However, one aspect of the susceptibility to smoking construct is lacking a firm commitment not to smoke if offered a cigarette by a friend. In addition, our finding that peer approval of smoking is a risk factor for susceptibility to smoking (OR = 1.76) is consistent with previous studies [33, 34]. Together, these results suggest that the peer social context in which youths find themselves plays an important role in determining susceptibility to smoking and warrants further research in this population.

Previous research has demonstrated that older adolescents are significantly more likely to be susceptible to smoking [2, 15], have experimented with cigarettes [35], and smoke than their younger peers [36]. Our findings are consistent with this well-established risk factor.

Temptations to smoke are typically examined among current smokers and recent quitters [37, 38], though temptations to try smoking have been examined as predictors of smoking initiation [39]. To the best of our knowledge, no studies have examined the relationship between temptations to try cigarettes and susceptibility to smoking. The temptations measure assesses how tempted the participant is to try cigarettes in positive social situations, as a means of coping with negative affect, and to satisfy curiosity. We found that participants who reported more temptations to try cigarettes had a higher risk of susceptibility to smoking (OR = 1.36). This finding is consistent with previous studies that reported curiosity [40] and socializing with peers [41, 42] as two of the major reasons why adolescents start smoking.

To date, one study has examined the relationship between subjective social status and smoking, and none have used susceptibility to smoking as an outcome variable. Finkelstein et al. [43] found that among adolescents in grades 7 through 12, those with lower social status were at increased risk of smoking at baseline and initiating smoking during the subsequent year. Using the same measure of subjective social status [44], similarly we found that participants who reported lower perceived social status were at greater risk for being susceptible to smoking (OR = 1.13).

Finally, we found that having had one or more detentions at school was significantly associated with being susceptible to smoking (OR = 1.08). Our finding is consistent with previous research that demonstrates detentions are associated with being susceptible [2, 45] and smoking [46].

Some factors were not associated with susceptibility. Neither the child's country of birth nor the language spoken at home was related to susceptibility status. This is consistent with the finding of Gritz et al. [2] that level of cultural identification was not associated with smoking susceptibility among Latino youth. Parental educational attainment also was not related to children's susceptibility status, although previous studies have noted an inverse relationship between parental educational attainment and smoking [47, 48].

The majority of participants (81%) reported having taken a class in school in which the bad effects of smoking on health were discussed; however, this variable did not impact susceptibility. Evaluations of school-based interventions designed to prevent smoking have demonstrated that knowledge-based interventions alone do not impact behavior [49], while those that teach resistance skills do [50]. In the current study, we do not know what content was presented and discussed in the classes or whether resistance skills were taught.

Studies investigating the association between smoking and participation in school sports have yielded mixed results. While most have found participation in school sports to be protective against smoking [51–53], others have found school sports to be associated with higher rates of smoking [54]. We found no association. It is possible that we did not observe an association because according to local school district policy, only students in the 7th grade and above participate in school sports. Most of our study participants were in the 5th or 6th grade when they enrolled in the study, and those who did answer the question about participation in school sports were likely answering about their participation in physical education classes rather than school sports.

Previous research has demonstrated that messages perceived as pro-tobacco and those perceived as anti-tobacco influence susceptibility to smoking [55] and that exposure to pro-tobacco media and advertising does increase susceptibility to smoking over time [56]. In the current study, these associations were not statistically significant.

Our study has both strengths and limitations. A strength of our study pertains to the large sample size, which allowed us to ascertain the number of co-occurring risk factors that tip the balance from non-susceptible to susceptible. This analysis, based on a cumulative risk score, revealed no differences in susceptibility among participants with zero or one risk factors. However, among the 72% of participants with two or more risk factors, the chances of being susceptible to smoking increased with the number of risk factors. Indeed, it was striking that the 169 participants (14.2% of participants) with five or more risk factors were over 20 times more likely to be susceptible compared to their peers with no risk factors.

Our study also generated some novel findings. To our knowledge, our study is the first to report that low subjective social status and temptations to smoke are associated with susceptibility to smoking. Future research will need to confirm these findings and determine if either or both risk factors generalize to other populations. Other strengths of the study include the fact that our participants were from a population-based cohort and included roughly equal numbers of girls and boys. In addition, we used validated measures and collected the data in the participants' homes using PDAs to ensure their privacy. A final strength of the study was our ability to recruit a large sample of Mexican origin participants, which is an understudied population.

One limitation of this study was that we were not able to examine the relationship between depression and susceptibility to smoking. We also did not have biochemical validation of the participants' smoking status (e.g., cotinine levels in saliva). However, we informed participants during the consent process that they might be selected to provide a saliva sample to check their smoking status; this "bogus pipeline" procedure has been shown to increase the validity of self-reported smoking status [57].

Conclusion

In summary, compared to non-susceptible participants, smoking-susceptible participants were more likely to hold more positive expectations about smoking, have a brother who smoked, report that their friends smoked, have a mother who smoked, believe their peers approve of their smoking, be older, report more temptations to try smoking, report lower subjective social status at school, and have had a detention during the school year. Overall, the strongest risk factor we identified was holding positive expectations about smoking, although both family- and school-based characteristics were important, too. These findings can be used to inform the development of culturally sensitive primary prevention programs.

School-based interventions that target positive expectations about smoking, the role that peers may play in promoting positive expectations, and potential differences in positive expectations among students with differing levels of social status, may be useful in this population. In addition, the findings that having a mother and a brother who smoke increase the risk of being susceptibility underscore the continued need to develop family- and community-based primary prevention programs.

References

  1. Pierce JP, Choi WS, Gilpin EA, Farkas AJ, Merritt RK: Validation of susceptibility as a predictor of which adolescents take up smoking in the United States. Health Psychol. 1996, 15: 355-361. 10.1037/0278-6133.15.5.355.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Gritz ER, Prokhorov AV, Hudmon KS, Chamberlain RM, Taylor WC, DiClemente CC, Johnston DA, Hu S, Jones LA, Jones MM, Rosenblum CK, Ayars CL, Amos CI: Cigarette smoking in a multiethnic population of youth: Methods and baseline findings. Prev Med. 1998, 27: 365-384. 10.1006/pmed.1998.0300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Huang M, Hollis J, Polen M, Lapidus J, Austin D: Stages of smoking acquisition versus susceptibility as predictors of smoking initiation in adolescents in primary care. Addict Behav. 2005, 30: 1183-1194. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.12.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Jackson C: Cognitive susceptibility to smoking and initiation of smoking during childhood: A longitudinal study. Prev Med. 1998, 27: 129-134. 10.1006/pmed.1997.0255.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Unger JB, Johnson CA, Stoddard JL, Nezami E, Chou CP: Identification of adolescents at risk for smoking initiation: Validation of a measure of susceptibility. Addict Behav. 1997, 22: 81-91. 10.1016/0306-4603(95)00107-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Meschack AF, Hu S, Pallonen UE, McAlister AL, Gottlieb N, Huang P: Texas tobacco prevention pilot initiative: Processes and effects. Health Educ Res. 2004, 19: 657-668. 10.1093/her/cyg088.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC): Racial/ethnic differences among youths in cigarette smoking and susceptibility to start smoking – United States, 2002–2004. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2006, 55: 1275-1277.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Youth risk behavior surveillance system (YRBSS) results. [http://apps.nccd.cdc.gov/yrbss/]

  9. Huang P, Alo CJ, Kropp R: Prevalence of Tobacco use among Public Middle and High School Students in Grades 6–12, 1999–2001. 2002, Austin, TX: Texas Department of Health

    Google Scholar 

  10. Guzman B: The Hispanic Population (Census Brief C2KBR/01-3). 2000, Washington, DC: US Census Bureau

    Google Scholar 

  11. Wilkinson AV, Spitz MR, Strom SS, Prokhorov AV, Barcenas CH, Cao Y, Saunders KC, Bondy ML: Effects of nativity, age at migration, and acculturation on smoking among adult Houston residents of Mexican descent. Am J Public Health. 2005, 95: 1043-1049. 10.2105/AJPH.2004.055319.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Marin G, Sabogal R, Marin BV, Otero-Sabogal R, Perez-Stable : Development of a short acculturation scale for Hispanics. Hispanic J Behav Sci. 1987, 9: 183-205. 10.1177/07399863870092005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Marin G, Gamba RJ: A new measurement of acculturation for Hispanics: The bidimensional acculturation scale for Hispanics (BAS). Hispanic J Behavioral Sciences. 1996, 18: 297-316. 10.1177/07399863960183002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Wu X, Lin J, Etzel CJ, Dong Q, Gorlova OY, Zhang Q, Amos CI, Spitz MR: Interplay between mutagen sensitivity and epidemiological factors in modulating lung cancer risk. Int J Cancer. 2007, 120: 2687-2695. 10.1002/ijc.22588.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Elder JP, Campbell NR, Litrownik AJ, Ayala GX, Slymen DJ, Parra-Medina D, Lovato CY: Predictors of cigarette and alcohol susceptibility and use among Hispanic migrant adolescents. Prev Med. 2000, 31: 115-123. 10.1006/pmed.2000.0693.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Simons-Morton B, Crump AD, Haynie DL, Saylor KE, Eitel P, Yu K: Psychosocial, school, and parent factors associated with recent smoking among early-adolescent boys and girls. Prev Med. 1999, 28: 138-148. 10.1006/pmed.1998.0404.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Dalton MA, Sargent JD, Beach ML, Bernhardt AM, Stevens M: Positive and negative outcome expectations of smoking: Implications for prevention. Prev Med. 1999, 29: 460-465. 10.1006/pmed.1999.0582.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Avenevoli S, Merikangas KR: Familial influences on adolescent smoking. Addiction. 2003, 98 (Suppl 1): 1-20. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.98.s1.2.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Rajan KB, Leroux BG, Peterson AV, Bricker JB, Andersen MR, Kealey KA, Sarason IG: Nine-year prospective association between older siblings' smoking and children's daily smoking. J Adolesc Health. 2003, 33: 25-30. 10.1016/S1054-139X(03)00044-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Bricker JB, Peterson AV, Robyn Andersen M, Leroux BG, Bharat Rajan K, Sarason IG: Close friends', parents', and older siblings' smoking: Reevaluating their influence on children's smoking. Nicotine Tob Res. 2006, 8: 217-226. 10.1080/14622200600576339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Chassin L, Presson CC, Pitts SC, Sherman SJ: The natural history of cigarette smoking from adolescence to adulthood in a Midwestern community sample: Multiple trajectories and their psychosocial correlates. Health Psychol. 2000, 19: 223-231. 10.1037/0278-6133.19.3.223.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Fleming CB, Kim H, Harachi TW, Catalano RF: Family processes for children in early elementary school as predictors of smoking initiation. J Adolesc Health. 2002, 30: 184-189. 10.1016/S1054-139X(01)00327-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Griesler PC, Kandel DB, Davies M: Ethnic differences in predictors of initiation and persistence of adolescent cigarette smoking in the national longitudinal survey of youth. Nicotine Tob Res. 2002, 4: 79-93. 10.1080/14622200110103197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hill KG, Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Abbott RD, Guo J: Family influences on the risk of daily smoking initiation. J Adolesc Health. 2005, 37: 202-210. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.08.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kandel DB, Kiros GE, Schaffran C, Hu MC: Racial/ethnic differences in cigarette smoking initiation and progression to daily smoking: A multilevel analysis. Am J Public Health. 2004, 94: 128-135.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Patterson AV, Leroux BG, Bricker J, Kealey KA, Marek PM, Sarason IG, Anderson MR: Nine-year prediction of adolescent smoking by number of smoking parents. Addict Behav. 2006, 31: 788-801. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.06.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Simons-Morton BG: The protective effect of parental expectations against early adolescent smoking initiation. Health Educ Res. 2004, 19: 561-569. 10.1093/her/cyg071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Sabogal F, Marin G, Otero-Sabogal R, Marin BV, Perez-Stable EJ: Hispanic familism and acculturation: What changes and what stays the same. Hispanic J Behav Sci. 1987, 9: 397-412. 10.1177/07399863870094003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Allen M, Donohue WA, Griffin A, Ryan D, Turner MM: Comparing the influence of parents and peers on the first choice to use drugs. Crim Justice Behav. 2003, 30: 163-186. 10.1177/0093854802251002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Andrews JA, Tildesley E, Hops H, Li F: The influence of peers on young adult substance use. Health Psychol. 2002, 21: 349-357. 10.1037/0278-6133.21.4.349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Epstein JA, Bang H, Botvin GJ: Which psychosocial factors moderate or directly affect substance use among inner-city adolescents?. Addict Behav. 2007, 32: 700-713. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.06.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Rose RJ: How do adolescents select their friends? A behavior-genetic perspective. Paths to Successful Development: Personality in the Life Course. Edited by: Pulikinnen L, Caspi A. 2002, New York: Cambridge University Press, 106-125.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  33. Otero-Sabogal R, Sabogal F, Perez-Stable EJ: Psychosocial correlates of smoking among immigrant Latina adolescents. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr. 1995, 65-71. 18

  34. Smith BN, Bean MK, Michell KS, Speizer IS, Fries EA: Psychosocial factors associated with non-smoking adolescents' intentions to smoke. Health Educ Res. 2007, 22: 238-247. 10.1093/her/cyl072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Maggi S, Hertzman C, Vaillancourt T: Changes in smoking behaviors from late childhood to adolescence: Insights from the Canadian national longitudinal survey of children and youth. Health Psychol. 2007, 26: 232-240. 10.1037/0278-6133.26.2.232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Silva MA, Rivera IR, Carvalho AC, Guerra Ade H, Moreira TC: The prevalence of and variables associated with smoking in children and adolescents. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2006, 82: 365-370.

    Google Scholar 

  37. O'Connell K, Schwartz J, Gerkovich M, Bott M, Shiffman S: Playful and rebellious states vs. negative affect in explaining the occurrence of temptations and lapses during smoking cessation. Nicotine Tob Res. 2004, 6: 661-674. 10.1080/14622200410001734049.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Segan CJ, Borland R, Greenwood KM: Can transtheoretical model measures predict relapse from the action stage of change among ex-smokers who quit after calling a quitline?. Addict Behav. 2006, 31: 414-428. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Pallonen UE, Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Prokhorov AV, Smith NF: Stages of acquisition and cessation for adolescent smoking: An empirical integration. Addict Behav. 1998, 23: 303-324. 10.1016/S0306-4603(97)00074-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Pierce JP, Distefan JM, Kaplan RM, Gilpin EA: The role of curiosity in smoking initiation. Addict Behav. 2005, 30: 685-696. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.08.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Quintero G, Davis S: Why do teens smoke? American Indian and Hispanic adolescents' perspectives on functional values and addiction. Med Anthropol Q. 2002, 16: 439-457. 10.1525/maq.2002.16.4.439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Valente TW, Unger JB, Johnson CA: Do popular students smoke? The association between popularity and smoking among middle school students. J Adolesc Health. 2005, 37: 323-329. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.10.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Finkelstein DM, Kubzansky LD, Goodman E: Social status, stress, and adolescent smoking. J Adolesc Health. 2006, 39: 678-685. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.04.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Goodman E, Adler NE, Kawachi I, Frazier AL, Huang B, Colditz GA: Adolescents' perceptions of social status: Development and evaluation of a new indicator. Pediatrics. 2001, 108: E31-10.1542/peds.108.2.e31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gritz ER, Prokhorov AV, Hudmon KS, Mullin Jones M, Rosenblum C, Chang CC, Chamberlain RM, Taylor WC, Johnston D, de Moor C: Predictors of susceptibility to smoking and ever smoking: A longitudinal study in a triethnic sample of adolescents. Nicotine Tob Res. 2003, 5: 493-506. 10.1080/1462220031000118568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Conwell LS, O'Callaghan MJ, Andersen MJ, Bor W, Najman JM, Williams GM: Early adolescent smoking and a web of personal and social disadvantage. J Paediatr Child Health. 2003, 39: 580-585. 10.1046/j.1440-1754.2003.00240.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, Edwards DA: Parent educational attainment and adolescent cigarette smoking. J Subst Abuse. 1992, 4: 219-234. 10.1016/0899-3289(92)90031-R.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Vereecken CA, Maes L, De Bacquer D: The influence of parental occupation and the pupils' educational level on lifestyle behaviors among adolescents in belgium. J Adolesc Health. 2004, 34: 330-338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Lindberg LC, Stahle A, Ryden L: Long-term influence of a health education program on knowledge and health behavior in children. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2006, 13: 91-97. 10.1097/00149831-200602000-00014.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Botvin GJ, Dusenbury L, Baker E, James-Ortiz S, Kerner J: A skills training approach to smoking prevention among Hispanic youth. J Behav Med. 1989, 12: 279-296. 10.1007/BF00844872.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Audrain-McGovern J, Rodriguez D, Wileyto EP, Schmitz KH, Shields PG: Effect of team sport participation on genetic predisposition to adolescent smoking progression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006, 63: 433-441. 10.1001/archpsyc.63.4.433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Escobedo LG, Marcus SE, Holtzman D, Giovino GA: Sports participation, age of smoking initiation, and the risk of smoking among US high school students. JAMA. 1993, 269: 1319-1395. 10.1001/jama.269.11.1391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Nelson MC, Gordon-Larsen P: Physical activity and sedentary behavior patterns are associated with selected adolescent health risk behaviors. Pediatrics. 2006, 117: 1281-1290. 10.1542/peds.2005-1692.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Garry JP, Morrissey SL: Team sports participation and risk-taking behaviors among a biracial middle school population. Clin J Sport Med. 2000, 10: 185-190. 10.1097/00042752-200007000-00006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Straub DM, Hills NK, Thompson PJ, Moscicki AB: Effects of pro- and anti-tobacco advertising on nonsmoking adolescents' intentions to smoke. J Adolesc Health. 2003, 32: 36-43. 10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00451-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Weiss JW, Cen S, Schuster DV, Unger JB, Johnson CA, Mouttapa M, Schreiner WS, Cruz TB: Longitudinal effects of pro-tobacco and anti-tobacco messages on adolescent smoking susceptibility. Nicotine Tob Res. 2006, 8: 455-465. 10.1080/14622200600670454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Murray DM, O'Connell CM, Schmid LA, Perry CL: The validity of smoking self-reports by adolescents: A reexamination of the bogus pipeline procedure. Addict Behav. 1987, 12: 7-15. 10.1016/0306-4603(87)90003-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Hudmon KS, Prokhorov AV, Koehly LM, DiClemente CC, Gritz ER: Psychometric properties of the decisional balance scale and the temptations to try smoking inventory in adolescents. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse. 1997, 6: 1-18. 10.1300/J029v06n03_01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Velicer WF, Diclemente CC, Rossi JS, Prochaska JO: Relapse situations and self-efficacy: An integrative model. Addict Behav. 1990, 15: 271-283. 10.1016/0306-4603(90)90070-E.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Epstein JA, Botvin GJ, Diaz T: Social influence and psychological determinants of smoking among inner-city adolescents. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse. 1999, 8: 1-19. 10.1300/J029v08n03_01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Carvajal SC, Wiatrek DE, Evans RI, Knee CR, Nash SG: Psychosocial determinants of the onset and escalation of smoking: Cross-sectional and prospective findings in multiethnic middle school samples. J Adolesc Health. 2000, 27: 255-265. 10.1016/S1054-139X(00)00124-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research is supported by the National Cancer Institute grant CA105203 (MRS) and CA126988 (AVW), by funds collected pursuant to the Comprehensive Tobacco Settlement of 1998 and appropriated by the 76th legislature to The University of Texas M. D. Anderson Cancer Center, and by the Caroline W. Law Fund for Cancer Prevention. In addition, we thank the cohort staff for their on-going work with participant recruitment and follow-up. Most importantly, we thank our study participants and their parents for their cooperation and participation, without which this research would not be possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anna V Wilkinson.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

AVW and AJW took the leads in writing the manuscript, computed initial analysis, and interpreted the data. VV completed additional analysis, coordinated the drafting of the manuscript, and was responsible for the referencing. MLB and AVP interpreted the results and provided critical feedback on the drafts. MRS conceived the study, interpreted the results, and provided critical revisions.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wilkinson, A.V., Waters, A.J., Vasudevan, V. et al. Correlates of susceptibility to smoking among Mexican origin youth residing in Houston, Texas: A cross-sectional analysis. BMC Public Health 8, 337 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-8-337

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-8-337

Keywords