Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptomatology: examination of several personality-related characteristics as potential confounders in a racial/ethnic heterogeneous adult sample

Abstract

Background

Research suggests that reports of interpersonal discrimination result in poor mental health. Because personality characteristics may either confound or mediate the link between these reports and mental health, there is a need to disentangle its role in order to better understand the nature of discrimination-mental health association. We examined whether hostility, anger repression and expression, pessimism, optimism, and self-esteem served as confounders in the association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and CESD-based depressive symptoms in a race/ethnic heterogeneous probability-based sample of community-dwelling adults.

Methods

We employed a series of ordinary least squares regression analyses to examine the potential confounding effect of hostility, anger repression and expression, pessimism, optimism, and self-esteem between interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms.

Results

Hostility, anger repression, pessimism and self-esteem were significant as possible confounders of the relationship between interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms, together accounting for approximately 38% of the total association (beta: 0.1892, p < 0.001). However, interpersonal discrimination remained a positive predictor of depressive symptoms (beta: 0.1176, p < 0.001).

Conclusion

As one of the first empirical attempts to examine the potential confounding role of personality characteristics in the association between reports of interpersonal discrimination and mental health, our results suggest that personality-related characteristics may serve as potential confounders. Nevertheless, our results also suggest that, net of these characteristics, reports of interpersonal discrimination are associated with poor mental health.

Peer Review reports

Background

Recent reports indicate that roughly nine percent of adults report symptoms of depression, making depression the most common mental illness in the United States (US) [1]. Furthermore, depression is the leading cause of disability for individuals ages 15–44, with an estimated loss of labor productivity exceeding $31 billion per year [2]. Researchers hypothesize that daily stressors may be an important factor in the development of depressive symptoms and disorders [3].

Evidence suggests that perceived interpersonal everyday discrimination is an important type of daily stressor associated with depressive symptoms that may have particular salience for racial/ethnic groups such as African Americans, Hispanics, and Asians [411]. In particular, evidence suggests that there is a positive association between reports of interpersonal discrimination and psychological distress, including depressive symptoms [12]. An important limitation in the literature, however, is a lack of clarity on the role of personality-related characteristics in this association. Specifically, it is unclear whether personality-related characteristics mediate or confound the association between interpersonal discrimination and psychological distress.

Personality-related characteristics, reports of interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms

To be considered a confounder, a personality-related characteristic would alter the report of both interpersonal discrimination (due either to exposure or perception) and depressive symptoms [13]. If a confounder, the observed association between interpersonal discrimination and mental health will be biased or altogether spurious. On the other hand, to be considered a mediator, the personality-related characteristic would result (partially or fully) from perceived experiences of interpersonal discrimination and then, in turn, alter the reports of depressive symptoms [13]. While there are some studies that suggest that certain personality-related characteristics (e.g., anger, self-esteem and hostility) increase in response to discriminatory experiences, there is a dearth of empirical examinations on the role of these characteristics in the association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms. In one study, researchers examined whether personality-related characteristics confounded the association between interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms in a sample of 250 gay and bi-sexual men [14]. They found that hostility and neuroticism were both significant confounders of the association between perceived anti-gay discrimination and depressive symptoms, together accounting for 42% of the association. Nevertheless, the interpersonal discrimination measure remained a significant predictor for depressive symptoms [14].

While there is a dearth of evidence specifically on the confounding or mediating role of personality-related characteristics, there is some literature on the separate associations among discrimination, personality-related characteristics, and mental health. For example, research has shown that both anger repression (inward reactions or behaviors when experiencing feelings of anger) and anger expression (reactions or behaviors towards others when experiencing feelings of anger) are related to both reports of interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms [1524]. While it may be that perceived interpersonal discrimination results in anger, suggesting a mediating role, results from other research have reported that the perceived expression of anger can serve to encourage or prompt discriminatory behavior from others, suggesting a confounding role [25].

Cynical hostility, a personality-related characteristic denoted by general cynicism and mistrust [26], has been shown to be associated with both perceived interpersonal discrimination [14] and depressive symptoms [2729]. Research has shown that hostility may be a response to perceptions of interpersonal discrimination suggesting a mediating role [16, 23]. However, like anger, hostility may elicit discriminatory responses from others suggesting a confounding role [14]. In fact, research has shown that individuals with high levels of hostility report more suspicious and fewer rational thoughts in reaction to interpersonal scenarios compared to those with low levels of hostility [30]. Similarly, a person with negative affect, such as high hostility, may erroneously recollect past experiences as discriminatory, even though these experiences would not be assessed similarly by a person without negative affect [31].

Likewise, the role of optimism and pessimism is not entirely clear base don the existing literature. Low levels of optimism, which is the generalized expectancy that one will experience good outcomes in the future [27], has been associated with a higher likelihood of interpreting an interpersonal event as discriminatory [9, 28]. Similarly, high levels of pessimism have been associated with a higher likelihood of interpreting a negative interpersonal event as discriminatory [29]. This suggests that optimism and pessimism may have confounding roles in the association between reports of interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms [3234]. On the other hand, some research suggests that stressful situations that are interpreted as uncontrollable [3538] may decrease optimism and increase pessimism and subsequently increase depression [32]. Interpersonal discrimination may be viewed as an uncontrollable form of stress [15, 16], which would imply a mediating role for optimism and pessimism.

Self-esteem, defined as a person’s overall sense of self-worth or personal value, has been linked with both reports of interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms in many [24, 3642] but not all studies [43, 44]. While there is debate in the literature on the causal direction of the discrimination-self-esteem association [44], it may be that some groups of individuals who experience interpersonal discrimination internalize some of the negative social devaluation of their group leading to lowered self-esteem [45] (and other poor mental health outcomes [4649]), suggesting a mediating role. On the other hand, other study findings suggest that, similar to optimism, higher self-esteem is associated with lower likelihoods of perceiving interpersonal discrimination [50] suggesting a confounding role.

In order to better understand the association between reports of interpersonal discrimination and mental health, research is needed to clarify the mediating and/or confounding role of personality-related characteristics in this association [14, 51, 52]. Internalized discrimination, which is a self-defeatist mindset developed from the internalization of negative messages of a particular group membership, may result from interpersonal discriminatory experiences. Researchers have argued that this internalized discrimination may be associated with certain personality-related characteristics, such as low self-esteem and periodic states of anger. However, it may be that adverse social conditions result in certain personality-related characteristics that may, in turn, result in more reports of interpersonal discrimination. For example, Williams [53] suggests that the high levels of material deprivation found in some racially- or ethnically-segregated communities are often associated with factors (e.g. low socioeconomic status (SES), poor quality medical care, etc.) that may promote high levels of low self-esteem and anger, which subsequently may lead to reports of perceived interpersonal discrimination. In other words, contextual factors at the neighborhood or community level may influence the development of personality-related characteristics in ways that impact the perception of interpersonal discrimination or increase the likelihood that others will act in discriminatory ways. Taken together, this body of research suggests that personality-related characteristics might serve as both mediators and confounders in the association between interpersonal discrimination and mental health.

In this study we chose a priori to examine the potential confounding (rather than the mediating) role of several personality-related characteristics in the association between perceived experiences of interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms. This approach was selected for two main reasons. First, as with the existing literature, the data we use is cross-sectional in nature, making it impossible to statistically distinguish between a confounding and mediating role. Indeed, it is likely that both mediation and confounding may be at play in some type of feedback (cross-lagged) loop for some, if not all, of the personality-related characteristics we examined. Second, critics of the perceived interpersonal discrimination measures may argue that these measures do not reflect actual experiences of discrimination, but simply the psychological characteristics of the respondent. This would imply that the psychological characteristics, not the perceptions of discrimination, are really harmful for mental health.

We examined the extent to which several personality-related characteristics (hostility, anger repression and expression, pessimism, optimism and self-esteem), collectively served to possibly confound the association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms in a racial/ethnic heterogeneous probability-based sample of community-dwelling adults. We hypothesized that: (1) personality-related characteristics would possibly confound the association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms; and (2) perceived interpersonal discrimination would remain significantly associated with depressive symptoms after adjusting for these potential confounders.

Methods

Sample

The Chicago Community Adult Health Study (CCAHS) is a stratified, multi-stage probability sample of 3,105 adults aged 18 years and over, living in Chicago, Illinois [54]. Briefly, the CCAHS, conducted between May 2001 and March 2003, obtained both self-reported and objective measures of individual and household psychosocial risk factors and resources, social factors, and residential contexts. The CCAHS sample includes 802 Hispanics, 1240 non-Hispanic Blacks (Blacks), 983 non-Hispanic Whites (Whites), and 80 individuals of other races/ethnicities. One adult per household was interviewed face-to-face, with a response rate of 71.82%. The weighted sample matched the distribution of the 2000 Census population estimates for the city of Chicago in age, race/ethnicity and sex. Additional study details are provided elsewhere [54].

Measures

Depressive symptoms were measured using the 11-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CESD) [55]. The CESD has been validated across various groups including by race and gender [52, 53, 56]. Participants were asked the frequency, in the previous two weeks, with which they experienced certain feelings (e.g. hopelessness, restless sleep, loneliness, fear, sadness and changes in appetite). Responses were provided on a Likert-like scale of 1(never) to 4 (most of the time). A scale was created as the sum of the responses, divided by 11, for a range of 1.00 to 3.82 (Cronbach’s α = 0.85).

The perceived everyday discrimination scale, a measure of perceived day-to-day interpersonal discrimination, assesses the occurrence and frequency with which individuals encounter routine and relatively minor experiences of unfair treatment [57]. In this study, participants were asked how often in their lifetime (1) they were treated with less courtesy or respect than others, (2) they received poorer service than others, (3) they believed others acted as if they were not smart, (4) others acted as if they were afraid of them, or (5) they felt threatened or harassed. Responses were provided on a Likert-like scale of 1(at least once a week) to 5 (never). A scale was created as the sum of the reverse-coded responses, divided by 5, for a range of 0 (no perceived discrimination) to 4 (highest level of perceived discrimination) (Cronbach’s α = 0.75).

Cynical hostility was measured using a modified 5-item version of the Cook-Medley cynical hostility scale [58]. Participants were asked the extent to which they agreed with the following statements: 1) most people inwardly dislike putting themselves out to help other people; 2) most people will use somewhat unfair means to gain profit or an advantage rather than lose it; 3) no one cares much what happens to you; 4) I think most people would lie in order to get ahead; 5) I commonly wonder what hidden reasons another person may have for doing something nice for me. Responses were provided on a Likert-like scale of 1 (agree strongly) to 4 (disagree strongly). The scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.74) was created with the mean of the five statements resulting in a range of values of 1 (lowest level of cynical hostility) to 4 (highest level of cynical hostility).

Expressed and repressed anger were measured using an abridged version of Spielberger’s anger-out and anger-in expression scales, respectively [59]. Participants were asked the frequency of specific typical reactions or behaviors when they are angry or mad. Expressed anger reactions and behaviors included: arguing with others, striking out, saying nasty things, and losing temper. Repressed anger reactions and behaviors include: keeping things in, withdrawing from people, getting irritated more than people are aware, and getting angrier more than willing to admit. Responses were provided on a Likert-like scale of 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always). The anger-in scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.71), constructed by taking the mean of the values for each of the four statements, had a range of 1 (lowest level of anger-In) to 4 (highest level of anger-In). The anger-out scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.76), constructed by taking the mean of the values of the four anger-out statements, had a range of 1 (lowest level of anger-out) to 4 (highest level of anger-out).

Three items from the Life Orientation Test–Revised [60], was used to form a scale that assesses dispositional optimism and pessimism (Cronbach’s α = 0.73). A sample item is “In uncertain times, I usually expect the best.” Participants rated the extent of their agreement with each item on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

We used an abridged 4-item version of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale [61]. Participants reported the extent to which they agreed with statements about their positive attitude toward themselves, satisfaction with themselves, feelings of usefulness and overall goodness. Responses were provided on a Likert-like scale of 1 (disagree strongly) to 4 (agree strongly). The range of values for the self-esteem scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.71) is from 1 (lowest level of self-esteem) to 4 (highest level of self-esteem).

Covariates included age, sex, race/ethnicity (Hispanics, non-Hispanic Blacks, non-Hispanic Others and non-Hispanic Whites), annual household income category ($4,000, $5,000-$9,999, $10,000-$29,999, $30,000-$49,999 and ≥ $50,000), lifetime education status (< 12 years, 12 years, and > 12 years), employment status (employed and not currently employed), marital status (married and not currently married), nativity status (US and foreign born), a count of self-reported chronic health conditions (ranging from 0–9) and a count of stressful major adverse life events (0–11).

Data analyses

The analytic aim of the paper was to determine whether and how much the selected personality-related variables potentially confounded the relationship between interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms. We first estimated means with standard errors of continuous variables and percentages within categorical variables in our total sample (see Table 1). We then examined the first-order correlation between perceived interpersonal discrimination and the personality-related characteristics (see Table 2).

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of participants in the Chicago community adult health study, 2001–03 (N = 3,105)
Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations (SD), Coefficient Alphas (α) and Pearson correlations of the personality-related measures

We used multivariable ordinary least squares (OLS) regression analyses to estimate the confounding role of the personality-related characteristics in a series of models, controlling for the covariates shown in Table 1 (see Table 3). First, we examined the total effect of perceived interpersonal discrimination on depressive symptoms controlling for the covariates (path c). We then separately characterized the association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and each of the personality-related characteristics as the outcome (path α). Although seemingly counterintuitive at first glance, as MacKinnon [13] noted, the proper regression models to test for possible confounding require that the potential confounder (i.e., personality-related characteristics) be modeled as an outcome of the predictor (i.e., perceived interpersonal discrimination) [13]. Next, we examined the effect of interpersonal discrimination on depressive symptoms (path β), controlling for all of the personality-related characteristics. We used the Sobel method [62], which divides the product of nonstandardized coefficients for path α and path β by the by the standard error of this product, to determine if the personality-related characteristics were statistically significant in their presumed role as potential confounders. Lastly, we modeled the effect of perceived interpersonal discrimination on depressive symptoms (path c’) controlling for all of the personality-related characteristics. Informed by previous investigations, multiplicative interaction terms between the interpersonal discrimination variables and race/ethnicity were explored in the multivariable analyses; however, because none were significant, the multivariable analyses included race/ethnicity as a covariate.

Table 3 Unstandardized regression coefficients 1 from OLS regression predicting CESD 2 using personality-related characteristics (n = 3,105)

Although likely missing at random, missing data were imputed using an iterative method that imputes multiple variables by using chained equations, a sequence of univariate imputation methods with fully conditional specification of prediction equations [63]. The final sample post-imputation for this investigation consisted of 3,105 respondents. All analyses were weighted to account for non-response and complex survey design. With the exception of the correlation analyses presented in Table 2, all of the analyses used the imputed data. All of the analyses were conducted using STATA (v12.1, Stata Corp., 2011).

Results

The weighted sociodemographic characteristics are presented in Table 1. Respondents were on average 43 years of age, with an age range of 18–92. Approximately half of the sample was male, 26% Hispanic, 38% White, 32% Black and 3.8% of another racial/ethnic group. The average educational level was approximately 13 years (53% reporting more than 12 years of education) and approximately 44% of the sample reported less than $30,000 in annual household income.

Table 2 shows the means, standard deviations, and correlation coefficients for the perceived interpersonal discrimination, depressive symptoms and personality-related characteristics. The mean CESD and interpersonal discrimination scores were 1.86 and 0.75, respectively; these measures were positively correlated. Both perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms were positively correlated with the personality-related characteristics, with the exception of optimism and self-esteem which were negatively correlated with the depression symptoms and interpersonal discrimination variables.

Standardized coefficients representing the personality-related characteristics from the regression models were used to construct the path model depicted in Figure 1. Controlling for the covariates in Table 1, perceived interpersonal discrimination positively predicted depressive symptoms (path c = 0.2545, p < 0.001). However, after controlling for the potential confounding role of the personality-related characteristics, the magnitude of the association between interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms was reduced but not eliminated (path c’ = 0.1582, p < 0.001). With the exception of the anger expression-depressive symptoms association, all of the personality-related characteristics were statistically related to both interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms (p < 0.05). Individuals who reported high levels of hostility, anger repression, and pessimism were more likely to report higher levels of depressive symptoms and higher levels of interpersonal discrimination than individuals who reported lower levels of these personality-related characteristics. In contrast, individuals with higher levels of optimism and self-esteem were more likely to report lower levels of interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms than those who reported lower levels of optimism and self-esteem. Following self-esteem (standardized beta = −0.2801, p < 0.001), interpersonal discrimination was the largest predictor of depressive symptoms after controlling for the potentially confounding effects of the personality-related characteristics.

Figure 1
figure 1

Potential confounding effects of the personality-related characteristics on the relation between perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms.

Table 3 presents the unstandardized regression coefficients, the Sobel test statistics, standard errors, and p-values, for each of the personality-related characteristics illustrating the hypothesized individual and total potential confounding role. Results from the Sobel test suggest that hostility, anger repression, pessimism, and self-esteem were all possible confounders (p < 0.001). The overall indirect effect of the personality-related characteristics was 0.0716, accounting for approximately 38% of the original association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms.

Discussion

We examined the role of several personality-related characteristics as potential confounders between perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms in a racially/ethnically-heterogeneous, probability-based sample of community-dwelling adults. Although some of these personality-related characteristics were potential confounders, the association between interpersonal discrimination and depression symptoms persisted. While others have examined separate pieces of the associations among perceived interpersonal discrimination, various personality characteristics, and depression, this study is one of the first to characterize the possible confounding role of more than two personality-related characteristics. An additional strength of this study is the generalizability of the results, as we used a representative, multiethnic sample of community dwelling adults, unlike previous studies.

Overall, the findings of this study are consistent with this literature in several ways. First, higher levels of perceived interpersonal discrimination were associated with increased depressive symptoms, controlling for all of the personality-related characteristics and additional covariates. Notably, perceived interpersonal discrimination was the second largest predictor of depressive symptoms after self-esteem, when controlling for the other personality-related characteristics [411]. Second, increased levels of hostility, the outward expression of anger, and pessimism were all positively related to higher levels of depressive symptoms [64]. Third, high levels of hostility [14] and the outward expression of anger [19, 65] predicted high levels of perceived interpersonal discrimination.

Although researchers suggest that personality may confound the association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and mental health, there is a dearth of empirical work in this area. In one study, Huebner and his colleagues [14] showed that anti-gay interpersonal discrimination was associated with depression after controlling for the potential confounding effects of hostility and neuroticism. Along with our results, these findings suggest that some personality-related characteristics may indeed be antecedent to interpersonal discrimination. Nevertheless, perceived interpersonal discrimination remained an important risk factor for depressive symptoms.

The stability of reports of discrimination over long periods of time might lend support for the notion that personality-related characteristics are subsequent to reports of interpersonal discrimination, and thus play a mediating role. The literature on reports of interpersonal discrimination over time is limited, as there are few datasets with this information at more than one time point. However, the small literature suggests that there is some stability in reports for a large portion of the population. For example, using data from the 1995–2005 cohort of the Midlife Development in the United States, researchers showed that approximately one quarter of the sample reported each of the possible trends over time: consistently low levels, consistently high levels, a decrease, or an increase in perceived interpersonal discrimination [66]. In a study of multiracial/ethnic, middle-aged women, reports of perceived interpersonal discrimination in the past 12 months were relatively stable (range of the within-person stability varied from 0.85 to 0.91) over the course of four to five years [67]. Stability in reports of interpersonal discrimination could be due stability in exposure (i.e. people live, work, play in the same environments from year to year). Alternatively, there may be stability in the perceptions of interpersonal discrimination, which might suggest that these perceptions are more influenced by personality characteristics than originally hypothesized in the existing literature.

Although this study is one of the first to examine the potential confounding role of personality-related characteristics, it is not without limitations. First, to reduce the respondents’ response burden, abbreviated scales of the psychometric measures were employed. Although the reliability of some of the measures may have been reduced, the correlations between measures were strong.

Second, our data were cross-sectional, as has been the case for most empirical studies in this area. Many of the personality-related characteristics we examined are not only correlated with depressive symptoms and interpersonal discrimination, but potentially causally linked through mechanisms not fully understood. It is not fully clear whether the personality-related characteristics are mediators or confounders. In line with previous research, we a priori conducted our analyses to determine whether the personality-related characteristics were antecedents to perceived interpersonal discrimination [14, 50]; however, because interpersonal discrimination may in fact be antecedent to the personality-related characteristics [19, 6871], they may be mediators and not confounders. Because confounding and mediation effects in cross-sectional data are generally estimated with the same statistical methods, the association of interest can only be distinguished on conceptual or theoretical grounds [13]. Despite the fact that some studies suggest that interpersonal discrimination is antecedent to various personality-related characteristics (implying mediation and not confounding), most of these studies use cross-sectional data, exposing them to the same limitation [19, 4042, 44, 64, 70, 7278].

We are aware of only two studies that have used longitudinal data to disentangle the directionality of these associations [19, 68]. In a diary study with 113 adults that collected entries over a one-day period, Broudy et al. [19] showed that baseline measures of ethnic interpersonal discrimination were positively associated with daily levels of anger. Similarly, results based on a longitudinal sample of North American indigenous adolescents, perceived interpersonal discrimination was associated with increased anger over a longer period of time [68]. However, we are not aware of any studies that have specifically examined the potential mediating-confounding association of personality-related characteristics with perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms over time. Indeed, it is likely that personality-related characteristics serve as both mediators and confounders in this association.

Future studies utilizing longitudinal data, along with methods that can address potential cycling or cross-lagged effects (e.g., perceived interpersonal discrimination influencing hostility which in turn influences perceived interpersonal discrimination which furthers influences hostility, and so on) are warranted. Because some personality-related characteristics, both positive and negative, may change over time, future research in this area must pay special attention to these methodological issues.

Conclusion

This study is among the first to empirically characterize the potential confounding role of multiple personality-related characteristics simultaneously in the association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptoms in a representative, multi-ethnic sample of community dwelling adults. Our results suggest that personality-related characteristics potentially confound this association. Nevertheless, perceived interpersonal discrimination remained significantly associated with depressive symptoms after adjusting for these potential confounders, supporting results from population-based studies that have consistently showed that perceived interpersonal discrimination is deleterious to an individual’s health and well-being [52]. In order to clarify the confounding versus mediating role of personality-related characteristics, future research using longitudinal data and sophisticated methods to disentangle the causal association between perceived interpersonal discrimination and mental health over time is warranted.

References

  1. CDC: Current depression among adults- United States, 2006 and 2008. MMWR. 2010, 59 (38): 1229-1235.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Stewart W, Ricci J, Chee E, Hahn S, Morganstein D: Cost of lost productive work time among us workers with depression. JAMA. 2003, 289 (23): 3135-3144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lewinsohn PM, Hoberman HM, Rosenbaum M: A prospective study of risk factors for unipolar depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 1988, 97 (3): 251-264.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Chae DH, Lee S, Lincoln KD, Ihara ES: Discrimination, family relationships, and major depression among Asian Americans. J Immigr Minor Health. 2012, 14 (3): 361-370.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Chou K-L: Perceived discrimination and depression among new migrants to Hong Kong: the moderating role of social support and neighborhood collective efficacy. J Affect Disord. 2012, 138 (1–2): 63-70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Lorenzo-Blanco EI, Unger JB, Ritt-Olson A, Soto D, Baezconde-Garbanati L: Acculturation, gender, depression, and cigarette smoking among US hispanic youth: the mediating role of perceived discrimination. J Youth Adolesc. 2011, 40 (11): 1519-1533.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Lewin A, Mitchell SJ, Rasmussen A, Sanders-Phillips K, Joseph JG: Do human and social capital protect young African American mothers from depression associated with ethnic discrimination and violence exposure?. J Black Psychol. 2011, 37 (3): 286-310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Berg AO, Melle I, Rossberg JI, Romm KL, Larsson S, Lagerberg TV, Andreassen OA, Hauff E: Perceived discrimination is associated with severity of positive and depression/anxiety symptoms in immigrants with psychosis: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry. 2011, 11: 7-

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Odom EC, Vernon-Feagans L, Family Life Project Key I: Buffers of racial discrimination: links with depression among rural African American mothers. J Marriage Fam. 2010, 72 (2): 346-359.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Schulz AJ, Gravlee CC, Williams DR, Israel BA, Mentz G, Rowe Z: Discrimination, symptoms of depression, and self-rated health among african american women in detroit: results from a longitudinal analysis. Am J Public Health. 2006, 96 (7): 1265-1270.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Noh S, Kaspar V: Perceived discrimination and depression: moderating effects of coping, acculturation, and ethnic support. Am J Public Health. 2003, 93 (2): 232-238.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Pascoe EA, Richman LS: Perceived discrimination and health: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2009, 135 (4): 531-554.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. MacKinnon D, Krull J, Lockwood C: Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev Sci. 2000, 1 (4): 173-181.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Huebner DM, Nemeroff CJ, Davis MC: Do hostility and neuroticism confound associations between perceived discrimination and depressive symptoms?. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2005, 24 (5): 723-740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Feagin JR, Sikes MP: Living with racism : the black middle-class experience. 1994, Boston: Beacon Press

    Google Scholar 

  16. Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, Williams DR: Racism as a stressor for African Americans. A biopsychosocial model. Am Psychol. 1999, 54 (10): 805-816.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Swim JK, Hyers LL, Cohen LL, Ferguson MJ: Everyday sexism: evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact from three daily diary studies. J Soc Issues. 2001, 57 (1): 31-53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Brondolo E, Thompson S, Brady N, Appel R, Cassells A, Tobin JN, Sweeney M: The relationship of racism to appraisals and coping in a community sample. Ethn Dis. 2005, 15 (4 Suppl 5): S5-14-19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Broudy R, Brondolo E, Coakley V, Brady N, Cassells A, Tobin JN, Sweeney M: Perceived ethnic discrimination in relation to daily moods and negative social interactions. J Behav Med. 2007, 30 (1): 31-43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Gibbons FX, Etcheverry PE, Stock ML, Gerrard M, Weng CY, Kiviniemi M, O’Hara RE: Exploring the link between racial discrimination and substance use: what mediates? What buffers?. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010, 99 (5): 785-801.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Borders A, Liang CTH: Rumination partially mediates the associations between perceived ethnic discrimination, emotional distress, and aggression. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol. 2011, 17 (2): 125-133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Huynh VW: Ethnic microaggressions and the depressive and somatic symptoms of Latino and Asian American adolescents. J Youth Adolesc. 2012, 41 (7): 831-846.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Lee H, Turney K: Investigating the relationship between perceived discrimination, social status, and mental health. Society and Mental Health. 2012, 2 (1): 1-20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Williams DR, John DA, Oyserman D, Sonnega J, Mohammed SA, Jackson JS: Research on discrimination and health: an exploratory study of unresolved conceptual and measurement issues. Am J Public Health. 2012, 102 (5): 975-978.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Brondolo E, Brady ver Halen N, Pencille M, Beatty D, Contrada R: Coping with racism: a selective review of the literature and a theoretical and methodological critique. J Behav Med. 2009, 32 (1): 64-88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Smith TW: Personality as risk and resilience in physical health. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2006, 15 (5): 227-231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Scheier MF, Carver CS: Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being- theorectical overview and empirical update. Cogn Ther Res. 1992, 16 (2): 201-228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Grote NK, Bledsoe SE, Larkin J, Lemay EP, Brown C: Stress exposure and depression in disadvantaged women: the protective effects of optimism and perceived control. Soc Work Res. 2007, 31 (1): 19-33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Bromberger JT, Matthews KA: A longitudinal study of the effects of pessimism, trait anxiety, and life stress on depressive symptoms in middle-aged women. Psychol Aging. 1996, 11 (2): 207-213.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Pope MK, Smith TW, Rhodewalt F: Cognitive, behavioral, and affective correlates of the cook and medley hostility scale. J Pers Assess. 1990, 54 (3–4): 501-514.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Brondolo E, Brady N, Thompson S, Tobin JN, Cassells A, Sweeney M, Mcfarlane D, Contrada RJ: Perceived racism and negative affect: analyses of trait and state measures of affect in a community sample. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2008, 27 (2): 150-173.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Scheier MF, Weintraub JK, Carver CS: Coping with stress - divergent strategies of optimists and pessimists. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986, 51 (6): 1257-1264.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Scott LD: Cultural orientation and coping with perceived discrimination among African American youth. J Black Psychol. 2003, 29 (3): 235-256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Sechrist GB: Making attributions to and plans to confront gender discrimination: the role of optimism. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010, 40 (7): 1678-1707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Szalacha LA, Erkut S, Coll CG, AlarcA3 O, Fields JP, Ceder I: Discrimination and Puerto Rican children’s and adolescents’ mental health. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol. 2003, 9 (2): 141-155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Moradi B, Risco C: Perceived discrimination experiences and mental health of Latina/o American persons. J Couns Psychol. 2006, 53 (4): 411-421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Greene ML, Way N, Pahl K: Trajectories of perceived adult and peer discrimination among black, Latino, and Asian American adolescents: patterns and psychological correlates. Dev Psychol. 2006, 42 (2): 218-238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Fisher CB, Wallace SA, Fenton RE: Discrimination distress during adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2000, 29 (6): 679-695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Utsey SO, Ponterotto JG, Reynolds AL, Cancelli AA: Racial discrimination, coping, life satisfaction, and self-esteem among African Americans. J Couns Dev. 2000, 78 (1): 72-80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Major B, Kaiser CR, O’Brien LT, McCoy SK: Perceived discrimination as worldview threat or worldview confirmation: implications for self-esteem. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007, 92 (6): 1068-1086.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Smokowski P, Bacallao M: Acculturation, internalizing mental health symptoms, and self-esteem: cultural experiences of Latino adolescents in North Carolina. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2007, 37 (3): 273-292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Panchanadeswaran S, Dawson BA: How discrimination and stress affects self-esteem among Dominican immigrant women: an exploratory study. Soc Work Public Health. 2011, 26 (1): 60-77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kobrynowicz D, Branscombe NR: Who considers themselves vitoms of discrimination?. Psychol Women Q. 1997, 21 (3): 347-363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Ruggiero KM, Taylor DM: Why minority group members perceive or do not perceive the discrimination that confronts them: the role of self-esteem and perceived control. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997, 72 (2): 373-389.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Crocker J, Major B: Social stigma and self-esteem: the self-protective properties of stigma. Psychol Rev. 1989, 96 (4): 608-630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Fischer AR, Shaw CM: African Americans’ mental health and perceptions of racist discrimination: the moderating effects of racial socialization experiences and self-esteem. J Couns Psychol. 1999, 46 (3): 395-407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Cassidy C, O’Connor RC, Howe C, Warden D: Perceived discrimination and psychological distress: the role of personal and ethnic self-esteem. J Couns Psychol. 2004, 51 (3): 329-339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Fischer SM, Sauaia A, Kutner JS: Patient navigation: a culturally competent strategy to address disparities in palliative care. J Palliat Med. 2007, 10 (5): 1023-1028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Meier LL, Semmer NK, Hupfeld J: The impact of unfair treatment on depressive mood: the moderating role of self-esteem level and self-esteem instability. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2009, 35 (5): 643-655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Shorey HS, Cowan G, Sullivan MP: Predicting perceptions of discrimination among Hispanics and Anglos. Hisp J Behav Sci. 2002, 24 (1): 3-22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Williams DR, Neighbors HW, Jackson JS: Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: findings from community studies. Am J Public Health. 2003, 93 (2): 200-208.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Williams DR, Mohammed SA: Discrimination and racial disparities in health: evidence and needed research. J Behav Med. 2009, 32 (1): 20-47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Williams DR, Collins C: Racial residential segregation: a fundamental cause of racial disparities in health. Public Health Rep. 2001, 116 (5): 404-416.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Morenoff JD, House JS, Hansen BB, Williams DR, Kaplan GA, Hunte HE: Understanding social disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control: the role of neighborhood context. Soc Sci Med. 2007, 65 (9): 1853-1866.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Radloff LS: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977, 1 (3): 385-401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Roberts R, Rhodes H, Vernon S: Using the CES-D to screen for depression and anxiety: the effects of language and ethnicity. Psychiatry Res. 1990, 31 (1): 69-83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB: Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol. 1997, 2 (3): 335-351.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Cook W, Medley D: Proposed hostility and pharasaic-virtue scales for the MMPI. J Appl Psychol. 1954, 38 (6): 414-418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Russell S, Crane RS, Jacobs GA, Worden TJ: The experience and expression of anger: construction and validation of an anger expression scale. Anger and hostility in cardiovascular and behavioral disorders edn. Edited by: RH CMR. 1985, New York: Meisphere, 5-30.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW: Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): areevaluation of the life orientation test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994, 67 (6): 1063-1078.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Rosenberg M: Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Revised edition. 1989, Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press

    Google Scholar 

  62. Sobel ME: Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology 1982. edn. Edited by: Leinhardt S. 1982, Washington, DC: American Sociological Association

    Google Scholar 

  63. Buhi ER, Goodson P, Neilands TB: Out of sight, not out of mind: strategies for handling missing data. Am J Health Behav. 2008, 32 (1): 83-92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Zvolensky MJ, Sachs-Ericsson N, Feldner MT, Schmidt NB, Bowman CJ: Neuroticism moderates the effect of maximum smoking level on lifetime panic disorder: a test using an epidemiologically defined national sample of smokers. Psychiatry Res. 2006, 141 (3): 321-332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Swim JK, Hyers LL, Cohen LL, Fitzgerald DC, Bylsma WH: African American college Students’s experiences with everyday racism: characteristics of and responses to these incidents. J Black Psychol. 2003, 29 (1): 38-67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Hunte HER: Association between perceived interpersonal everyday discrimination and waist circumference over a 9-year period in the midlife development in the United States cohort study. Am J Epidemiol. 2011, 173 (11): 1232-1239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Lewis TT, Troxel WM, Kravitz HM, Bromberger JT, Matthews KA, Hall MH: Chronic exposure to everyday discrimination and sleep in a multiethnic sample of middle-aged women. Health Psychol. 2013, 32 (7): 810-819.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Armstead CA, Lawler KA, Gorden G, Cross J: Relationship of racial stressors to blood pressure responses and anger expression in black college students. Health Psychol. 1989, 8 (5): 541-556.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Hartshorn KJ, Whitbeck LB, Hoyt DR: Exploring the relationships of perceived discrimination, anger, and aggression among North American indigenous adolescents. Society and Mental Health. 2012, 2 (1): 1-20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Mellor D, Merino ME, Saiz JL, Quilaqueo D: Emotional reactions, coping and long-term consequences of perceived discrimination among the Mapuche people of Chile. J Community Appl Soc. 2009, 19 (6): 473-491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Brondolo E, Hausmann LRM, Jhalani J, Pencille M, Atencio-Bacayon J, Kumar A, Kwok J, Ullah J, Roth A, Chen D, et al: Dimensions of perceived racism and self-reported health: examination of racial/ethnic differences and potential mediators. Ann Behav Med. 2012, 42 (1): 14-28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Bennett GG, Merritt MM, Edwards CL, Sollers JJ: Perceived racism and affective responses to ambiguous interpersonal interactions among African American men. Am Behav Sci. 2004, 47 (7): 963-976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Karlsen S, Nazroo JY: Relation between racial discrimination, social class, and health among ethnic minority groups. Am J Public Health. 2002, 92 (4): 624-631.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Karlsen S, Nazroo JY, McKenzie K, Bhui K, Weich S: Racism, psychosis and common mental disorder among ethnic minority groups in England. Psychol Med. 2005, 35 (12): 1795-1803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Klonoff EA, Landrine H, Ullman JB: Racial discrimination and psychiatric symptoms among Blacks. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol. 1999, 5 (4): 329-339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Landrine H, Klonoff EA: The schedule of racist events: a measure of racial discrimination and a study of its negative physical and mental health consequences. J Black Psychol. 1996, 22 (2): 144-168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Mattis JS, Fontenot DL, Hatcher-Kay CA: Religiosity, racism, and dispositional optimism among African Americans. Personal Individ Differ. 2003, 34 (6): 1025-1038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Utsey SO, Giesbrecht N, Hook J, Stanard PM: Cultural, sociofamilial, and psychological resources that inhibit psychological distress in African Americans exposed to stressful life events and race-related stress. J Couns Psychol. 2008, 55 (1): 49-62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by an appointment (King) to the Research Participation Program for the US Environmental Protection Agency, Office of Research and Development, administered by the Oak Ridge Institute for Science and Education through an interagency agreement between the US Department of Energy and the EPA. The CCAHS study work was supported by Grants P50HD38986 and R01HD050467 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health, with additional support from the MacArthur Foundation (via Harvard University and the University of California, San Francisco) and the US Department of Justice (via Harvard University). The authors thank the reviewers during the peer review process. Their comments were very thoughtful and insightful and have helped to further improve the manuscript. The contents of the manuscript are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the aforementioned institutions or individuals.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Haslyn ER Hunte.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

HERH conducted the study design study analysis. HERH, KK, MH and HL drafted portions of the manuscript. All authors revised and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hunte, H.E., King, K., Hicken, M. et al. Interpersonal discrimination and depressive symptomatology: examination of several personality-related characteristics as potential confounders in a racial/ethnic heterogeneous adult sample. BMC Public Health 13, 1084 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1084

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1084

Keywords